Skip to main content
Log in

The Role of Social Ties in Explaining Heterogeneity in the Association Between Economic Growth and Subjective Well-Being

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Happiness Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Whether economic growth improves subjective well-being has been under debate. Studies that find such an association also document heterogeneity between countries in the magnitude of the relationship. We test a theoretical model in which economic growth enhances subjective well-being only when a large share of the population derives their subjective well-being from relational goods with positive externalities instead of positional goods with negative externalities. The choice between relational and positional goods is determined by individuals’ relational abilities and expectations which we operationalize as attachment security. We specifically test whether economic growth improves subjective well-being more in those countries where the average attachment security is higher. We find support for the hypothesis in the Eurobarometer data but less support in the World Values Survey data.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We use “subjective well-being” as a concept which includes both “happiness” and “life satisfaction”. Happiness and life satisfaction are somewhat distinct concepts and empirical counterparts of SWB and can have different correlates (Graham et al. 2010). We treat them separately in our empirical analysis although they are mostly not distinguished in the theoretical literature on the relationship between economic growth and SWB.

  2. Another possible explanation is adaptation to the consumption level over time. There is empirical evidence suggesting that relative income concerns have a larger effect (Layard et al. 2010). Increases in inequality associated with economic growth can also result in stagnant SWB if inequality decreases SWB. Country differences in the effects of adaptation or in inequality could therefore also explain the heterogeneity in the GDP–SWB association.

  3. The model implicitly assumes that market goods can affect well-being only through relational goods or through consumption which has negative externalities via positional comparisons. There is no effect of the higher quantity or quality of market goods such as tastier meals, for example, outside these channels. If deviations from this assumption are important, and product quality and quantity have significant positive effects on well-being, we should not observe a lack of association between GDP and SWB. If the deviation is significant only below a certain level of consumption, this prediction should accordingly be valid only among countries below that level (Akay et al. 2012).

  4. A problem in using psychological survey measures in cross-country analyses is that people tend to evaluate themselves in comparison to the averages of the country they live in (Heine et al. 2008). This makes it more difficult to find statistically significant relationships across countries.

  5. See Hovi and Laamanen (2015) for a more elaborate discussion on different types of models of GDP and SWB and their interpretations.

  6. The inclusion of the attachment main effect in the regression equations leads to very high multicollinearity. Mean centering of the variables has been proposed as a way of alleviating multicollinearity (Aiken and West 1991). We tried this as a robustness check, and the results remained qualitatively intact.

  7. We checked the robustness of the estimates by excluding Belgium from the sample. In the Eurobarometer data, the prediction of life satisfaction by the interaction of attachment and log GDP was no longer statistically significant, whereas the prediction of happiness by the interaction became, in general, more statistically significant. The estimates of the interaction term were not affected by the exclusion of Belgium in the WVS data.

  8. Excluding Belgium from the sample led the estimates of log GDP to be statistically significant in the lowest quartile when predicting life satisfaction in the Eurobarometer data in levels with and without the country fixed effects. In the WVS data, the exclusion of Belgium decreased the statistical significance of differences in log GDP predicting life satisfaction in the lowest quartile. The estimate of log GDP predicting happiness in long first differences became almost statistically significant (p < .10).

  9. The results are available from the authors.

  10. There are earlier empirical studies that have examined various measures of SWB in panel data settings (e.g. Böckerman and Ilmakunnas 2009).

References

  • Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akay, A., Martinsson, P., & Medhin, H. (2012). Does positional concern matter in poor societies? Evidence from a survey experiment in rural Ethiopia. World Development, 40, 428–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alpizar, F., Carlsson, F., & Johansson-Stenman, O. (2005). How much do we care about absolute versus relative income and consumption? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 56, 405–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alvarez-Cuadrado, F., & Long, N. V. (2011). The relative income hypothesis. Journal of Economic Dynamics and Control, 35, 1489–1501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 195–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles in young adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 226–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolini, S., Bilancini, E., & Sarracino, F. (2009). Sociability predicts happiness: World-wide evidence from time series. Department of Economics University of Siena No. 579. Department of Economics, University of Siena. Retrieved from http://ideas.repec.org/p/usi/wpaper/579.html.

  • Böckerman, P., & Ilmakunnas, P. (2009). Unemployment and self-assessed health: Evidence from panel data. Health Economics, 18, 161–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borrero, S., Escobar, A. B., Cortés, A. M., & Maya, L. C. (2013). Poor and distressed, but happy: Situational and cultural moderators of the relationship between wealth and happiness. Estudios Gerenciales, 29, 2–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, C. J., Brown, G. D. A., & Moore, S. C. (2010). Money and happiness: Rank of income, not income, affects life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 21, 471–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruni, L., & Stanca, L. (2008). Watching alone: Relational goods, television and happiness. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 65, 506–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caporale, G. M., Georgellis, Y., Tsitsianis, N., & Yin, Y. P. (2009). Income and happiness across Europe: Do reference values matter? Journal of Economic Psychology, 30, 42–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. E., Frijters, P., & Shields, M. A. (2008). Relative income, happiness, and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature, 46, 95–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corazzini, L., Esposito, L., & Majorano, F. (2012). Reign in hell or serve in heaven? A cross-country journey into the relative vs absolute perceptions of well-being. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81, 715–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deaton, A. (2008). Income, health and well-being around the world: Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 22, 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deaton, A., & Stone, A. A. (2013). Two happiness puzzles. American Economic Review, 103, 591–597.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • del Salinas-Jiménez, M., Artés, J., & Salinas-Jiménez, J. (2010). Income, motivation, and satisfaction with life: An empirical analysis. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11, 779–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Lucas, R. E. (2003). Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: Emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 403–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiTommaso, E., Brannen-McNulty, C., Ross, L., & Burgess, M. (2003). Attachment styles, social skills and loneliness in young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 303–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1974). Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence. In P. A. David & W. R. Melvin (Eds.), Nations and households in economic growth (pp. 89–125). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1995). Will raising the incomes of all increase the happiness of all? Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 27, 35–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A., McVey, L. A., Switek, M., Sawangfa, O., & Zweig, J. S. (2010). The happiness–income paradox revisited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 22463–22468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrer-i-Carbonell, A. (2005). Income and well-being: An empirical analysis of the comparison income effect. Journal of Public Economics, 89, 997–1019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4, 132–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgellis, Y., Tsitsianis, N., & Yin, Y. P. (2009). Personal values as mitigating factors in the link between income and life satisfaction: Evidence from the European Social Survey. Social Indicators Research, 91, 329–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, C., Chattopadhyay, S., & Picon, M. (2010). The Easterlin and other paradoxes: Why both sides of the debate may be correct. In E. Diener, D. Kahneman, & J. Helliwell (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 247–290). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gui, B. (1996). On “relational goods”: Strategic implications of investment in relationships. International Journal of Social Economics, 23, 260–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational framework for research on close relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Buchtel, E. E., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). What do cross-national comparisons of personality traits tell us? The case of conscientiousness. Psychological Science, 19, 309–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hilburn-Cobb, C. (2004). Adolescent psychopathology in terms of multiple behavioral systems: The role of attachment and controlling strategies and frankly disorganized behavior. In L. Atkinson & S. Goldberg (Eds.), Attachment issues in psychopathology and intervention (pp. 99–135). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hovi, M., & Laamanen, J.-P. (2015). It’s complicated. The relationship between GDP and subjective well-being. Tampere Economic Working Papers 97. University of Tampere.

  • Irons, C., & Gilbert, P. (2005). Evolved mechanisms in adolescent anxiety and depression symptoms: The role of the attachment and social rank systems. Journal of Adolescence, 28, 325–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karreman, A., & Vingerhoets, A. J. (2012). Attachment and well-being: The mediating role of emotion regulation and resilience. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 821–826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, A. (2008). Comments on economic growth and subjective well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. Brookings Papers on Economic Activity, 39, 95–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R., Mayraz, G., & Nickell, S. (2010). Does relative income matter? Are the critics right? In E. Diener, D. Kahneman, & J. Helliwell (Eds.), International differences in well-being (pp. 139–165). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Noftle, E. E., & Shaver, P. R. (2006). Attachment dimensions and the big five personality traits: Associations and comparative ability to predict relationship quality. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 179–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proto, E., & Rustichini, A. (2013). A reassessment of the relationship between GDP and life satisfaction. PLoS One, 8(11), e79358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pugno, M. (2009). The Easterlin paradox and the decline of social capital: An integrated explanation. Journal of Socio-Economics, 38, 590–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rojas, M. (2007). Heterogeneity in the relationship between income and happiness: A conceptual-referent-theory explanation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, D., & Wolfers, J. (2010). Debunking the Easterlin paradox, again. The Brookings Institution. Retrieved April 14, 2014, from http://www.brookings.edu/research/opinions/2010/12/13-debunking-easterlin-wolfers.

  • Schmitt, D. P., Alcalay, L., Allensworth, M., Allik, J., Ault, L., Austers, I., et al. (2004). Patterns and universals of adult romantic attachment across 62 cultural regions are models of self and of other pancultural constructs? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 367–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2008). Economic growth and subjective well-being: Reassessing the Easterlin Paradox. Brookings Papers on Economic Activity, 39, 1–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2013). Subjective well-being and income: Is there any evidence of satiation? The American Economic Review, 103, 598–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Towler, A. J., & Stuhlmacher, A. F. (2013). Attachment styles, relationship satisfaction, and well-being in working women. Journal of Social Psychology, 153, 279–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlaner, C. J. (1989). “Relational goods” and participation: Incorporating sociability into a theory of rational action. Public Choice, 62, 253–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Lange, P. A., De Bruin, E. M. N., Otten, W., & Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: Theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 733–746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Academy of Finland (Project No. 252369). We are grateful for helpful discussions and comments from Markus Haavio, Juha Itkonen, Jukka Pirttilä, Maurizio Pugno and participants at the Annual Congress of the International Institute of Public Finance (Lugano) as well as from two anonymous reviewers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Petri Böckerman.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Böckerman, P., Laamanen, JP. & Palosaari, E. The Role of Social Ties in Explaining Heterogeneity in the Association Between Economic Growth and Subjective Well-Being. J Happiness Stud 17, 2457–2479 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9702-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-015-9702-0

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation