Publication Cover
Stress
The International Journal on the Biology of Stress
Volume 24, 2021 - Issue 6
1,320
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Research Reports

Gender roles are related to cortisol habituation to repeated social evaluative stressors in adults: secondary analyses from a randomized controlled trial

ORCID Icon, , , ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon & show all
Pages 723-733 | Received 31 Aug 2020, Accepted 13 Feb 2021, Published online: 02 Apr 2021

Abstract

Masculine and feminine gender roles influence stressor appraisals and coping in everyday life, but their effect on stress response systems like the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis is unclear. Accordingly, the present study tested the association between gender roles and cortisol responses to repeated stress as part of secondary analyses of data from a randomized controlled trial examining the effects of stress management interventions on cortisol habituation. Participants (Nfinal = 86; 72% female) completed a baseline survey assessing gender role endorsement using the Bem Sex Role Inventory, from which 4 groups were derived: masculine (n = 20), feminine (n = 20), androgynous (high masculinity, high femininity; n = 22), and undifferentiated (low masculinity, low femininity; n = 24). Following the stress management intervention (mindfulness-based stress reduction or cognitive-behavioral skills training) or waitlist period control, participants completed the Trier Social Stress Test on two laboratory visits (48 h apart). Salivary cortisol was assessed 0, 25, 35, and 60 min post-stressor during both laboratory visits. Androgynous and undifferentiated individuals both exhibited a significant decrease in total cortisol from visit 1 to visit 2 (i.e. habituation) whereas feminine and masculine individuals did not. Undifferentiated individuals exhibited greater habituation than feminine and masculine individuals, whereas androgynous individuals only exhibited greater habituation than the feminine group. Controlling for study condition assignment did not alter these results. Results imply that gender roles may be implicated in stress-related disease because of their association with HPA axis functioning during episodes of acute stress.

Introduction

Endorsing masculine and/or feminine gender roles may profoundly impact health and well-being (Mayor, Citation2015). Sociocultural gender refers to the behavioral, social, and psychological characteristics (e.g. roles, identities, socialized behavioral patterns) associated with men, women, and gender diverse people (Pryzgoda & Chrisler, Citation2001). The process by which culture defines masculinity and femininity is gradually assimilated with one’s identity from a young age (Bem, Citation1981; Solbes-Canales et al., Citation2020) as boys and girls are typically encouraged to enact masculine and feminine behaviors throughout their lives (Ruble et al., Citation2007). Gender roles are therefore expected to influence a wide range of health-relevant behaviors over the lifespan. Consistent with this view, gender roles are associated with physical and mental health outcomes (Annandale & Hunt, Citation1990; Lengua & Stormshak, Citation2000; Mayor, Citation2015), exposure to stressors (Mayor, Citation2015; Nica & Potcovaru, Citation2015), as well as stress appraisals (Kaplan & Marks, Citation1995; Sarrasin et al., Citation2014) and coping strategies (Gianakos, Citation2002; Lengua & Stormshak, Citation2000; Long et al., Citation1992).

If gender roles impact stress appraisals and coping, they may influence health in part by modulating activation of major stress response systems like the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) axis (Dedovic et al., Citation2009). This is important because cortisol influences metabolism and immunity, and thus dysregulated HPA axis functioning can be detrimental to health (Chrousos, Citation2009; Cohen, Citation2002; Glaser & Kiecolt-Glaser, Citation2005). In the context of acute stressors, cortisol responses that are too large (i.e. heightened reactivity), too small (i.e. blunted reactivity), fail to temper once the stressor has ended (i.e. prolong recovery), or fail to diminish with repeated stressor exposure (i.e. non-habituation) are considered maladaptive (McEwen, Citation1998). Yet, a review of stress reactivity (Dedovic et al., Citation2009) highlights the lack of research linking sociocultural gender to HPA axis functioning.

When reviewing the literature linking sex, gender and the HPA axis, it is important to consider measurement. Dichotomous measures of biological sex (i.e. male v. female) are imperfect because sex and gender are entangled, and thus the literature linking sex with HPA axis functioning in humans likely include gender effects (Springer et al., Citation2012). Nevertheless, this literature suggests that men and women exhibit similar HPA axis stress reactivity before puberty (Romeo, Citation2005; Romeo et al., Citation2008) but not thereafter (Uhart et al., Citation2006). HPA axis reactivity is also influenced by menopause (Seeman et al., Citation2001), menstrual phase and oral contraceptive use (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1999), pregnancy (de Weerth & Buitelaar, Citation2005), and sex hormone levels (Juster et al., Citation2016). Similarly, the animal literature (where confounding gender effects should be minimal) reports the existence of reliable sex difference in HPA axis reactivity, but mixed associations with HPA axis habituation (Babb et al., Citation2014; Bhatnagar et al., Citation2005; Heck & Handa, Citation2019).

By contrast, Gender Schema Theory does not conceptualize gender roles as a dichotomy (Bem, Citation1981). This framework posits that masculinity is characterized by an instrumental orientation and cognitive focus on action whereas femininity is characterized by expressive orientation and affective concern for the welfare of others (Juster et al., Citation2011). Moreover, masculinity and femininity are considered independent constructs, such that individuals may be categorized as endorsing both (androgynous), either (masculine-typed or feminine-typed), or neither (undifferentiated). To date, two studies (Juster et al., Citation2016; Juster & Lupien, Citation2012) link these gender role categories to allostatic load—an index of neuroendocrine, cardiovascular, immune and metabolic “wear and tear”—and one study finds no association between gender role categories and cortisol reactivity (Juster et al., Citation2016).

Yet more work is needed to evaluate how gender roles are linked with HPA axis functioning, and such work may benefit from examining HPA axis habituation. This is because examining habituation can reveal effects that are not observable during initial stressor exposure. In addition, an effect of gender roles on cortisol habituation would be generally consistent with the extant literature which links trait reappraisal with cortisol habituation (Roos et al., Citation2019) and gender roles with stressor appraisals and coping behaviors (Cheng, Citation2005; Washburn-Ormachea et al., Citation2004).

In summary, the present study examined the association between gender role categories (as conceptualized by Gender Schema Theory (Bem, Citation1981)) and HPA axis responses to two repeated stressors, as part of secondary analyses of data from a previously published study (Manigault et al., Citation2019). Participants self-reported gender role endorsement prior to being randomly assigned to one of two stress-reduction interventions (mindfulness-based stress reduction or cognitive-behavioral therapy training) or a passive control condition (waitlist), and then completed two Trier Social Stress Tests (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1993) (TSST) post-intervention. Gender schema theory proposes that androgynous individuals should be most adaptable across situational contexts because they exhibit both greater behavioral flexibility than sex-typed individuals and more psychosocial resources than undifferentiated individuals (Bem, Citation1981; Juster et al., Citation2016; Orlofsky & Windle, Citation1978). Accordingly, we hypothesized that androgynous individuals would display higher habituation (i.e. a larger decrease in response magnitude) than masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated individuals. Finally, we also tested biological sex effects (as measured dichotomously), and examined if gender role endorsement effects were independent of intervention effects and biological sex.

Material and method

Participants

One-hundred and thirty-eight individuals were recruited from an American Midwestern university to participate in a randomized control trial to test the effects of Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) on HPA axis habituation (Manigault et al., Citation2019). The majority of the final sample was female (72.1%) and had completed at least some college education (91.9%). Exclusionary criteria include the following: those who were younger than 18 or older than 50 years of age, were pregnant, endorsed a major endocrine or psychiatric disorder, were on steroid medication, had experience with an MBSR or CBT intervention, were not usually awake by 10:00 AM (excluding weekends), did not have evening availability to participate in interventions, or were associated with the psychology department. Eligible individuals also needed to report experiencing at least moderate levels of stress over the last month (i.e. scored a 4 or above on a 1–10 scale where 1 = little to no stress, and 10 = a great deal of stress). The stress screener item and 4-point cutoff were derived from the annual Stress in AmericaTM surveys (American Psychological Association, Citation2018) and are fully described in a previous publication of this data (Manigault et al., Citation2019). These exclusion criteria served to minimize inter-individual differences in diurnal cortisol rhythms as well as maximize stress reduction intervention effects.

Procedure

Recruitment, randomization, and baseline survey

Individuals deemed eligible from an online survey were invited to a group session to receive background information on intervention and assessment procedures, provide consent, complete a baseline survey, and receive their condition assignment. After providing written consent, participants were given a set of initial questionnaires and informed of their random assignment by the study coordinator. A blocked randomization scheme was used to randomly allocate participants to interventions during each of 4 cohorts. As a random allocation procedure, the study coordinator shuffled appointment cards on which study condition assignment was pre-written; the study condition on each card was unknown to researchers and participants until the assignment was given. The 4 cohorts were formed between August 2016 to January 2018, where the first cohort had a 1:1:1 allocation ratio, and cohorts 2–4 had adjusted allocation ratios so that about the same number of participants attended at least one intervention session for the MBSR and CBT conditions.

Follow up survey and laboratory visits

After all interventions had taken place for the cohort, participants were emailed a survey to complete before the first of two laboratory visits. Laboratory visits occurred 48 h apart between the hours of 2:00–8:30 PM to control for natural fluctuation in diurnal cortisol. Participants were fitted with 7 electrodes on the neck and torso to measure electrocardiography and impedance cardiography upon arrival to the lab (data not presented in this manuscript). Participants completed health behavior questionnaires and rested (with emotionally neutral reading) for 15 min. The experimenter collected the first saliva sample at the end of this 15 min period. Afterward, participants underwent the TSST (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1993). After the TSST, participants completed questionnaires, provided a second saliva sample, and rested for another 10 min (without emotionally neutral reading). The rest period was followed by more questionnaires, the third saliva sample, and a 20 min rest period during which emotionally neutral readings were provided. The last resting period was followed by more questionnaires and the fourth saliva sample. Both laboratory visits followed the same format up until the end of the visit. Participants were asked to confirm their next appointment at the end of the first laboratory visit, whereas they were debriefed and paid (up to $70) at the end of the second laboratory visit. The study was approved by the Ohio University Institutional Review Board and was registered as a clinical trial on ClinicalTrials.gov (identifier #: NCT02894229).

Materials

Trier social stress test

Following the 6-week intervention period, participants underwent modified versions of the Trier Social Stress Test (Kirschbaum et al., Citation1993) (TSST) on both laboratory visits (where speech preparation lasted five minutes instead of 10 min). The TSST consisted of a five minute period to anticipate and prepare for a mock job interview speech. This was followed by a five minute period to give the speech and five minutes to perform a difficult arithmetic task in front of an evaluative panel. The evaluative panel was composed of two research assistants wearing white lab coats and trained to maintain stoic expressions. The evaluative panel had no interaction with the participants prior to the TSST. The two TSSTs differed with respect to the starting value of the arithmetic tasks to minimize practice effects but speech instructions remained unchanged, consistent with prior work examining habituation (Thoma et al., Citation2017). The TSST produces a robust cortisol response because this laboratory stressors is characterized by uncontrollability and social evaluative threat (Dickerson & Kemeny, Citation2004). For the first visit, 57% of evaluative panels were “mixed” and 43% were “all female”. For the second visit, 66% of evaluative panels were “mixed”, 30% were “all female”, and 4% were “all male”. Gender role categories were independent of evaluative panel gender composition (all ps > .39).

Mindfulness based stress reduction

Those assigned to the MBSR condition underwent a 6-week version of MBSR training (Klatt et al., Citation2008; Lengacher et al., Citation2009) adapted from standardized treatment manuals (Kabat-Zinn, Citation1990; Kabat-Zinn & Santorelli, Citation1999). Participants were asked to attend group sessions for two hours weekly for a 6-week period in addition to participation in 45–60 min of formal and informal meditation practice at home each day. Those who participated received information on didactic mindfulness, mindfulness meditation practices, and were asked to engage in group discussion.

Cognitive behavioral therapy

A 6-week version of group CBT was developed and implemented from CBT treatments shown to reduce stress (Cully & Teten, Citation2008). Weekly two hour meetings lasted for 6 weeks, along with instructions to practice CBT techniques for 45–60 min at home daily. Topics covered included didactic information on stress and CBT techniques to reduce stress, supplemented by group discussion.

Waitlist

Those assigned to the Waitlist control could participate in the CBT stress management intervention at the end of the study but did not receive any intervention during the study.

Measures

Salivary cortisol

Salivary cortisol was collected four times per lab visit, including samples taken immediately before the TSST and 25, 35, and 60 min after the TSST. Participants were given Salivettes (Sarstedt, Inc., Newton, N.C.) and instructed to saturate the swab with saliva for up to 3 min. Instructions were given to ensure that swabs were not touched by hand. Samples were immediately stored at −20 °C after collection and then transferred to a −80 °C freezer until processed. All samples were centrifuged and assayed in duplicate (values averaged) at Ohio University facilities with standard enzyme-linked immunoassay procedures (Salimetrics, LLC, State College, PA). The assay sensitivity was less than 0.007 ug/dL, inter-assay coefficients of variation were less than 11.0%, and intra-assay coefficients of variation were less than 7.0%.

Gender roles

The 60-item Bem Sex Role Inventory (Bem, Citation1974) was used to assess participants’ perceptions of their gender roles as part of the baseline survey. This instrument uses a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never or almost never true) to 7 (always or almost always true) for stereotypically masculine (n = 20; e.g. ambitious, dominant) and feminine (n = 20; e.g. affectionate, gentle) descriptors, plus neutral filler items (n = 20; e.g. sincere, conscientious). Both the masculinity (α = .77) and femininity (α = .78) subscales showed high internal consistency. Masculinity and femininity ratings did not differ between MBSR, CBT and the waitlist (all ps > .57).

Masculinity and femininity subscales were median split such that all individuals were categorized as either high (above the median) or low (less than or equal to the median) masculinity and femininity. Consistent with prior work (Bem, Citation1981), median split variables were used to categorize participants as either androgynous (high femininity/high masculinity), feminine (high femininity/low masculinity), masculine (low femininity/high masculinity), or undifferentiated (low femininity/low masculinity). As shown on , assignment to study condition was independent from gender role category.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and between-gender role category contrasts of demographic measures and study condition assignment.

This median-based coding scheme was preferred over treating masculinity and femininity as continuous scales (and modeling their interaction) for important reasons. More specifically, the present study aimed to detect any difference between the four gender role categories hypothesized to exist under Bem’s model (Bem, Citation1981). To this end, it was necessary to derive four gender categories using median splits (and model gender category as a single factor with 4 levels). In contrast, modeling the interaction of masculinity and femininity as continuous variables would shift the scope of this project toward a different research question (i.e. we would now test if the association between masculinity and cortisol habituation changes as a function of femininity, and vice versa).

Additional measures

Participants self-reported biological sex at birth and waketime, whereas experiment start time was recorded by research assistants.

Analytic plan

Treatment of missing and outlying data

Final analyses included 86 participants who completed the study up to the first laboratory visit. Among these individuals, 83 completed the second visit for a total of 676 possible cortisol samples. However, 1 sample contained too little saliva to be assayed and 4 were below assay threshold. Among the remaining 671 valid cortisol samples, 4 were outliers (more than 3 SD from mean). Excluding these outlying values had no influence on the present analyses. Accordingly, outlying cortisol values were retained in final analyses. Individuals who withdrew prior to the first visits did not differ from participants included in the final analyses on the basis of gender role category, age, sex, education, race, stress or ethnicity (all ps > .05).

Modeling total cortisol output

Maximum likelihood multilevel models were used to examine the change in total cortisol as a function of visit (0 = visit 1; 1 = visit 2), and dummy-coded gender role category (contrasting all four gender role categories: androgynous, masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated). These models were run using PROC MIXED on SAS 9.4/STAT 13.1 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, N.C.). Three-level models were used as sampling occasions (level 1) were nested within laboratory visits (level 2) and visits were nested within individuals (level 3). Consistent with the following equation, visit-level and individual-level intercepts were included as random effects: ln ( Cortisol ) ijk =   π 0 jk +  e ijk π 0 jk =   β 00 k +   β 0 1k ( Visit jk )   +  r 0 jk β 00 k =   γ 000 +   γ 00 1 ( dummy1 k ) +   γ 00 2 ( dummy2 k )   +   γ 00 3 ( dummy3 k ) +  u 00 k β 0 1k =   γ 0 1 0 +   γ 0 11 ( dummy1 k ) +   γ 0 12 ( dummy2 k )   +   γ 0 13 ( dummy3 k )

Habituation was indexed as a change in total cortisol output from visit 1 to visit 2 (i.e. where a greater decrease in total output implies greater habituation). This modeling approach yields insight comparable to testing a change in Area under the Curve relative to Ground or AUCg (Pruessner et al., Citation2003) but has the advantage of making use of partially missing cortisol data. It is worth noting that HPA axis habituation has been successfully examined and linked to impactful health outcomes using measures of total cortisol output (Kudielka et al., Citation2006; Wüst et al., Citation2005), however, other work also indexes habituation using measures of cortisol reactivity (i.e. peak - baseline) (Breines et al., Citation2014; Roos et al., Citation2019; Thoma et al., Citation2017). Accordingly, analyses of cortisol AUCg and reactivity scores are presented in online supplemental materials to facilitate comparison with other work. Associations between gender role categories and total cortisol output were also examined separately for each visit to provide a frame of reference for habituation effects (i.e. was habituation in a given group driven by low cortisol output during the second visit or high cortisol output during the first visit?). P-values below .05 were considered significant; p-values between .05 and .10 were considered marginally significant; p-values greater than .10 were considered non-significant.

Treatment of covariates

Consistent with prior analyses of the present data (Manigault et al., Citation2019), sex, wake time, and experiment start time were examined as covariates (i.e. tests of primary hypotheses were repeated while including/excluding covariate measures).

Results

Preliminary results

As shown on , the final analytic sample (n = 86) was primarily comprised of young, non-Hispanic White women with some college education. Comparisons of gender categories revealed that the androgynous, feminine, masculine, and undifferentiated individuals differed with respect to age and education level (marginally significant differences), but not sex, ethnicity, or assignment to study condition. Accordingly, education level and age were also included as covariates (in addition to sex, wake time, and experiment start time).

Habituation

The interaction of gender role category x visit was significant when predicting total natural log transformed cortisol (F(3,502) = 2.65, p = .048), suggesting that the degree to which cortisol changed across visits differed between at least two gender role categories. As illustrated in and , the undifferentiated group exhibited a larger decrease in total cortisol from visit 1 to visit 2 than the feminine group (b = −.385, t(502) = 2.58, p = .010) and the masculine group (b = −.287, t(502) = 1.90, p = .057), but not the androgynous group (b = −.113, t(502) = 0.76, p = .44). In contrast, the androgynous group exhibited a marginally larger decrease in total cortisol from visit 1 to visit 2 than the feminine group (b = −.271, t(502) = 1.75, p = .081) but not the masculine group (b = −.174, t(502) = 1.11, p = .26). Finally, masculine and feminine individuals did not differ with respect to decrease in total cortisol from visit 1 to visit 2 (b = 0.097, t(502) = 0.62, p = .53).

Figure 1. Salivary cortisol as a function of study visit and gender role category. Undifferentiated individuals scored below the median on both masculinity and femininity; feminine individuals scored above the median on femininity and below the median on masculinity; masculine individuals scored above the median on masculinity and below the median on femininity; androgynous individuals scored above the median on both masculinity and femininity. Data shown are unadjusted marginal means estimates of salivary cortisol. Error bars shown correspond to standard errors of mean estimates.

Figure 1. Salivary cortisol as a function of study visit and gender role category. Undifferentiated individuals scored below the median on both masculinity and femininity; feminine individuals scored above the median on femininity and below the median on masculinity; masculine individuals scored above the median on masculinity and below the median on femininity; androgynous individuals scored above the median on both masculinity and femininity. Data shown are unadjusted marginal means estimates of salivary cortisol. Error bars shown correspond to standard errors of mean estimates.

Figure 2. Salivary cortisol as a function of time since stressor onset, laboratory visit, and gender role category. Undifferentiated individuals (panel a) scored below the median on both masculinity and femininity; feminine individuals (panel b) scored above the median on femininity and below the median on masculinity; masculine individuals (panel c) scored above the median on masculinity and below the median on femininity; androgynous individuals (panel d) scored above the median on both masculinity and femininity. Data shown are unadjusted marginal means estimates of salivary cortisol. Error bars shown correspond to standard errors of mean estimates.

Figure 2. Salivary cortisol as a function of time since stressor onset, laboratory visit, and gender role category. Undifferentiated individuals (panel a) scored below the median on both masculinity and femininity; feminine individuals (panel b) scored above the median on femininity and below the median on masculinity; masculine individuals (panel c) scored above the median on masculinity and below the median on femininity; androgynous individuals (panel d) scored above the median on both masculinity and femininity. Data shown are unadjusted marginal means estimates of salivary cortisol. Error bars shown correspond to standard errors of mean estimates.

Relevant to primary hypotheses, both the undifferentiated and androgynous groups showed a significant decrease in total cortisol from visit 1 to visit 2 (b = −.381, t(79) = 3.79, p < .001, and b = −.267, t(79) = 2.44, p = .016, respectively), whereas the feminine and masculine groups did not (b = .004, t(79) = 0.04, p = .97, and b = −0.093, t(79) = 0.83, p = .40, respectively). The interaction of gender role category x visit remained significant after controlling for sex, wake time, experiment start time, age, education level, and study conditions (F(3,489) = 4.65, p = .003). Moreover, the interaction of sex x gender role category x visit was non-significant (F(3,502) = 1.11, p = .34) suggesting that biological sex did not moderate the effect of gender role categories on cortisol habituation.

Examining sex effects independently revealed a non-significant interaction of sex x visit (F(1, 502) = 0.88, p = .35) and a significant main effect across visits (b = 0.39, t(502) = 2.43, p = .015), such that males exhibited higher total cortisol output across visits relative to females.

First visit

Total natural log transformed cortisol during the first visit was not significantly predicted by gender role category (p = .66).

Second visit

Total natural log transformed cortisol during the first visit was not significantly predicted by gender role category (p = .79).

Supplemental analyses

To examine the degree to which intervention effects may underlie differences observed among gender role categories, we opted to compare gender role categories on the basis of pre- and post-intervention levels of psychosocial resources and coping strategies, and repeated primary analyses while controlling for any pre- to post-intervention difference observed. These analyses revealed that androgynous individual exhibited greater pre-intervention levels of mindful non-reactivity, equanimity, and perceived control over thoughts than masculine, feminine, and undifferentiated individuals and lower difficulty with emotional regulation than masculine and feminine individual (as shown on Supplemental Table 1). Yet, these pre-intervention differences were no longer present post-intervention. Accordingly, the interaction of gender role category x visit was reexamined while controlling for pre- to post-intervention change scores in mindful non-reactivity, equanimity, perceived control over thoughts and difficulty with emotional regulation. The interaction of gender role category x visit predicting total cortisol output remained significant after controlling for sex, wake time, experiment start time, education, age as well as pre- to post-intervention change in mindful non-reactivity, equanimity, perceived control over thoughts and difficulty with emotional regulation (F(1,426) = 4.02, p = .045). In response to the peer-review process, we repeated primary analyses while controlling for three additional factors: body mass index, tobacco use (number of cigarettes smoked on a given visit day), and pretreatment depressive symptoms (20-item Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression [CES-D] scale (Radloff, Citation1977)). The interaction of gender categories by visit predicting total cortisol output remained significant when controlling for body mass index, tobacco use, and depressive symptoms (p = .005).

Discussion

This study examined the association between gender role categories (as conceptualized by Gender Schema Theory (Bem, Citation1981)) and HPA axis habituation to two repeated social evaluative stressors. This research is important because gender roles may impact a variety of health behaviors including stress-related appraisals and coping, but their association with HPA axis functioning remains unclear. We hypothesized that androgynous individuals would display greater habituation than sex-typed (i.e. masculine and feminine) and undifferentiated individuals. This is because androgynous individuals are thought to exhibit greater behavioral flexibility than sex-typed individuals and more psychological resources than undifferentiated individuals (Bem et al., Citation1976; Bem & Lewis, Citation1975). Consistent with this prediction, the current study found that androgynous individuals exhibited greater HPA axis habituation than feminine individuals, and a similar but non-significant trend was observed for masculine individuals. Yet, undifferentiated individuals showed an unexpectedly large cortisol habituation pattern, such that they displayed greater habituation than both feminine and masculine (but not androgynous) individuals.

Considering predictions of Gender Schema Theory (Bem, Citation1981) could yield valuable insights into why habituation occurred for the androgynous group (as expected) and the undifferentiated groups (contrary to expectations) but not the masculine or feminine groups. This theory predicts that sex-typed individuals (i.e. primarily feminine or masculine) are expected to rigidly adhere to sex-typed behaviors and experience distress when needing to adopt a behavior typically associated with the opposite sex-type. In contrast, androgynous individuals can enact both masculine and feminine roles with ease. Finally, undifferentiated individuals, although not expected to experience distress when enacting behaviors associated with the opposite sex-type, tend to exhibit fewer psychosocial resources (e.g. lower self-esteem, well-being, and higher depression) than masculine, feminine, and androgynous individuals (Juster et al., Citation2016; Orlofsky & Windle, Citation1978). Accordingly, it may be that the androgynous and undifferentiated groups habituated because they benefited from greater behavioral flexibility. Nevertheless, additional work will be necessary to test this hypothesis, and future work in this area would benefit from monitoring the extent to which individuals engage in stereotypically masculine/feminine behaviors in response to repeated stress exposure.

Alternatively, it may be that gender role endorsement serves as an indicator of a broader range of appraisal and coping processes that influence HPA axis activity. Consistent with this view, prior work finds that androgynous individuals tend to exhibit higher active, acceptance and emotion-focused coping than other gender role categories (Washburn-Ormachea et al., Citation2004). Similarly, some work implies that androgynous individuals are more responsive to situational demands than either masculine or feminine individuals (Cheng, Citation2005). Further, androgynous individuals are generally in good mental health as evidenced by low depression, high self-esteem, and high well-being (Juster et al., Citation2016).

Secondly, the finding that gender role endorsement effects could uniquely predict HPA axis habituation is important because of its implications for long-term health and associated mechanisms. More specifically, established theoretical models (McEwen, Citation1998) and a growing literature (Bem et al., Citation1976; Bem & Lewis, Citation1975; Kudielka et al., Citation2006; Thoma et al., Citation2017) link HPA axis non-habituation with poor health. Accordingly, specific patterns of gender role endorsement could indicate greater disease susceptibility because of their association with HPA axis habituation. Moreover, the process of adapting to repeated stressors involves a set of behaviors that are distinct from those invoked in the context of a single stressor, and examining such differences could yield insight into the mechanism via which gender roles become associated with HPA axis habituation. Consequently, future work may benefit from focusing on characteristics of stress responses (e.g. coping behaviors) that are unique to repeated stressor paradigms to further characterize this relationship.

It should be noted that although the current study found that gender role endorsement was related to cortisol responses to repeated stressors, there was no association between gender role categories and cortisol responses during the first TSST. This finding is consistent with one prior study (Juster et al., Citation2016) where gender role grouping was unrelated to total cortisol output (i.e. cortisol AUCg) in response to the TSST. By contrast, supplemental analyses of cortisol reactivity scores revealed between group differences during the second visit, such that the androgynous group exhibited lower visit 2 reactivity scores than the masculine group and the undifferentiated group exhibited lower visit 2 reactivity scores than both the masculine and feminine groups, consistent with . Altogether, results supported the claim that examining repeated stressor exposure revealed new gender role effects.

Finally, a discussion of intervention effects is warranted. More specifically, the present study was designed to test the effects of stress-reduction interventions rather than gender role endorsement. Accordingly, we examined the possibility that intervention effects may have obscured gender role effects. Pre-intervention differences were observed where androgynous individuals exhibited higher levels of mindful non-reactivity, equanimity and perceived control over thoughts than masculine, feminine and undifferentiated individuals and lower difficulty with emotional regulation than masculine and feminine individuals. Overall, these results are consistent with past literature and suggests that androgynous individuals may be more apt at regulating their emotions (Cheng, Citation2005; Washburn-Ormachea et al., Citation2004). Yet, the aforementioned group differences were no longer present post-intervention, and could imply that intervention effects obscured preexisting contrasts among gender role groups. To address this possibility, primary analyses were repeated while controlling for study condition assignment, as well as repeated when controlling for any pre- and post-intervention differences (i.e. in mindful non-reactivity, equanimity and perceived control over thoughts, and difficulty with emotional regulation). In all these analytical scenarios, the association between gender roles and cortisol habituation remained significant. Accordingly, most of the evidence reviewed does not support the claim that the association between gender role endorsement and cortisol habituation was attributable to stress-reduction intervention effects. Yet, conducting a replication study where stress-reduction training is not manipulated will be necessary to fully address this issue.

Strengths and limitations

Some strengths and limitations of the present study should be noted. The present study used a large sample size relative to other studies examining stress habituation, with 4 cortisol samples per visit. Yet, most of the final sample reported female sex (72%) which may limit generalizability to male populations and the present study did not control for menstrual cycle or contraceptive use. Accordingly, replication will be necessary to ensure that these factors did not influence results. Moreover, the present study was not designed to test the association between gender roles and HPA axis habituation. Accordingly, intervention effects and gender role effects could be confounded (though statistical tests of this explanation did not support this conclusion). Related, sample size was determined for the purpose of comparing three conditions rather than four gender role categories and thus likely limited analytical sensitivity. More specifically, limited statistical power may explain why separate examination of the second visit revealed no differences between gender role categories. In contrast, null results for the first response to the TSST are consistent with prior work (Juster et al., Citation2016) making use of a larger sample (n = 204). Nevertheless, future work in this area would benefit from using larger sample sizes to more precisely examine how cortisol habituation patterns differ between gender role categories. In addition, future work would benefit from controlling for menstrual cycle and contraceptive use. Psychometric issues of the Bem Sex Role Inventory should be noted (Hoffman & Borders, Citation2001). Deriving four gender role categories using median splits was necessary for direct comparison with the existing literature. Yet, the coding of this scale is subject to controversy because it relies on median splits, and future work may benefit from using alternative measures of gender role endorsement. Results should also be replicated in the context of other stressor tasks. This is because some research finds that men and women respond differently to stressors emphasizing performance versus interpersonal rejection (Stroud et al., Citation2002), and implications of this finding for gender roles categories are unclear. Finally, the present findings should be replicated in other populations (e.g. older adults) to further evaluate generalizability.

Conclusions

In conclusion, gender role endorsement was associated with cortisol habituation to repeated acute stress, such that both androgynous and undifferentiated individuals exhibited a significant decrease in total cortisol levels from the first to the second visit (i.e. habituation) whereas feminine and masculine individuals showed no significant decrease in cortisol levels from the first to the second visit (i.e. non-habituation). Foremost, the present results contribute to the literature by suggesting that gender roles may influence HPA axis functioning during episodes of acute stress, consistent with a growing literature linking gender roles with stress-related disease susceptibility.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (83.8 KB)

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Ohio University Student Enhancement Award (awarded to the first author). Katrina R. Hamilton was supported by the Osteopathic Heritage Foundation’s Graduate Assistantship Program at Ohio University Heritage College of Osteopathic Medicine while completing this work. Robert-Paul Juster is supported by the Fonds de recherche Québec Sante and holds a Sex and Gender Science Chair from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research.

Disclosure statement

The funding sources were not involved in the study design, collection, analysis and interpretation of data, the writing of the report, or the decision to submit the article for publication. The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this paper.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Andrew W. Manigault

Andrew W. Manigault earned his Ph.D. in Experimental Health Psychology from Ohio University in 2020. He is now a postdoctoral research fellow at the University of California, Los Angeles. Broadly, his research examines stress management strategies, cognitive tendencies, and social factors as they relate to biological stress-response systems. His postdoctoral research examines factors underlying vulnerability to inflammation-induced depressive symptoms.

Ryan C. Shorey

Ryan C. Shorey earned his Ph.D. in Clinical Psychology from the University of Tennessee – Knoxville in 2014. He is now an Assistant Professor of Psychology at the University of Wisconsin – Milwaukee. His research focuses on intimate partner violence, substance use, and mindfulness-based interventions.

Haley Appelmann

Haley Appelmann earned her B.S. in Neuroscience from Ohio University in 2020. She is now a clinical research assistant in the Early Intervention Program at the University of Cincinnati. Her research focuses on the prevalence of COVID-19 in the local community, as well as identifying genetic markers associated with opioid use disorder.

Katrina R. Hamilton

Katrina R. Hamilton earned her Ph.D. in Experimental Psychology from Ohio University in 2019. Currently, she is a postdoctoral fellow in the Institute for Interdisciplinary Salivary Bioscience Research at the University of California, Irvine. Broadly, her work has focused on acute and chronic stress, salivary assay methodology, and noninvasive interventions for individuals with chronic pain.

Matt C. Scanlin

Matt C. Scanlin earned his Ph.D. in Experimental Psychology and his MPH from Ohio University. He is currently a Fellow with the Wisconsin Population Health Service Fellowship at the School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin. As part of his fellowship, he works at the Wisconsin Department of Health Services, where his work is focused on epidemiology and project management within the Bureau of Communicable Diseases.

Robert-Paul Juster

Robert-Paul Juster earned his Ph.D. in Neuroscience from McGill University in 2015. He is now an Assistant Research Professor in the Department of Psychiatry and Addiction at the University of Montreal. As a Canadian Institutes of Health Research Sex and Gender Science Chair, his research focuses on stress physiology and psychosocial resilience in diverse human populations.

Peggy M. Zoccola

Peggy M. Zoccola earned her Ph.D. in Psychology and Social Behavior from the University of California, Irvine, in 2010. She is now an Associate Professor and Director of Experimental Training at Ohio University. Her work focuses on cognitive, emotional, and social factors that influence stress physiology and health.

References

Appendix A

Cortisol AUCg

The interaction of gender role category x visit was non-significant when predicting cortisol AUCg (F(3,75) = 2.05, p = .11). However, we observed follow-up contrasts that resembled those reported in the main body of the manuscript. See supplemental Figure S1 for a graphical representation for this interaction. Follow up contrasts revealed that the undifferentiated group exhibited a larger decrease in cortisol AUCg from visit 1 to visit 2 than the feminine group (b = −103.63, t(75) = 2.10, p = .039) and the masculine group (b = −100.27, t(75) = 1.97, p = .052) but not the androgynous group (b = −37.52, t(75) = .77, p = .44). No other between-group contrasts were significant (all ps > .05). Of interest, both the androgynous and undifferentiated groups showed a significant decrease in cortisol AUCg from visit 1 to visit 2 (b = −92.94, t(75) = 2.62, p = .010, and b = −130.47, t(75) = 3.93, p < .001, respectively), whereas the feminine and masculine groups did not (b = −26.83, t(75) = 0.73, p = .46, and b = −30.20, t(75) = 0.79, p = .43, respectively). The interaction of gender role category x visit became significant after controlling for sex, wake time, experiment start time, and study conditions (F(3,65) = 3.48, p = .021). Moreover, the interaction of sex x gender role category x visit was non-significant (F(3,62) = 0.30, p = .82) suggesting that biological sex did not moderate the effect of gender role category on cortisol habituation. Within the first or the second visit, comparing cortisol AUCg across gender category groups revealed no significant contrasts (all ps > .05).

Cortisol reactivity scores

For each visit, cortisol reactivity scores were computed by subtracting log-transformed cortisol values for samples obtained 0 min post-stressor onset from samples obtained 25 min post stressor onset. Accordingly, positive cortisol reactivity scores index an increase in cortisol reactivity from 0 to 25 min post-stressor onset.

The interaction of gender role category x visit was non-significant when predicting cortisol reactivity scores (F(3,78) = 1.71, p = .17). However, follow-up contrasts revealed a pattern of results that mirrored findings reported in the main body of the manuscript. Consistent with , the undifferentiated group exhibited a larger decrease in cortisol reactivity scores from visit 1 to visit 2 than the masculine group (b = −.38, t(78) = 2.16, p = .034) but no other between-group contrasts were significant (all ps > .05). Of interest, both the androgynous and undifferentiated groups showed a significant decrease in cortisol reactivity from visit 1 to visit 2 (b = −.25, t(78) = 2.00, p = .049, and b = −.35, t(78) = 2.98, p = .003, respectively), whereas the feminine and masculine groups did not (b = −.13, t(78) = 1.04, p = .30, and b = .02, t(78) = 0.21, p = .83, respectively). The interaction of gender role category x visit remained non-significant after controlling for sex, wake time, experiment start time, and study conditions (F(3,68) = 1.96, p = .13). Moreover, the interaction of sex x gender role category x visit was non-significant (F(3,74) = 1.08, p = .38) suggesting that biological sex did not moderate the effect of gender role category on cortisol habituation. Within the first visit, comparing cortisol scores across gender category groups revealed no significant contrasts (all ps > .05). However, within the second visit, gender role category predicted cortisol reactivity scores (F(3, 79) = 4.96, p = .003), such that the undifferentiated group exhibited significantly lower visit 2 reactivity scores than the feminine and masculine groups (b = −.37, t(79) = 2.85, p = .005, and b = .47, t(79) = 3.53, p < .001, respectively). The androgynous group also exhibited significantly lower visit 2 reactivity scores than the masculine group (b = −.28, t(79) = 2.04, p = .044); no other contrasts were significant (all ps > .05).

Appendix B

The following section compares the androgynous, feminine, masculine, and undifferentiated groups on the basis of pre- and post-intervention levels of trait mindfulness, equanimity, experiential avoidance, habitual coping strategies, perceived control over thoughts, worry, rumination, and difficulty with emotional regulation.

Measures

The following measures were administered pre-intervention (i.e. during group consenting sessions) and post-intervention (i.e. as part of online surveys administered after the last session of the interventions and before the first laboratory visit).

Mindfulness. Trait mindfulness was assessed using the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer, 2006). Participants rated the degree to which a series of 39 statements were generally true for them over the past 6-weeks on a 5-point scale (1 = never or very rarely true; 5 = very often or always true). The FFMQ is comprised of 5 subscales (i.e. observe, describe, act with awareness, non-judgment, and non-reactivity. As such, separate averages were computed for each subscale of the FFMQ.

Equanimity. Equanimity was measured using the Non Attachment Scale (NAS; Sahdra, Shaver, & Brown, 2010). Participants rated the degree to which they agree with 30 statements when considering the past 6-weeks on 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). Scores on this scale were averaged.

Experiential Avoidance. Experiential avoidance was measured using the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire II (AAQ-II; Bond et al., 2011). Participants rated the degree to which 10 statements were true for them over the past 6-weeks on a 7-point scale (1 = never true; 7 = always true). Scores on this scale were averaged.

Habitual Coping Strategies. Habitual coping was assessed using the brief-COPE Inventory (Carver, 1997). Participants rated the degree to which they engaged in 28 statements over the past 6 weeks using a 4-point scale (1 = I haven’t been doing this at all; 5 = I’ve been doing this a lot). The Brief Cope inventory captures 14 distinct coping strategies (using the average of 2 questionnaire items per strategies). Consistent with prior work (Wilson, Pritchard, & Revalee, 2005), the 14 coping strategies measured by the Brief Cope were grouped into an emotion-focused coping subscale (i.e. substance use, use of emotional support, venting, positive reframing, humor, acceptance, religion, and self-blame), a problem-focused coping subscale (i.e. active coping, use of instrumental support, and planning), or avoidant coping subscale (i.e. distraction, denial, behavioral disengagement). Scores for each of these subscales were averaged separately.

Perceived Control over Thoughts. Perceived control over thoughts was measured using the Thought Control Ability Questionnaire (TCAQ; Luciano, Algarabel, Tomás, & Martínez, 2005). Participants rated the degree to which they agreed with 25 statements in the last 6 weeks on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). Scores on this scale were averaged.

Worry. Worry was measured using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990). Participants rated the degree to which 16 statements were typical for them over the last 6 weeks using a 5-point scale (1 = not at all typical of me; 5 = very typical of me). Scores on this scale were summed.

Rumination. Rumination was measured using the rehearsal subscale of the Revised Emotional Control Questionnaire (ECQ-2R; Roger & Najarian, 1989). Participants rated the degree to which they felt that 14 statements had been true or false for them over the past 6 weeks on a 2-point scale (0 = TRUE; 1 = FALSE). Scores on this scale were summed.

Difficulty with Emotion Regulation. Difficulty with emotional regulation was assessed using the Difficulty with Emotional Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2003). Participants rated how often 36 statements applied to them over the past 6-weeks on a 5-point scale (1 = almost never 0–10%; 5 = almost always 91–100%). Scores on this scale were summed.

Results

As shown on Supplemental Table 1, pre-intervention contrasts of the androgynous, feminine, masculine and undifferentiated group revealed significant or marginal between-group difference in non-reactivity, equanimity, perceived control over thoughts, and difficulty with emotional regulations. Follow up contrasts revealed that the androgynous group exhibited higher levels of mindful non-reactivity, equanimity and perceived control over thoughts than the feminine (non-reactivity: p = .012; equanimity: p = .001; and perceived control over thoughts: p = .017), masculine (non-reactivity: p = .011; equanimity: p = .046; and perceived control over thoughts: p = .073), and undifferentiated group (non-reactivity: p = .073; equanimity: p = .023; and perceived control over thoughts: p = .079). Finally, the androgynous group exhibited lower levels of difficulty with emotional regulation than the feminine (p = .022) and masculine groups (p = .055), but not the undifferentiated group (p = .53). No other between-group contrasts were significant or marginally significant (all ps < .10).

As shown on supplemental Table 1, the androgynous, feminine, masculine and undifferentiated group did not differ on any of the post-intervention measures tested (all ps > .26).  

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.