Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Physiological stress in response to multitasking and work interruptions: Study protocol

  • Linda Becker ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    linda.becker@fau.de

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Chair of Health Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nürnberg, Erlangen, Germany

  • Helena C. Kaltenegger,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute and Clinic for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, LMU University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany

  • Dennis Nowak,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute and Clinic for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, LMU University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany

  • Matthias Weigl,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute and Clinic for Occupational, Social and Environmental Medicine, LMU University Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany, Institute for Patient Safety, University Hospital, Bonn, Germany

  • Nicolas Rohleder

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychology, Chair of Health Psychology, Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nürnberg, Erlangen, Germany

Abstract

Background

The biopsychological response patterns to digital stress have been sparsely investigated so far. Important potential stressors in modern working environments due to increased digitalization are multitasking and work interruptions. In this study protocol, we present a protocol for a laboratory experiment, in which we will investigate the biopsychological stress response patterns to multitasking and work interruptions.

Methods

In total, N = 192 healthy, adult participants will be assigned to six experimental conditions in a randomized order (one single-task, three dual-task (two in parallel and one as interruption), one multitasking, and one passive control condition). Salivary alpha-amylase as well as heart rate as markers for Sympathetic Nervous System Activity, heart rate variability as measure for Parasympathetic Nervous System (PNS) activity, and cortisol as measure for activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis will be assessed at six time points throughout the experimental session. Furthermore, inflammatory markers (i.e., IL-6, C-reactive protein (CRP), and secretory immunoglobulin-A) will be assessed before and after the task as well as 24 hours after it (IL-6 and CRP only). Main outcomes will be the time course of these physiological stress markers. Reactivity of these measures will be compared between the experimental conditions (dual-tasking, work interruptions, and multitasking) with the control conditions (single-tasking and passive control).

Discussion

With this study protocol, we present a comprehensive experiment, which will enable an extensive investigation of physiological stress-responses to multitasking and work interruptions. Our planned study will contribute to a better understanding of physiological response patterns to modern (digital) stressors. Potential risks and limitations are discussed. The findings will have important implications, especially in the context of digital health in modern working and living environments.

Introduction

Rationale

Stress is one important factor influencing human health [e.g., 1,2]. Stress is part of everyday private and working life, is experienced by almost everyone, and is increasingly having an impact on health and life expectancy [3,4]. In modern, technology-driven working and living environments, new potential stressors related to digitalization (i.e., digital stressors) are more and more present. In the following, we will–similar to the concepts of techno strain and technostress [5,6]–refer to stress that is related to the usage of digital technology and media as digital stress. Important potential digital stressors are multitasking and work interruptions [e.g., due to flooding text messages or emails; 711]. Both can be perceived as stressful and overwhelming [1214] and cannot be avoided in many situations. However, this does not necessarily indicate that the feeling of being stressed or overwhelmed when being faced with these demands is also associated with a physiological stress response [15,16]. Although the biopsychological effects of several acute [e.g., social evaluation; 17] and chronic psychosocial stressors [e.g., caregiving; 18,19] and psychological determinants of biological stress-response patterns in general are well understood, only few attempts have been made so far to use this knowledge to understand the effects of stressors such as multitasking and work interruptions on biological stress system-activity. The aim of our planned study is to close this research gap.

The interpretation of a situation as threatening activates stress centers in the brain, which use stress systems to prepare the entire organism for dealing with the situation [20]. The Sympathetic Nervous System (SNS) activates systems throughout the body through noradrenergic innervation, which leads to the release of epinephrine and norepinephrine from the adrenal medulla and, among other things, results in an increase in heart rate and blood pressure [20,21]. The up-regulation of the SNS is accompanied by a down-regulation of the Parasympathetic Nervous System [PNS; 1,21]. The slower response of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis modulates the effects of the SNS and PNS by releasing the stress hormone cortisol from the adrenal cortex [21,22]. With a further delay [e.g., about 1.5–2 hours for interleukin-6 (IL-6); 23,24], complex effects of the immune system are activated with up-regulation of some components (most importantly inflammatory pathways) and down-regulation of others [most importantly cellular immunity; 2,25]. Temporarily, all these physiological stress responses are adaptive. Potentially harmful consequences arise when stress becomes chronic, i.e., when long-term stress exposure occurs [e.g., 19,2628], or when so-called maladaptive stress-response patterns are used [e.g., 2931]. We refer to stress-responses patterns as being maladaptive–in contrast to adaptive [e.g., 32,33]–when they do not allow the organism to efficiently cope with or to adjust the individual’s physiological responses or behavior to the situation.

With regard to stress effects on health, SNS, PNS, and HPA axis interact with patho-physiologically relevant systems, of which the inflammatory system is seen–beside e.g., glucocorticoids [34]–as one key factor [35]. Inflammatory processes are one of the central mechanisms in mediating the negative effects of stress on health [35]. Ultimately, acute stress exposure leads to systemic low-grade inflammation, which is–in the long-term–a key factor for the development of the most important diseases in industrialized nations such as cardiovascular diseases, type-2 diabetes, and cancer [35,36]. Moreover, these patho-physiological stress-related processes are associated with a large number of other diseases such as chronic dermatological conditions (e.g., skin aging [37], urticaria [38,39], or skin tumors; [40]), asthma bronchiale [41,42], or obesity [43,44], and many more.

Although physiological responses can be triggered by stressors in principle, the actual stress response is associated with the nature of the stressor [so-called specificity hypothesis; 45,46]. According to this hypothesis, specifically situations that are perceived as threatening in contrast to challenging trigger HPA axis responses. Moreover, situations which are shameful or in which the social self is devaluated are associated with strong HPA responses [so-called social self-preservation theory; 4749]. For cognitive stressors, both SNS and HPA axis responses have been reported [50,51], depending on task difficulty and on the presence of further stressors [52]. In a recent systematic review and meta-analysis from our group, which is currently under review [53], we found that SNS activity is significantly higher and PNS activity is significantly lower during dual- or multitasking than during single tasking. We identified no associations with HPA axis activity. However, the number of studies in which HPA axis reactivity to dual- or multitasking was investigated was small. We found no eligible studies in which immune system (re-) activity was investigated.

To summarize, so far, laboratory experiments in which the potential stressors multitasking and work interruptions are systematically induced are rare. Moreover, they vary in their potential how the biological stress systems are modulated and investigated. Multitasking and work interruptions differ from commonly investigated stressors in their nature as they primarily include a cognitive component in contrast to a psychosocial one, especially when induced digitally (i.e., without the presence of further persons). Therefore, with regard to the specificity hypothesis, it remains an open question whether physiological stress responses to multitasking and work interruptions differ between digital and non-digital stressors. In our planned study, we will therefore, differentiate between pure digital multitasking as well as work interruptions and comparable tasks in which another person is involved.

Objectives

While the biopsychological effects of acute [e.g., social evaluation; 54] and chronic [e.g., caregiving; 18,19] psychosocial stressors and psychological determinants of biological stress-response patterns in general are well understood [e.g., 55,56], only few attempts have been made so far to use this knowledge to understand the effects of two prominent stressors in modern, digitalized working environments (i.e., multitasking and work interruptions) on biological stress systems. With this protocol, we present a study plan for a comprehensive experiment, in which responses of the SNS, PNS, HPA axis, as well as of the immune system to multitasking and work interruptions will be investigated in a controlled laboratory setting.

Our primary research questions are:

  1. Do dual- and multitasking conditions lead to physiological stress responses in comparison to a single-task control condition or a passive control condition?
  2. Do work interruptions lead to physiological stress responses in comparison to a single-task control condition or a passive control condition?
  3. Do the stress response patterns differ between digital and non-digital stressors?
    Additionally, the following secondary research question will be investigated exploratorily:
  4. Do dual- and multitasking lead to perceived stress responses in comparison to a single-task control condition or a passive control condition?
  5. Is the physiological stress response to dual- or multitasking, or work interruptions associated with person characteristics (e.g., age, sex, body-mass-index (BMI), education) and psychological variables (e.g., personality, coping style, self-efficacy, depression, anxiety, preference for multitasking [so-called polychronicity; 57]).

Because the associations with these factors will be investigated exploratorily (i.e., without specific hypotheses), only a subset, which is known to be highly relevant in stress research (i.e., age, sex, BMI), will be considered as covariates in the statistical analyses. The actual hypotheses are specified in the Materials and Methods section below.

Materials and methods

Study design

The study design is a cross-sectional laboratory experiment, in which participants will be randomly assigned to one of six conditions (four experimental conditions and two control conditions; Table 1; Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Experimental conditions.

Each participant will be randomly assigned to one of the six experimental conditions. In condition 5 and 6, an experimenter will be present in the same room as the participant to introduce a non-digital, social-evaluative stress component.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Tasks that will be used in the six experimental conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.t001

Participants

In total, N = 192 (32 per condition) healthy, German-speaking adults between 18 and 40 years will be recruited. Exclusion criteria are diagnosed physical or psychological disorders (operationalized as diagnosed within the last 2 years), regular medication intake (exception oral contraceptives), pregnancy, a BMI ≥ 35 kg/m2, regular smoking (more than 5 cigarettes per week), or being an employee of the Friedrich-Alexander University Erlangen-Nürnberg (FAU). The latter is a requirement from the workers’ council of the FAU.

Ethics approval

The study will be conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki and has been approved by the local ethics committee of the FAU (protocol number: 397_19 B).

Power analysis

An a-priori power analysis has been conducted using G*Power (version 3.1.9.6). We expect to find small, but relevant effects and therefore used an effect size of f = 0.14 for power calculation (f2 = 0.02). We intend to achieve a power of 1 – β = 0.95 and used a Bonferroni-adjusted α-level of αadjusted = 0.05/3 = 0.017 (see data preparation and statistical analysis). With these parameters a total sample size of N = 174 (29 per condition) is needed for an analysis of variance for repeated measurements (rmANOVA) with six measurement time points and six conditions. Based on our experience with similar studies, we expect a small drop-out rate of about 10% due to insufficient saliva or blood volumes and will, therefore, recruit additional 18 participants (3 per condition). This results in a total sample size of N = 192.

Status and timeline of the study

Recruitment has started in August 2021 and data collection has started at the end of September 2021. The first version of this study protocol has been submitted before start of data collection. All recommendations from the reviewers’ first reviews were considered. Data collection will last until the recruitment goal is fulfilled. The actual duration will depend on the development of the Covid-19 pandemic but is intended to last about one year. Data analysis will start after all data will have been collected. Preliminary analyses are not planned.

Experimental conditions

An overview about all experimental conditions is provided in Table 1. Condition 1 will be a passive control condition, in which participants will watch a non-stressful video. Condition 2 will be an active control condition, in which participants will conduct a digital single-task, which is the primary task in the other (dual- and multitasking as well as the interruption) experimental conditions. In conditions 3 to 5, participants will conduct the primary task either in combination with a parallel secondary task or will be interrupted by a secondary task. Participants who are assigned to condition 6 will be engaged in multitasking, which will include three tasks, the primary task as well as a digital and a non-digital parallel task. All conditions are visualized in Fig 1 and are summarized in Table 1.

Tasks

Digital primary task.

A computerized continuous-performance task [CPT; 58,59] will be the primary task for the single-task as well as the dual-task and multitasking conditions (conditions 2–6 in Fig 1). The CPT is a measure for sustained attention [60]. We will use an AX-CPT variant [e.g., 61], i.e. that the target is the letter ‘X’ occurring after an ‘A’. In our version (Fig 2), the letters A, B, X, and Y will be presented, and a response button should be pressed after every second letter, i.e., after a pair of letters. The cue letter is ‘A’ and the target letter is ‘X’, i.e., that one response button should be pressed after each A-X pair. In all other cases (after A-Y, B-X, B-Y pairs), the other response button should be pressed (Fig 2). In our AX-CPT version, the letter presentation time is 2,000 ms and the inter-stimulus interval is 3,500 ms. The probability of an A-X pair is 0.50 and 0.17 for the other pairs.

thumbnail
Fig 2. AX-CPT.

In our version of the AX-CPT, the target pair will be A-X. A-Y, B-X, as well as B-Y pairs will be non-targets. This task will be used as the digital single-task and will be the primary task in the dual- and multitasking conditions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.g002

Digital secondary task.

As a digital secondary task, questions and five answer possibilities, of which one is correct, will be presented to the participants. The answer possibilities will be assigned to five answer buttons, and the instruction is that the correct answer should be chosen as fast as possible. As questions, items from the Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000R [IST- 2000R; 62,63] will be used and will be presented in a randomized order. The following sub-tasks will be chosen: sentence addition (Satzergänzung), analogies (Analogien), and figure selection (Figurenauswahl). In condition 3 (digital interruptions), the primary task will be interrupted by questions from the IST 2000R, which will appear on the same monitor. The primary task will stop and will be overlayed by the secondary one during the presentation of the secondary task and will automatically continue after a response is given by the participant. In condition 4 (dual-task, parallel condition), participants will have to perform the IST 2000R on a separate computer screen parallel to the primary task. The participants will be instructed that both tasks are equally relevant, i.e., that none of the tasks should be prioritized, and that both tasks should be performed as accurately as possible.

Non-digital secondary task.

As non-digital secondary task, participants will have to conduct a verbal-fluency task [VFT; e.g., 50,64]. The instruction, which will be verbally given by the experimenter, is to name as many words as possible that belong to a given category or which begin with a given letter. The time per category/letter will be 2 minutes, and time between two rounds will be 1 minute. Each participant will conduct six rounds with the same categories/letters in a randomized order. This secondary task will be conducted in parallel to the primary task (i.e., the AX-CPT; condition 5 in Fig 1). The experimenter will be in the same room as the participant to introduce a social-evaluative component. As for the digital parallel task, participants will be instructed that both tasks are equally relevant, i.e., that none of the tasks should be prioritized, and that both tasks should be performed as accurately as possible.

In the multitasking condition (condition 6 in Fig 1), all three tasks (AX-CPT, the digital parallel task, as well as the VFT) should be performed in parallel and, again, none of the tasks should be prioritized.

Passive control condition.

The (digital) passive control condition will be to watch a non-stressful documentary video (condition 1 in Fig 1). The content of the video is landscapes and animals. These videos have been successfully used in previous studies and evaluated as being non-arousing [65,66].

Biological and physiological outcome measures

Main outcomes will be measures for physiological stress responses, which will be assessed via saliva and capillary blood samples as well as by means of electrocardiogram (ECG) recordings. An overview is provided in Table 2. As measures for SNS re-activity, salivary alpha-amylase [67] as well as the participants’ heart rate will be used. PNS re-activity will be assessed from heart rate variability (HRV) measures [e.g., root mean square of successive differences of consecutive R-R intervals (RMSSD); 68]. Activity of the HPA axis will be assessed via salivary cortisol measurements, which will be collected by means of Salivettes (Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany). IL-6 and CRP will be assessed from capillary blood samples, which will be measured in Dried Blood Spots [69,70], which is an established procedure in our group [e.g., 71,72]. s-IgA will be measured in unstimulated saliva samples, which will be collected by means of Salicaps (IBL international, Hamburg, Germany).

thumbnail
Table 2. Biological and physiological outcome measures, number of samples, collection time point, and assessment procedure.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.t002

Assessment of sample characteristics

Demographic, anthropometric, socio-economic, lifestyle, as well as health-related variables will collected via questionnaires (e.g., age, sex, ethnicity, education, occupation, mother tongue, smoking status, diseases, medication intake). Some of these characteristics (e.g., smoking status and mother tongue) will be only collected to double-check whether inclusion criteria are fulfilled. A full item list is provided as Supplementary Material in S1 File.

Stress perception, affect and anxiety during the experiment

During the experimental session, stress perception, anxiety, and affect as measures for the perceived (non-physiological) stress response will be assessed via the following instruments:

Affect.

Positive and negative affect will be measured four times during the experiment by means of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [PANAS; 73,74]. Assessment time points will be before the task, immediately after it, 20 minutes after the task, as well as 90 minutes later.

State anxiety.

State anxiety and depression before and immediately after the task will be measured by using the state items from the State-Trait Anxiety-Depression Inventory [STADI-S; 75].

Stress perception.

Perceived stress will be assessed throughout the experiment at the time points of the saliva samples by means of 10-point Likert scales with the anchors “not stressed at all” and “extremely stressed”. This scale has been successfully used in previous studies from our group [e.g., 7679]. Additionally, participants are asked about their perceived exertion as well as their level of tiredness, again on 10-point Likert scales. All three scales are provided as Supplementary Material in S2 File.

Psychological variables

Beside demographic, anthropometric, and health -related variables (see above), the acute stress response is related with a variety of psychological variables [e.g., 55]. Therefore, psychological variables as well as lifestyle factors, which might be related with the stress responses under study, will be assessed additionally. The following standardized questionnaires or in-house developed items will be used.

Burnout.

The Maslach Burnout Inventory [MBI; 80] will be used for assessment of burnout symptoms.

Coping.

Coping will be assessed by means of the German 24-item version of the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations [CISS; 81,82]. The scale assesses task-oriented, emotion-focused, as well as avoidance-oriented coping. The avoidance-oriented coping scale can be further divided into distraction coping and social-diversion coping subscales.

Depression.

Depression will be assessed by means of the German version of the long form of the depression scale from the Center for Epidemiological Studies [CES-D; 83,84].

Emotion regulation.

A German version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire [ERQ; 85,86] will be used to assess the emotion regulation dimensions reappraisal and suppression.

Habitual multimedia consumption.

For assessment of habitual television and internet usage, items from a questionnaire for the assessment of habitual media consumption by Koch [2010; 87] will be used. Each subscale consists of 8 items that should be answered on 7-point Likert-scales. A further scale, which assesses habitual mobile phone usage, has been developed analogously based on the items by Koch [2010, 87] for habitual television and internet usage.

Multimedia usage.

Self-developed items will be used to assess time of day at which media is used, separately for weekdays and weekends. A German version of the Media and Technology Usage and Attitudes Scale [MTUAS; 88] will be used to assess frequency of multimedia usage, social media activity, as well as attitudes towards media usage. The polychronicity (i.e., the preference for multitasking) items from the MTUAS will be left out. Polychronicity will be assessed by means of the Multitasking Preference Inventory [MPI; 57] instead.

Multitasking.

The amount and causes of multitasking behavior in everyday life will be assessed by means of 8 self-developed items, which will be answered on 5-point Likert Scales. The items are provided as Supplementary Material in S3 File.

Perceived stress.

Perceived stress during the last month will be assessed by means of a German translation of the 10-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale [PSS; 89,90].

Personality.

For personality assessment, the short version of the Big Five Inventory [BFI-K; 91] will be used. This questionnaire enables assessment of the big-five personality-dimensions extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience by means of 21 items.

Resilience.

For assessment of resilience, the German 11-item resilience scale [RS-11; 92] will be used.

Self-efficacy.

Self-efficacy will be assessed by means of a German scale for general self-efficacy [SWE; 93]. The scale includes ten items, with which self-efficacy is rated on 4-point Likert scales.

Social anxiety.

Social anxiety will be assessed by means of the Social Interaction Anxiety Scale (SIAS) as well as the Social Phobia Scale [SPS; 94].

Social support.

The 14-item version of the Fragebogen zur sozialen Unterstützung [FSozU-K14; 95] will be used to assess social support.

Trait anxiety.

Trait anxiety and depression will assessed by means of the trait-items from the State-Trait Anxiety-Depression Inventory [STADI-T; 75]. Note that we will use the CES-D as main outcome measure for depression. However, we will leave in the depression items to not alter the psychometric properties of the STADI-T.

Experimental setting and procedure

After providing informed and written consent, participants will be equipped with the heart rate monitors and will be familiarized with the saliva collection procedure via Salivettes (i.e., the collection of the stimulated saliva samples), and a practice saliva sample (s0) will be collected, which will not be used for later analyses. After this, all participants–irrespective of the actual group assignment–will conduct practice trials of the VFT and the CPT and will be informed that they will possibly have to repeat these tasks throughout the session. This will be followed by a resting period of about 20 minutes. At the end of the resting period, the first blood spot sample (DBS1) and the next stimulated saliva sample (s1) as well as the first unstimulated saliva sample (us1) will be collected. After this, the main task will be introduced to the participants and will then start immediately and will last about 21 minutes. Introduction of the task and the actual task will take a total of about 25 minutes. Immediately after the end of the main task, participants will provide the second saliva sample (s2; i.e., end of the main task +0 minutes). The subsequent stimulated saliva samples will be collected at the following time points: +10 (s3), +20 (s4), +45 (s5), and +90 (s6) minutes after the end of the main task. The second Dried Blood Spot (DBS2) as well as the second unstimulated saliva sample (us2) will also be collected at + 90 minutes post task. During the time window between the task and the last saliva and blood sample, participants will rest and will fill out questionnaires until the end of the session and will be allowed to take as many breaks as they prefer. The goal of this period is not to stress the participants while conducting a non-stressful task (i.e., filling-out the questionnaires). The timeline of the whole experiment is shown in Fig 3. The entire session will last about 3 hours. Additionally, participants will provide another blood sample (DBS3) 24 hours after the first one. This DBS collection will be conducted by the participants themselves at their homes and samples will be send back via mail.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Timeline of the experiment.

The entire session will last about 3 hours. A further Dried Blood Spot Sample (DBS3) will be collected 24 hours after DBS1 (not shown). Note. CPT: continuous performance task; DBSi: Dried Blood Spot #i; HRV: heart rate variability; si: stimulated saliva sample #i, which will be collected by means of Salivettes; usi: unstimulated saliva sample #i; VFT: verbal-fluency task.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.g003

Outcome measures

Primary outcomes.

Primary outcomes will be the physiological stress measures for SNS, PNS, HPA axis, and immune system reactivity (i.e., sAA, HR, RMSSD, cortisol, CRP, s-IgA, and IL-6).

Secondary outcomes.

Secondary outcomes will be subjective stress perception, state anxiety, and affect as well as associations between the time course of the primary outcomes with person characteristics (e.g., age and sex) and psychological variables (e.g., personality, coping style, self-efficacy, depression, anxiety, polychronicity).

Hypotheses

Main hypotheses.

With regard to the specificity hypothesis [45,46], we expect that pure digital stressors will trigger physiological responses of the SNS (i.e., an up-regulation), PNS (i.e., a down-regulation), as well as an activation of the immune system, but not of the HPA axis (due to the absence of a social-evaluative component). An additional activation of the HPA axis (i.e., an increase of cortisol levels) is expected for the conditions 5 and 6 (non-digital parallel dual-tasking and multitasking), in which we included a social-evaluative component by the presence of an experimenter.

Our main hypotheses, that are associated with research questions 1, 2, and 3 are:

  1. Conditions 3 and 4 (digital work interruptions and digital parallel dual-tasking) will trigger responses of the SNS, PNS, and the immune system that are stronger than in the passive control condition 1 and the single-task control condition 2. No HPA axis response are expected for conditions 3 and 4.
  2. Conditions 5 and 6 (non-digital parallel dual-tasking and multitasking) will trigger responses of the SNS, PNS, HPA axis, and the immune system that are stronger than in the passive control condition 1 and the single-task control condition 2.

Regarding the time course of the physiological responses (if any will be found), we hypothesize fast responses of the SNS and PNS with a maximum immediately after the tasks. The HPA axis response is expected to be delayed with a maximum 20 minutes after the tasks. Slower responses are expected for the immune system with a maximum of 90 minutes after the task for IL-6 and s-IgA and 24 hours after the experimental session for CRP.

Secondary hypotheses.

We hypothesize that conditions 3, 4, 5, and 6 will induce the perception of being stressed and that perceived stress will be stronger in these conditions than in the passive control condition 1 and the single-task control condition 2 immediately after the stressor. Furthermore, we expect to find an increase in state anxiety and in negative affect as well as a decrease in positive affect immediately after the tasks, which will be stronger than in the passive control condition 1 and the single-task control condition 2. Moreover, we expect that the physiological stress response patterns will be associated with person characteristics [e.g., age and sex; e.g., 55] and psychological variables (e.g., personality, coping style, self-efficacy, depression, anxiety, and polychronicity).

Sample handling and laboratory analysis of blood and saliva samples

The saliva and blood samples will be analyzed in our in-house laboratory (FAU, Chair of Health Psychology, Biopsychological Laboratory, Nürnberg, Germany) by trained staff using established procedures [e.g., 71]. After collection, Salivettes and Salicaps will be stored at -30°C. On the analysis day, they will be thawed and centrifuged at 2,000 g at 4°C before further processing. The DBS samples will be dried for at least 8 hours at room temperature before they will be stored at -30°C. The further handling during analysis will also be conducted according to established procedures [e.g., 69,71,96]. In short, a circle with a diameter of 3.5 mm will be punched out and will be eluted overnight in phosphate buffered saline which contains 0.1% Tween 20 solution. The next morning, samples will be shaken at 300 rpm for one hour before further processing.

Concentration of sAA will be measured with an enzyme kinetic assay, as described elsewhere [e.g., 97]. For salivary cortisol, CRP, as well as s-IgA measurement, high-sensitive Enzyme-linked Immunosorbant Assays [ELISA; e.g., 98,99] will be used. For IL-6 measurement, a ProQuantum-Immunoassay-Kit (ThermoFisher Scientific, USA) will be used. For determination of absolute CRP concentrations, linear regressions will be conducted which been validated in-house. sAA, cortisol, CRP, and s-IgA analyses will be conducted in duplicates and IL-6 analyses in triplicates. Analyses will be repeated if inter- or intra- coefficients of variation will be greater than 10%.

Heart-rate variability analysis

Several HRV parameters can be derived from the ECG signal. The most prominent one, that is calculated in the time domain of the signal, is the RMSSD which is usually used as a marker for PNS activity [68]. The RMSSD will be the main HRV measure for our analyses and will be used for main hypotheses testing. Yet, there are further HRV parameters that can be extracted from the HRV signal, e.g., in the very-low frequency (VLF), low frequency (LF), and high-frequency (HF) range, which can be derived from the power spectrum of the HRV signal [68,77]. It has been suggested that VLF power is associated with SNS activity, LF power with both SNS and PNS activity, and HF power with PNS activity [68]. However, there remains a debate whether all these components in the frequency domain reflect parts of both, SNS and PNS activity [100]. Therefore, these components will not be included in our main analyses, but additional analyses will be conducted. Another frequently used HRV parameter as a measure for sympatho-vagal balance is the ratio LF/HF [101], which will also be used for additional analyses in our study. For all these additional HRV analyses (besides the RMSSD) an adjusted alpha-level of αadjusted = .001 will be applied.

Randomization

For randomization, computer-generated randomization lists will be used, which will be stored in closed envelopes and will be handed out to the experimenters immediately prior to the experimental sessions. A team member who is not involved in data collection is responsible for randomization. Subjects are blinded and are informed that the intention of the study is the assessment of physiological responses to interaction with digital devices. Participants’ sex will be considered in the randomization process to ensure an equal sex distribution.

Data preparation and statistical analysis

For statistical analysis, IBM SPSS Statistics (version 26 or higher for Windows) will be used. Data will be screened for outliers, and outliers which differ more than 3 standard deviations from the participants’ mean will be excluded from further analysis. Test of normality will be conducted by means of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. If necessary, data will be transformed (e.g., by means of the natural logarithm or square root transformation) to achieve a normality distribution.

For main hypotheses testing (research questions 1, 2, and 3), rmANOVAs will be conducted. In the analyses, the between-subjects factor ‘Condition’ (with six levels, see above) as well as the within-subjects factor ‘Time’ (with either six levels (for sAA, cortisol, and ECG measures), two for CRP and IL-6, and two for s-IgA) will be included. Three separate analyses will be conducted, one for SNS and PNS markers (i.e., sAA, HR, and RMSSD) and six measurement time points (s1 to s6), one for cortisol as the only HPA-axis marker with the same six time points, and one for the immune parameters (i.e., IL-6, CRP, and s-IgA) and two measurement time points. An additional factor ‘Measure’ (e.g., with the levels IL-6, CRP, and s-IgA for immune parameters) will be included if necessary. A Bonferroni-adjusted α-level of αadjusted = 0.05/3 = 0.017 will be used for the main analyses, because three separate rmANOVAs will be conducted. The potential confounders age, sex, BMI, use of oral contraceptives and menstrual cycle phase for female participants, as well as time of day will be included in all statistical analyses as covariates.

Research question 4 (i.e., perceived stress, state anxiety, and affect) will be investigated analogously to research questions 1 to 3 by means of a further rmANOVA. The same αadjusted = 0.017 as for main hypothesis testing will be used. For analysis of research question 5 (i.e., associations with person characteristics and psychological variables), nominal scaled variables (e.g., sex and education) will be included as additional factors in further rmANOVAs. For metric variables, multivariate regression analyses will be conducted in which the potential moderator variables will be included as moderator variables. For these analyses, the SPSS macro PROCESS [102] will be used. For these analyses which refer to research question 5, an adjusted α-level of αadjusted = 0.001 will be used.

Data management plan

To protect the participants’ privacy and to maintain confidentiality, all personal data is stored in password-protected files and secured against unauthorized access by third parties. The raw data and materials are only accessible to project-team members. Each participant is assigned a randomly generated code that does not allow any conclusions to be drawn about the person. Only this code is used for naming files and samples. Only completely anonymized data will be made available to other researchers after completion of the study or in data repositories. Only mean values and group statistics will be reported in publications.

Dissemination and analysis plans

Data analyses will start after data collection is completed. Interim analyses are not planned. At least one paper will be submitted to a leading journal in the field. The pre-processed and anonymized data will be made publicly available at the Open Science Framework (OSF) after an Embargo period of about 5 years.

Discussion

Summary

Psychosocial stress and its immediate and long-term biological effects have been studied relatively well, but a link between psychobiological-oriented stress research and the effects of digital stress has largely been lacking. The aim of our study is to apply the methods of psychobiological stress research in the context of digital stress (i.e., the widespread phenomena multitasking and work interruptions), with the overarching goal to provide the foundations for a better understanding of the health effects of digital stress. We describe a study protocol of a comprehensive experiment that investigates effects of multitasking and work interruptions on physiological stress response patterns including SNS, PNS, HPA axis, and the immune system.

Strengths of the planned study

So far, acute psychobiological stress response patterns have been well researched in the context of “classic” stress research for non-digital stressors. In contrast, psychobiological stress reaction patterns in the context of digital stress have hardly been studied so far [103]. The aim of the presented study is to bring digital stress from every day and working life into the laboratory context, and thus to make it experimentally investigable. Our experimental approach is the key strength of our study. It enables us to systematically induce multitasking demands or work interruptions and compare them with single-task and passive control conditions.

Moreover, our study will enable to differentiate between stress responses to pure digital stressors and stressors that also include a non-digital component (operationalized by the presence of an experimenter). Nevertheless, all active tasks–the digital and the non-digital ones–include a cognitive component and therefore our study design does not allow to differentiate between digital and cognitive stressors. However, this is not a restriction of our study with respect to our definition of digital stress as being related to the usage of digital technology and media, which is independent of the (e.g., mental) processes being involved.

Potential risks

From a practical point of view, there is a risk that the blood and saliva volumes will be too low for analysis. However, this has been included in the sample size-calculation and will be minimized by training the experimenters by experienced researchers. A further potential risk is that the recruitment goal might not be fulfilled, e.g., due to the Covid-19 pandemic. In the case that another lockdown will be imposed, recruitment will be paused.

Limitations

In our study, we will focus on multitasking and work interruptions as highly relevant modern stressors. Nevertheless, a variety of further digital stressors is conceivable [e.g., techno insecurity, techno overload, or techno invasion; 104]. Moreover, our operationalization of multitasking and work interruptions is just one of many possibilities and many more are possible.

A further limitation is that we cannot assess certain factors which might be related with the physiological stress response to multitasking and work interruptions. One of these are primary and secondary appraisal [105], which are known to be associated with acute stress responses in general. However, assessing them between the introduction of the task and the beginning of the task [e.g., by means of the Primary and Secondary Appraisal Scale; 106] would disrupt the procedure too much. A further and related factor which we cannot assess is executive functioning, of which especially attentional control has been shown to be related with HPA axis responses after acute stressors [107]. Furthermore, although we assess intelligence during some of the sub tasks, a comprehensive assessment [including emotional intelligence; 108] would be even more meaningful. A further potential limitation is the chosen passive control condition as we cannot rule out that watching the videos unintendedly leads to either stress induction or relaxation. However, the videos’ contents have been rated as being low arousing in previous studies [65,66]. Moreover, this task is better suited than other potential control tasks in which participants are instructed to do nothing at all, which are–in our opinion–more likely to induce relaxation and which are also much more difficult to control.

We will use a pragmatic age restriction of 40 years, because this has been shown to be suitable in previous studies from our group for recruiting healthy participants who do meet all inclusion criteria. Other age groups (older than 40 years as well as children and adolescents) would be interesting target groups for future follow-up studies. Besides, non-healthy participants with diseases which are known to be associated with stress reactivity or being associated with chronic stress development [e.g., depression or post-traumatic stress disorder, 109] should be subject of further investigations.

Implications

Nevertheless–despite these limitations–, we deem that our study contributes to a deeper understanding on influences of stressors associated with the use of digital technology on humans’ health-outcomes. Specifically, our results are expected to expand our current knowledge base on the impact of multitasking and information load on humans’ psychobiological stress responses with particular focus on physiological response patterns. Stress exerts a strong influence on health via well-described processes [e.g., conceptualized in the Allostatic Load Model; 3,32,110]. In the long-term, stress is negatively associated with quality of life, health, and longevity of individuals and, thus, productivity of society [111,112]. Therefore, our study is of high relevancy.

Given the ubiquitous applications of digital technologies in modern workplaces and living environments, our findings will help to further understand the mechanisms between digital stressors in various occupational settings and adverse health outcomes [113]. Eventually, examination of job-related risks will inform policy and practice interventions in occupational health. Overall, the findings from our study will have important implications for better understanding the long-term health effects of the potential stressors multitasking and work interruptions in several settings.

Conclusions

Our planned study will expand our understanding of the physiological response patterns due to the modern (digital) stressors multitasking and work interruptions. By quantifying objective parameters of biological stress responses and, thus, patho-physiologically relevant markers of digital stress, the health effects of digital stress will be made assessable. The findings will have important implications, especially in the context of health in digitalized working environments.

Supporting information

S1 File. Questionnaire sample characteristics.

Questionnaire that will be used for the assessment of sample characteristics. In the actual study, a German version will be used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.s001

(PDF)

S2 File. Visual-analogous scales.

Visual-analogous scales with which perceived stress, tiredness, and exertion will be assessed. In the actual study, a German version will be used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.s002

(PDF)

S3 File. Multitasking questionnaire.

Questionnaire that will be used for the assessment of multitasking. In the actual study, a German version will be used.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0263785.s003

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We thank Laura Carolina Manns for assisting the development of the multitasking items and Janek Ruß for supporting translation of the MTUAS. Furthermore, we thank Swathi Hassan Gangaraju for programming the tasks and Katharina Hahn for supporting setting-up the experiment.

References

  1. 1. Chrousos GP, Gold PW. The concepts of stress and stress system disorders. Overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis. Jama [Internet] 1992;267(9):1244–52. pmid:1538563
  2. 2. Chrousos GP. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature reviews endocrinology 2009;5(7):374. pmid:19488073
  3. 3. Juster R-P, McEwen BS, Lupien SJ. Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2010;35(1):2–16.
  4. 4. Kopp MS, Réthelyi J. Where psychology meets physiology: chronic stress and premature mortality—the Central-Eastern European health paradox. Brain research bulletin 2004;62(5):351–67. pmid:15168900
  5. 5. Salanova M, Llorens S, Cifre E. The dark side of technologies: Technostress among users of information and communication technologies. International journal of psychology 2013;48(3):422–36. pmid:22731610
  6. 6. Brod C. Managing technostress: optimizing the use of computer technology. Personnel Journal 1982;61(10):753–7. pmid:10258012
  7. 7. Gupta A, Li H, Sharda R. Should I send this message? Understanding the impact of interruptions, social hierarchy and perceived task complexity on user performance and perceived workload. Decision Support Systems 2013;55(1):135–45.
  8. 8. Reinecke L, Aufenanger S, Beutel ME, Dreier M, Quiring O, Stark B, et al. Digital stress over the life span: The effects of communication load and internet multitasking on perceived stress and psychological health impairments in a German probability sample. Media Psychol 2017;20(1):90–115.
  9. 9. Lindström J. Understanding digital distraction: a longitudinal study on disruptive everyday media multitasking among diginatives 2020.
  10. 10. Hefner D, Vorderer P. Digital stress: Permanent connectedness and multitasking. In: The Routledge handbook of media use and well-being: Routledge; 2016. p. 255–67.
  11. 11. Barley SR, Meyerson DE, Grodal S. E-mail as a source and symbol of stress. Organization Science 2011;22(4):887–906.
  12. 12. Mark G, Wang Y, Niiya M, editors. Stress and multitasking in everyday college life: an empirical study of online activity. CHI; Toronto, ON, Canada; 2014.
  13. 13. Kim J, Ingersoll-Dayton B, Kwak M. Balancing eldercare and employment: The role of work interruptions and supportive employers. Journal of Applied Gerontology 2013;32(3):347–69. pmid:25474393
  14. 14. Weigl M, Beck J, Wehler M, Schneider A. Workflow interruptions and stress at work: a mixed-methods study among physicians and nurses of a multidisciplinary emergency department. BMJ Open 2017;7(12):e019074. pmid:29275350
  15. 15. Oldehinkel AJ, Ormel J, Bosch NM, Bouma EMC, van Roon AM, Rosmalen JGM, et al. Stressed out? Associations between perceived and physiological stress responses in adolescents: The TRAILS study. Psychophysiology 2011;48(4):441–52. pmid:21361964
  16. 16. Goldman N, Glei DA, Seplaki C, Liu I-W, Weinstein M. Perceived stress and physiological dysregulation in older adults. Stress 2005;8(2):95–105. pmid:16019601
  17. 17. Burke PJ. Identity processes and social stress. American sociological review 1991:836–49.
  18. 18. Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Preacher KJ, MacCallum RC, Atkinson C, Malarkey WB, Glaser R. Chronic stress and age-related increases in the proinflammatory cytokine IL-6. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2003;100(15):9090–5. pmid:12840146
  19. 19. Rohleder N, Marin TJ, Ma R, Miller GE. Biologic cost of caring for a cancer patient: Dysregulation of pro-and anti-inflammatory signaling pathways. Journal of Clinical Oncology 2009;27(18):2909–15. pmid:19433690
  20. 20. Stress Selye H. and the general adaptation syndrome. British medical journal 1950;1(4667):1383. pmid:15426759
  21. 21. Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature reviews neuroscience 2009;10(6):397. pmid:19469025
  22. 22. Sapolsky RM, Romero LM, Munck AU. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine reviews 2000;21(1):55–89. pmid:10696570
  23. 23. Marsland AL, Walsh C, Lockwood K, John-Henderson NA. The effects of acute psychological stress on circulating and stimulated inflammatory markers: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Behav Immun 2017;64:208–19. pmid:28089638
  24. 24. McInnis CM, Thoma MV, Gianferante D, Hanlin L, Chen X, Breines JG, et al. Measures of adiposity predict interleukin-6 responses to repeated psychosocial stress. Brain Behav Immun 2014;42:33–40. pmid:25107874.
  25. 25. Morey JN, Boggero IA, Scott AB, Segerstrom SC. Current directions in stress and human immune function. Current opinion in psychology 2015;5:13–7. pmid:26086030
  26. 26. Hänsel A, Hong S, Cámara RJA, Kaenel R von. Inflammation as a psychophysiological biomarker in chronic psychosocial stress. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2010;35(1):115–21.
  27. 27. Pretscher A, Kauzner S, Rohleder N, Becker L. Associations between social burden, perceived stress, and diurnal cortisol profiles in older adults: implications for cognitive aging. European Journal of Ageing 2021:1–16.
  28. 28. McEwen BS. Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: Understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. European journal of pharmacology 2008;583(2–3):174–85. pmid:18282566
  29. 29. Wadsworth ME. Development of maladaptive coping: A functional adaptation to chronic, uncontrollable stress. Child development perspectives 2015;9(2):96–100. pmid:26019717
  30. 30. Richardson CME, Rice KG, Devine DP. Perfectionism, emotion regulation, and the cortisol stress response. Journal of Counseling Psychology 2014;61(1):110. pmid:24040777
  31. 31. Russell G, Lightman S. The human stress response. Nature reviews endocrinology 2019;15(9):525–34. pmid:31249398
  32. 32. McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1998;840(1):33–44.
  33. 33. Del Giudice M, Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA. The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2011;35(7):1562–92. pmid:27163558
  34. 34. McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: Central role of the brain. Physiological reviews 2007;87(3):873–904. pmid:17615391
  35. 35. Rohleder N. Stimulation of systemic low-grade inflammation by psychosocial stress. Psychosom Med 2014;76(3):181–9. pmid:24608036.
  36. 36. Couzin-Frankel J. Inflammation bares a dark side: American Association for the Advancement of Science; 2010.
  37. 37. Chen Y, Lyga J. Brain-skin connection: stress, inflammation and skin aging. Inflammation & Allergy-Drug Targets (Formerly Current Drug Targets-Inflammation & Allergy)(Discontinued) 2014;13(3):177–90. pmid:24853682
  38. 38. Gieler U, Gieler T, Schut C, Niemeier V, Peters EM, Kupfer J. Quality of Life and Comorbidities in Urticaria: What Is Known? Current Dermatology Reports 2015;4(2):77–82.
  39. 39. Schut C, Magerl M, Hawro T, Kupfer J, Rose M, Gieler U, et al. Disease activity and stress are linked in a subpopulation of chronic spontaneous urticaria patients. Allergy 2020;75(1):224–6. pmid:31392735
  40. 40. Peters EMJ. Stressed skin?–a molecular psychosomatic update on stress‐causes and effects in dermatologic diseases. JDDG: Journal der Deutschen Dermatologischen Gesellschaft 2016;14(3):233–52. pmid:26972185
  41. 41. Chen E, Miller GE. Stress and inflammation in exacerbations of asthma. Brain Behav Immun 2007;21(8):993–9. pmid:17493786
  42. 42. Kirkham P, Rahman I. Oxidative stress in asthma and COPD: antioxidants as a therapeutic strategy. Pharmacology & therapeutics 2006;111(2):476–94. pmid:16458359
  43. 43. Fernández-Sánchez A, Madrigal-Santillán E, Bautista M, Esquivel-Soto J, Morales-González Á, Esquivel-Chirino C, et al. Inflammation, oxidative stress, and obesity. International journal of molecular sciences 2011;12(5):3117–32. pmid:21686173
  44. 44. Bondia-Pons I, Ryan L, Martinez JA. Oxidative stress and inflammation interactions in human obesity. Journal of physiology and biochemistry 2012;68(4):701–11. pmid:22351038
  45. 45. Kemeny ME. The psychobiology of stress. Current directions in psychological science 2003;12(4):124–9.
  46. 46. Lazarus RS. Theory-based stress measurement. Psychological inquiry 1990;1(1):3–13.
  47. 47. Dickerson SS, Gruenewald TL, Kemeny ME. When the social self is threatened: Shame, physiology, and health. Journal of personality 2004;72(6):1191–216. pmid:15509281
  48. 48. Gruenewald TL, Dickerson SS, Kemeny ME. A social function for self-conscious emotions: The social self preservation theory. pmid:15938548 2007.
  49. 49. Gruenewald TL, Kemeny ME, Aziz N, Fahey JL. Acute threat to the social self: Shame, social self-esteem, and cortisol activity. Psychosom Med 2004;66(6):915–24. pmid:15564358
  50. 50. Becker L, Schade U, Rohleder N. Activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis in response to a verbal fluency task and associations with task performance. PLoS ONE 2020;15(4):e0227721. pmid:32298298
  51. 51. Skoluda N, Strahler J, Schlotz W, Niederberger L, Marques S, Fischer S, et al. Intra-individual psychological and physiological responses to acute laboratory stressors of different intensity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015;51:227–36. pmid:25462896
  52. 52. Wetherell MA, Craw O, Smith K, Smith MA. Psychobiological responses to critically evaluated multitasking. Neurobiol Stress 2017;7:68–73. pmid:28540348
  53. 53. Becker L, Kaltenegger HC, Nowak D, Rohleder N, Weigl M. Differences in stress system (re-) activity between single and dual- or multitasking in healthy adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis submitted.
  54. 54. Kirschbaum Pirke, Hellhammer . The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 1993;28(1–2):76–81. pmid:8255414
  55. 55. Kudielka BM, Hellhammer DH, Wüst S. Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009;34(1):2–18. pmid:19041187
  56. 56. Kudielka BM, Kirschbaum C. Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: A review. Biol Psychol 2005;69(1):113–32. pmid:15740829
  57. 57. Poposki EM, Oswald FL. The multitasking preference inventory: Toward an improved measure of individual differences in polychronicity. Human Performance 2010;23(3):247–64.
  58. 58. Klee SH, Garfinkel BD. The computerized continuous performance task: A new measure of inattention. Journal of abnormal child psychology 1983;11(4):487–95. pmid:6689172
  59. 59. van den Bosch RJ, Rombouts RP, van Asma MJO. What determines continuous performance task performance? Schizophrenia Bulletin 1996;22(4):643–51. pmid:8938918
  60. 60. Borgaro S, Pogge DL, DeLuca VA, Bilginer L, Stokes J, Harvey PD. Convergence of different versions of the continuous performance test: Clinical and scientific implications. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology 2003;25(2):283–92. pmid:12754684
  61. 61. Smid H, de Witte, Homminga I, van den Bosch RJ. Sustained and transient attention in the continuous performance task. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology 2006;28(6):859–83. pmid:16822729
  62. 62. Amthauer R, Brocke B, Liepmann D, Beauducel A. IST 2000 R: Intelligenz-Struktur-Test 2000 R: Hogrefe, Verlag für Psychologie; 2001.
  63. 63. Petermann F. Intelligenz-Struktur-Test. Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie, Psychologie und Psychotherapie 2014;62(3):225–6.
  64. 64. Tombaugh TN, Kozak J, Rees L. Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of clinical neuropsychology 1999;14(2):167–77. pmid:14590600
  65. 65. Langer K, Wolf OT, Jentsch VL. Delayed effects of acute stress on cognitive emotion regulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2021;125:105101. pmid:33460986
  66. 66. Langer K, Jentsch VL, Wolf OT. Cortisol promotes the cognitive regulation of high intensive emotions independent of timing. European Journal of Neuroscience 2021. pmid:33709613
  67. 67. Nater UM, Rohleder N. Salivary alpha-amylase as a non-invasive biomarker for the sympathetic nervous system: current state of research. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009;34(4):486–96. pmid:19249160
  68. 68. Sammito S, Thielmann B, Seibt R, Klussmann A, Weippert M, Böckelmann I. Leitlinie Nutzung der Herzschlagfrequenz und der Herzferquenzvariabilität in der Arbeitsmedizin und der Arbeitswissenschaft. AWMF Registernummer 002–042; 2018.
  69. 69. McDade TW, Burhop J, Dohnal J. High-sensitivity enzyme immunoassay for C-reactive protein in dried blood spots. Clinical chemistry 2004;50(3):652–4. pmid:14981035
  70. 70. McDade TW, Williams S, Snodgrass JJ. What a drop can do: dried blood spots as a minimally invasive method for integrating biomarkers into population-based research. Demography 2007;44(4):899–925. pmid:18232218
  71. 71. Britting S, Kob R, Sieber CC, Rohleder N, Freiberger E, Becker L. Physiological stress in safer cycling in older age (SiFAr-stress): effect of a multicomponent exercise intervention—a study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 2021;22(1). pmid:34419134
  72. 72. Becker L, Dupke A, Rohleder N. Associations Between C-Reactive Protein Levels, Exercise Addiction, and Athlete Burnout in Endurance Athletes. Frontiers in psychology 2021;12:2150.
  73. 73. Crawford JR, Henry JD. The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non‐clinical sample. British journal of clinical psychology 2004;43(3):245–65.
  74. 74. Krohne HW, Egloff B, Kohlmann C-W, Tausch A. Untersuchungen mit einer deutschen Version der”Positive and Negative Affect Schedule“(PANAS). Diagnostica 1996;42:139–56.
  75. 75. Laux L, Hock M, Bergner-Köther R, Hodapp V, Renner K-H. Das State-Trait-Angst-Depressions-Inventar: STADI; Manual 2013.
  76. 76. Becker L, Schade U, Rohleder N. Evaluation of the socially evaluated cold-pressor group test (SECPT-G) in the general population. PeerJ 2019;7:e7521. pmid:31423367
  77. 77. Becker L, Semmlinger L, Rohleder N. Resistance training as an acute stressor in healthy young men: associations with heart rate variability, alpha-amylase, and cortisol levels. Stress 2020:1–13.
  78. 78. Becker L, Rohleder N. Associations between Attention and Implicit Associative Learning in Healthy Adults: The Role of Cortisol and Salivary Alpha-Amylase Responses to an Acute Stressor. Brain Sciences 2020;10(8):544.
  79. 79. Becker L, Rohleder N. Time course of the physiological stress response to an acute stressor and its associations with the primacy and recency effect of the serial position curve. PLoS ONE 2019;14(5):e0213883. pmid:31100063
  80. 80. Maslach C, Jackson SE. Maslach burnout inventory Consulting Psychologists Press Palo Alto. MaslachMaslach Burnout Inventory: Second Edition1986 1986.
  81. 81. Endler N, Parker JDA. Coping inventory for stressful situations 2008.
  82. 82. Kälin W, Semmer N. Coping-Inventar zum Umgang mit Stress-Situationen: Deutschsprachige Adaptation des Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS) von Norman S. Endler und James D. A. Parker: Hogrefe; 2020.
  83. 83. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied psychological measurement 1977;1(3):385–401.
  84. 84. Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Hofmeister D, Keller F. ADS: Allgemeine Depressionsskala: Testhandbuch (2te Ausgabe): Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2012.
  85. 85. Abler B, Kessler H. Emotion regulation questionnaire–Eine deutschsprachige Fassung des ERQ von Gross und John. Diagnostica 2009;55(3):144–52.
  86. 86. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of personality and social psychology 2003;85(2):348. pmid:12916575
  87. 87. Koch T. Macht der Gewohnheit?: der Einfluss der Habitualisierung auf die Fernsehnutzung: Springer; 2010.
  88. 88. Rosen LD, Whaling K, Carrier LM, Cheever NA, Rokkum J. The media and technology usage and attitudes scale: An empirical investigation. Computers in Human Behavior 2013;29(6):2501–11. pmid:25722534
  89. 89. Klein EM, Brähler E, Dreier M, Reinecke L, Müller KW, Schmutzer G, et al. The German version of the Perceived Stress Scale–psychometric characteristics in a representative German community sample. BMC psychiatry 2016;16(1):159.
  90. 90. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. Perceived stress scale. Measuring stress: A guide for health and social scientists 1994:235–83.
  91. 91. Rammstedt B, John OP. Kurzversion des big five inventory (BFI-K). Diagnostica 2005;51(4):195–206.
  92. 92. Schumacher J, Leppert K, Gunzelmann T, Strauß B, Brähler E. Die Resilienzskala–ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung der psychischen Widerstandsfähigkeit als Personmerkmal. Z Klin Psychol Psychiatr Psychother 2005;53(1):16–39.
  93. 93. Jerusalem M, Schwarzer R. SWE-Skala zur Allgemeinen Selbstwirksamkeitserwartung 2003.
  94. 94. Heimberg RG, Mueller GP, Holt CS, Hope DA, Liebowitz MR. Assessment of anxiety in social interaction and being observed by others: The Social Interaction Anxiety Scale and the Social Phobia Scale. Behavior therapy 1992;23(1):53–73.
  95. 95. Fydrich T, Sommer G, Brähler E. Fragebogen zur sozialen Unterstützung: F-SozU: Hogrefe; 2007.
  96. 96. Danese A, Caspi A, Williams B, Ambler A, Sugden K, Mika J, et al. Biological embedding of stress through inflammation processes in childhood. Molecular psychiatry 2011;16(3):244. pmid:20157309
  97. 97. Rohleder N, Nater UM. Determinants of salivary alpha-amylase in humans and methodological considerations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009;34(4):469–85. pmid:19155141.
  98. 98. Becker L, Rohleder N, Schultheiss O. Salivary Hormone Assays. In: APA, editor. APA Handbook Of Research Methods In Psychology; in press. xx-yy.
  99. 99. Lequin RM. Enzyme immunoassay (EIA)/enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA). Clinical chemistry 2005;51(12):2415–8. pmid:16179424
  100. 100. Berntson GG, Thomas Bigger J Jr, Eckberg DL, Grossman P, Kaufmann PG, Malik M, et al. Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 1997;34(6):623–48. pmid:9401419
  101. 101. Malik M. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use: Task force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society for Pacing and Electrophysiology. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology 1996;1(2):151–81.
  102. 102. Hayes AF. Mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach edn. New York: Guilford Publications 2013:1–20.
  103. 103. Riedl R. On the biology of technostress: literature review and research agenda. ACM SIGMIS database: the DATABASE for advances in information systems 2012;44(1):18–55.
  104. 104. Ragu-Nathan TS, Tarafdar M, Ragu-Nathan BS, Tu Q. The consequences of technostress for end users in organizations: Conceptual development and empirical validation. Information systems research 2008;19(4):417–33.
  105. 105. Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping: Springer Publishing Company; 1984.
  106. 106. Gaab J. PASA–Primary Appraisal Secondary Appraisal-Ein Fragebogen zur Erfassung von situations-bezogenen kognitiven Bewertungen. Verhaltenstherapie 2009;19(2):114–5.
  107. 107. Grimm E, Agrigoroaei S, Rohleder N, Becker L. Executive Functioning as a Predictor of Physiological and Subjective Acute Stress Responses in Non-Clinical Adult Populations: A Systematic Literature Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2021. pmid:34562543
  108. 108. Matthews G, Emo AK, Funke G, Zeidner M, Roberts RD, Costa PT Jr, et al. Emotional intelligence, personality, and task-induced stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2006;12(2):96. pmid:16802891
  109. 109. Cohen BE, Edmondson D, Kronish IM. State of the art review: depression, stress, anxiety, and cardiovascular disease. American journal of hypertension 2015;28(11):1295–302. pmid:25911639
  110. 110. Juster R-P, Sindi S, Marin M-F, Perna A, Hashemi A, Pruessner JC, et al. A clinical allostatic load index is associated with burnout symptoms and hypocortisolemic profiles in healthy workers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2011;36(6):797–805. PubMed PMID: pmid:21129851.
  111. 111. Clark MS, Bond MJ, Hecker JR. Environmental stress, psychological stress and allostatic load. Psychology, Health & Medicine 2007;12(1):18–30.
  112. 112. Seeman TE, McEwen BS, Rowe JW, Singer BH. Allostatic load as a marker of cumulative biological risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2001;98(8):4770–5. pmid:11287659
  113. 113. Kaltenegger HC, Becker L, Rohleder N, Nowak D, Weigl M. Associations of working conditions and chronic low-grade inflammation among employees: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Scandinavian journal of work, environment & health 2021;47(8):565–81. pmid:34523689