Next Article in Journal
Design of a Computer Model for the Identification of Adolescent Swimmers at Risk of Low BMD
Previous Article in Journal
A Shift in Asthma Treatment According to New Guidelines: An Evaluation of Asthma Patients’ Attitudes towards Treatment Change
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric Properties and Prediction of Posttraumatic Consequences during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Chilean Adults

by
Felipe E. García
1,*,
Pablo Vergara-Barra
1,
Pablo Concha-Ponce
2,
Mariela Andrades
3,
Paulina Rincón
4 and
Mauricio Valdivia-Devia
5
1
Departamento de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Concepción, Concepción 4070409, Chile
2
Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Talca, Talca 3465548, Chile
3
Escuela de Psicología y Terapia Ocupacional, Universidad Central de Chile, Santiago 8370292, Chile
4
Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad de Concepción, Concepción 4070386, Chile
5
Academia de Ciencias Policiales, Carabineros de Chile, Santiago 7591168, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(4), 3452; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043452
Submission received: 30 December 2022 / Revised: 13 February 2023 / Accepted: 14 February 2023 / Published: 16 February 2023

Abstract

:
The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) is widely used to assess the use of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies to regulate negative emotions. The present study evaluates the psychometric properties, reliability and validity of a Chilean adaptation of the ERQ in a large sample of 1543 participants aged between 18 and 87 (38% male, 62% female). The results of the confirmatory factor analysis showed the expected two-factor structure and factorial invariance in relation to gender. Results also indicated adequate internal consistency, test–retest reliability, convergent and predictive validity in predicting posttraumatic stress symptoms and posttraumatic growth six months after the first measurement in a subsample of students exposed to the COVID-19 pandemic. The use of reappraisal was positively associated with general well-being, whereas the use of suppression was positively associated with depressive symptomatology. In terms of posttraumatic consequences, the use of reappraisal was negatively associated with posttraumatic symptomatology and positively associated with posttraumatic growth six months later; in turn, suppression was positively associated with posttraumatic symptomatology and negatively associated with posttraumatic growth six months later. This study demonstrates that the ERQ is a valid and reliable instrument to measure emotional regulation strategies in Chilean adults.

1. Introduction

The expression and regulation of emotions are part of everyday life and represent one of the fields of study of psychology. The person affected by a stressful event needs to deal with the emotions, sometimes intense and negative, that the situation generates. To do this, emotional regulation strategies are used [1]; better emotional regulation makes one less vulnerable to developing symptoms of emotional distress or relapse [2,3].
The Process Model [4] is one of the most used theories to explain the various emotional regulation strategies. This model defines regulatory strategies as the process by which we influence the emotions we have, when we have them, and the ways in which we experience and express them. It proposes four phases in the generation of these strategies: (a) the modification of the situation that generates the emotion, (b) the attentional deployment to change the focus to another situation, (c) the change in the meaning of the situation, and (d) the modification in the response. Gross and John (2003) highlight two strategies of emotional regulation related to the last two phases mentioned above: cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Cognitive reappraisal is oriented to the change in the meaning of the situation; it involves changing the way we look at the stressful situation to reduce the reaction associated to discomfort. Expressive suppression is oriented to the modification of the response, which consists of the forced attempt not to express the discomfort [5].
It has been observed that cognitive reappraisal has a direct relationship with positive indicators, such as optimism and well-being, and an inverse relationship with negative indicators, such as depression; likewise, expressive suppression has a direct relationship with depressive symptoms [6] and an inverse one with optimism and well-being [5]. Likewise, a relationship of cognitive reappraisal with decreased posttraumatic symptomatology (PTSS) [7] and increased posttraumatic growth (PTG) [8,9,10] has been evidenced. In turn, expressive suppression is related to an increase in levels of discomfort, such as perceived stress, depressive symptoms and PTSS [11], which has been observed in people exposed to contagion or lifestyle changes derived from COVID-19 [7,12].
Expressive suppression has been determined to be a strategy used more by men than by women [13,14,15] and cognitive reappraisal has been determined to influence mood more positively in men than in women [16]. In order to measure these two strategies, Gross and John (2003) proposed the Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), a 10-item instrument originally built in English. It has since been translated into more than 33 languages, but few studies have formally examined its factorial structure beyond university populations [5,17]. Available studies show the presence of the two original factors: expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal, and adequate levels of internal consistency in both subscales [18].
The first known translation of the ERQ into Spanish was conducted by Rodríguez-Carvajal et al. (2006); another late version was developed for Peruvian university students by Gargurevich and Matos (2010) [19,20]. Subsequently, Cabello et al. (2013) presented a new translation for Spaniards [21]. Other studies that have evaluated the psychometric properties of the scale in the Spanish language have been Navarro et al. (2018) [22] and Gómez et al. (2016) in Spaniard adolescents [15], Moreta et al. (2021) in Ecuadorian university students [23,24], Olalde et al. (2022) in Mexican adults [25] and Del Valle et al. (2022) in Argentine university students [26]. In all these studies, a factorial structure of two independent factors was obtained; although in a different sample of Ecuadorian university students, Moreta et al. (2022) detected a better fit in a bifactor model composed of a general factor and two specific factors [23]. Internal consistency was adequate for all studies, with the exception of the version by Navarro et al. (2018) where it was unsatisfactory [22]. The study by Gómez et al. (2016) also evaluated test–retest reliability with good indicators [15].
The convergent validity of ERQ in Spanish has been evaluated with other measures of emotional regulation, in addition to negative affectivity and different psychopathological measures [20,21,27]. Regarding measurement invariance, it has been shown to be temporarily invariant, i.e., the items of the ERQ evaluate the same attribute over time [27], and invariant according to sex [15,23,27,28]. Measurement invariance is defined as the probability that an individual obtains a score, showing no change according to the group to which they belong [28]; in this sense, the scores obtained by the ERQ allow comparison over time and between men and women.
Kwon et al. (2013) note that most studies on emotion regulation have been conducted in the United States. However, they claim that each culture encourages and reinforces emotional responses differently and, therefore, they also value differently the emotional regulation strategies used to channel those responses appropriately within a given culture [29]. Liddell and Williams (2019) reinforce the idea that there are cultural differences in the use of these strategies, which could be explained by the more individualistic or more collectivist orientation of each culture [30]. Another axis that could explain the cultural differences within Latin America is the lesser gender differentiation–greater gender differentiation in their rights and in their social roles, placing Chile as a less differentiated country than, for example, Mexico, Ecuador and Argentina [31]. For example, anger is expressed more in Chile than in other Latin American countries [32]. Thus, it is necessary to investigate the psychometric behavior of the scale in each country. In this sense, to date, there is no known study that explores the properties of ERQ in the Chilean population.
Due to its association with emotional discomfort or well-being, in addition to being considered a key concept in the explanation of comorbidity present in various emotional disorders [27], the evaluation of emotional regulation strategies is key to promotion, prevention and intervention in the field of mental health. On the other hand, during the COVID-19 pandemic, emotional distress has increased in the population and, therefore, so did the need to regulate these emotions [7,33]. In a study of the general Chilean population, an increase in depression, anxiety and stress levels was observed during the first months of the pandemic, which the authors attribute to the constant tension caused by the risk of virus infection, changes in lifestyle and quality of life, social isolation, information overload in the media and panic about shortages, among other stressors [33]. In another study with a Chilean university population, it was observed that virtual classes increased stress levels in students, caused by poor internet signal quality, inadequate spaces to connect to classes, with many distractions and a long connection time, among other stressors [7]. This shows the need to have a valid and reliable version of this instrument for use in the adult Chilean population, even more so in a period characterized by the threat and changes in lifestyles caused by the COVID-19 pandemic.
Thus, the aim of this study is to evaluate the psychometric properties of the ERQ in the Chilean adult population. We hypothesize (1) a two-dimensional factorial structure of the ERQ scale; (2) the presence of factorial invariance between women and men; (3) an adequate reliability both in its internal consistency and temporal stability; (4) the existence of convergent validity with depressive symptoms, PTSS, PTG and general well-being; (5) and the existence of predictive validity with PTSS and PTG measured six months after a first assessment in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Additionally, gender and age differences are analyzed.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

One thousand five hundred and forty-three adults participated in this study, a first group of 887 people from the general population, a second group of 147 people who had suffered a stressful event in the last three months, and a third group of 509 people exposed to the changes generated by the COVID-19 health alert, all residents of Chile. In total, 990 were women (64.2%) and 553 were men (35.8%); in the first group, 59.5% were women and 40.5% were men; in the second group, 79.6% were women and 20.4% were men; in the third group, 67.8% were women and 32.2% were men. The overall mean age was 28.82 years (SD = 11.24) ranging from 18 to 87 years. The mean age of the first group was 31.93 years (SD = 12.36); in the second group, it was 32.95 years (SD = 12.22); in the third group, it was 22.20 years (SD = 2.87).
Regarding the stressful events to which the participants in second group were exposed, 30.6% were due to social, police, criminal or domestic violence; 19.8% were due to family or partner crisis; 13.6% were due to their own serious illness or that of someone close; 8.2% were due to a work, traffic, or domestic accident; 8.2% were due to the death of someone close to them; and 6.1% were due to work problems.

2.2. Instruments

Emotional regulation. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) [5] was used. The Spanish translation by Rodríguez-Carvajal et al. (2006) [19] was used, after evaluating its idiomatic relevance, due to its previous use in other research with the Chilean population [6,7,34]. It is composed of ten items divided into two factors: cognitive reappraisal with six items and expressive suppression with four items. It is answered on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (total disagreement) to 7 (total agreement). The psychometric properties in this study are shown in the Results section.
General well-being. The Mental Health Continuum-Short Form (MHC-14) [35] adapted to Spanish by Aragonés et al. (2011) was used [36]. It measures hedonic well-being related to the experience of pleasure and emotions, as well as eudaimonic well-being related to psychological development and personal growth. Together, both measurements establish a general measurement of well-being [37]. The MHC-14 is composed of 14 items that are answered on a 6-point Likert scale, from 1 (never) to 6 (every day). In the present study, the instrument obtained an internal consistency of α = 0.88.
Posttraumatic stress symptoms. The SPRINT-E scale [38] was used, translated and validated for Chilean population by Leiva-Bianchi and Gallardo (2013) [39]. It has 12 questions that are answered on a Likert scale from 0 (nothing) to 3 (a lot). The total score was used in the present study. The instrument obtained an internal consistency of α = 0.91.
Posttraumatic growth. The Posttraumatic Growth Inventory, short form (PTGI-SF) [40], in the version translated and validated for the Chilean population by García and Wlodarczyk (2016) was used [41]. It is composed of 10 items that are answered on a 6-point Likert scale, from 0 (no change) to 5 (a very important change). In the present study, the total score was used, and an adequate internal consistency was obtained at α = 0.90.
Depressive symptoms. The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) [42] translated and validated for Chilean population by Gempp et al. (2004) [43] was used. It is a self-report composed of 20 items that evaluate symptoms associated with depression and in which participants are asked to indicate the frequency at which they experienced each symptom in the most recent week using a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (“rarely or never”) to 3 (“most of the time”). In the present study, the total score was used, and the instrument obtained a high reliability (α = 0.85).

2.3. Procedure

Based on the Spanish translation of the ERQ proposed by Rodríguez-Carvajal et al. (2006) [19], the questionnaire was revised following the suggestions of Larrain et al. (2017) [44] for the adaptation of scales. Thus, two psychologist specialists were required to review the items according to three criteria: (a) maintenance of the meaning and intentionality of the statements, (b) use of language appropriate to the context and characteristics of the Chilean population, and (c) review of formal aspects of the instrument. Then, a pilot application was offered to 15 people from the general population: eight men and seven women, with complete primary education, using a saturation criterion. From these reviews, it was assessed that no changes were needed to be made to either the items or the instructions. The scale used can be reviewed in Annex S1 (Supplementary Materials).
Participants were selected using three methods. The first group was selected by sampling on successive occasions with a panel sample design. An initial sample of 1198 people out of a total of 1,105,658 inhabitants of the Province of Concepción (Chile), according to the 2017 census data available, were randomly selected and contacted by telephone; of these, 887 people (74.04%) answered the calls and agreed to participate in the study, and were therefore included in the final sample. The scales were applied by previously trained psychologists and psychology students and were answered face-to-face with pencil and paper. Data collection was conducted from August to November 2018. A total of 4% of the data were lost due to incomplete surveys.
The second group of participants was selected through a general call, through the universities collaborating with the project, from people who had faced a stressful event from a list of previously formulated events, including being a victim of violence, having suffered an accident, and having been exposed to a natural disaster, among others. The event had to have occurred within the last three months and the incentive was the offer of free psychotherapeutic care. Of the 149 people who responded to the call, two were excluded because they were already receiving psychological support for the same event, so 147 people participated. The scores presented in this study were those obtained in the first evaluation, before the interventions. The scales were applied by previously trained psychologists and were answered face-to-face with pencil and paper. Data collection was conducted from March to October 2019. There was no loss of data.
The third group answered a self-administered online questionnaire that was applied to higher education students residing in Chile. Recruitment was conducted through social networks and not in the usual study spaces, since the universities were closed due to the COVID-19 pandemic, so it was a purposive sampling by accessibility. Participants were provided with an access link to a Google form, configured to move to the next item as long as all previous items were answered, so there was no loss of data. In this first measurement, the participants were assigned a code associated with their contact data (telephone and email); they were then contacted six months later so that the same persons could participate in the second assessment. The relationship between the personal code and the contact data was kept confidential by the principal investigator. A total of 509 people were surveyed in October and November 2020 in the first measurement and 502 in April and May 2021 in the second measurement, six months later, as there were seven dropouts. All surveys were answered after signing or accepting an informed consent form in which confidentiality, protection of the data provided, and voluntary participation were guaranteed.
The whole study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Concepción, in its resolution CEBB 1150-2022.

2.4. Data Analysis

The database was prepared using the Bayesian method for the treatment and replacement of lost values. A multivariate normality analysis was performed through the Mardia coefficient [45]. Normality was confirmed when the values are within the range of ±5 [46]; it is considered appropriate to use the Maximum Likelihood estimation method when the values do not exceed the range of ±70 [47].
To assess the goodness of fit of the two-factor model, a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) was performed. The χ2, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) with its confidence interval, and the Standardized Root mean square (SRMR) were used as fit indices. The criteria for an adequate fit were χ2 with a p > 0.05, the CFI and TLI greater than 0.90, the RMSEA less than 0.08 with a confidence interval not exceeding 0.10, and the SRMR less than 0.08 [48].
Considering the previous findings regarding gender differences in emotional regulation strategies [11], once the factorial structure of the scale was established, a factorial invariance test was performed comparing women and men using successive multi-sample CFA, comparing the nested models with the CFI, TLI and RMSEA adjustment indices. Decreases in the CFI of less than 0.01 (Δ ± 0.01) were considered as an adequate indicator of invariability when compared with the previous model; in addition, a CFI and TLI above 0.90 and an RMSEA below 0.08 were expected [49].
Subsequently, reliability analyses were performed through internal consistency using Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s Ω, in addition to temporal stability through Pearson’s correlation coefficient r.
For convergent validity, Pearson’s r correlation between expressive suppression, cognitive reappraisal, general well-being, depressive symptoms, PTSS and PTG was evaluated. Multiple linear regression was used to evaluate the predictive capacity of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression at T1 over PTSS and PTG at T2.
For the analyses, the SPSS v.21 [50] and the AMOS SPSS 20.0 [51] software were used.

3. Results

The Mardia coefficient obtained a value of 39.58, so the use of the Maximum Likelihood estimation method in the CFA was selected. As adjustment indices, a χ2 of 216.89 (df = 34), p < 0.001, as is usual with large samples, is obtained; however, the other indices show that the two-factor correlated model presents an appropriate fit. These indices are TLI = 0.94; CFI = 0.95; RMSEA = 0.059 (CI: 0.052–0.067); SRMR = 0.041.
Standardized factorial weights can be seen in Figure 1. It can be observed that the factorial weights on the expressive suppression scale varied between 0.58 and 0.83, and on the cognitive reappraisal scale they varied between 0.52 and 0.76. The correlation between expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal was r = 0.20, p < 0.001. The modification indices did not suggest any changes that significantly varied the parameters (Figure 1).
Factorial invariance between the samples of women and men was evaluated. To this end, the sequential evaluation of configural, metric, strong and strict invariance was carried out [52]. Configural invariance is the basic model of analysis and requires factors to be specified for the same items in both groups. The refutation of the configural invariance hypothesis implies the absence of substantial equivalence of the construct assessed between women and men. Metric invariance assesses the equality of regression coefficients. Strong invariance assesses equality in intercepts. Strict invariance assesses equality in variance and covariance of errors.
The analysis showed the existence of configural invariance between women and men, as the values of the fit indices were acceptable. Metric invariance was also observed, since the CFI decreased its value by 0.01 compared to the previous model and the TLI and RMSEA were almost unchanged; thus, it can be concluded that the factorial loads are equivalent between women and men. Strong invariance is also accepted as the fit indices are still adequate and the CFI is reduced by only 0.01, so we conclude that the two models evaluated are equivalent with respect to factor coefficients and intercepts. Finally, strict invariance shows no variation in the CFI and the other indices are still adequate, so it is also accepted (see Table 1).
We proceeded to evaluate the internal consistency of the factors. The expressive suppression subscale obtained a α Cronbach of 0.76, an Ω of 0.76, and the corrected item-total correlation ranged from 0.50 to 0.66. The cognitive reappraisal subscale obtained an α of 0.82, an Ω of 0.82, with an item-total correlation corrected from 0.46 to 0.67. Regarding the temporal stability of the factors, it is observed that the test–retest correlation in cognitive reappraisal, r = 0.69, p < 0.001, and expressive suppression, r = 0.70, p < 0.001, are satisfactory (see Table 2).
The descriptive statistics of the scales used in the study are presented in Table 2. Skewness and kurtosis indicate the existence of univariate normality in all variables. Through repeated measure ANOVA, it is observed that there are no significant differences in cognitive reappraisal, expressive suppression and PTSS scores between T1 and T2. In contrast, PTG scores at T2 are significantly higher than at T1, F (1, 501) = 12.481, p < 0.001.
To evaluate convergent validity, the bivariate correlations of cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression with the other variables measured at T1 were calculated, which is observed in Table 3. Cognitive reappraisal at T1 correlates directly and significantly with general well-being and PTG at T1, and inversely and significantly with depressive and post-traumatic symptoms at T1. In turn, expressive suppression presents a direct and significant correlation with PTSS at T1, but there is no significant relationship with depressive symptoms, general well-being and PTG at T1. It is also observed that the cognitive reappraisal at T1 shows a direct and significant relationship with PTG at T2 and inverse with PTSS at T2. The expressive suppression at T1 shows a direct and significant relationship with PTSS at T2; it shows no significant relationship with PTG at T2 (see Table 3).
In order to evaluate the predictive validity of the emotional regulation strategy, two MLRs were then performed. In each of them, the two types of emotional regulation strategies were included as predictor variables, controlling in a first step the baseline of the predicted criterion variable, either PTSS or PTG at T1. Table 4 shows that expressive suppression directly predicts and cognitive reappraisal inversely predicts PTSS at T2 (F = 183.17; p < 0.001; R2 = 0.53). In turn, Table 5 shows that expressive suppression inversely predicts and cognitive reappraisal directly predicts PTG at T2 (F = 203.66; p < 0.001; R2 = 0.52) (see Table 4).
Further analyses assessed whether there was a relationship between regulation strategies with sex, age and participant group. Regarding sex, it is observed that men (M = 16.42; SD = 5.67) use expressive suppression more than women (M = 14.89; SD = 5.79), t(1535) = −4.970, p < 0.001; there are no differences in cognitive reappraisal. In relation to age, a positive correlation is obtained with expressive suppression (r = 0.15; p < 0.001) and with cognitive reappraisal (r = 0.13; p < 0.001), that is, the older the age, the more frequent the use of these strategies. Finally, in relation to the group of participants, it is observed that the general population group (M = 31.28; SD = 7.01) uses the cognitive reappraisal more than the group exposed to a recent stressful event (M = 28.94; SD = 7.84) or than the group exposed to the changes generated by COVID-19 (M = 28.97; SD = 6.47) (see Table 5).

4. Discussion

Factor analysis confirmed the existence of two correlated factors detected in most of the studies that were analyzed from ERQ: expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. The items have positive factorial loads and values higher than 0.52 with each of the dimensions, which is appropriate considering that in psychology factorial loads, the values tend to oscillate around 0.50 [53]. The factorial invariance observed between men and women suggests that the instrument would be free of gender biases, which would allow the comparison of scores between men and women.
Regarding the reliability of the subscales, the internal consistency indices through Cronbach’s α and McDonald’s Ω are adequate according to the literature [54]. Temporal stability was measured six months after the first evaluation, as suggested by Cohen and Swerdlik (2010); in both subscales, the values obtained were significant and positive [55].
Emotional regulation strategies are used to address negative emotions that arise after exposure to stressful events [56]. Expressive suppression is a cognitive avoidance strategy that inhibits the ability to deploy more adaptive resources. Thus, a direct relationship with depressive and post-traumatic symptoms was observed, and inverse relationship was observed with general well-being and PTG. These results coincide with other studies that show the relationship between suppression, depression and hopelessness [57], and it has been shown that their regular use can have a negative impact on mental health [5,32]. It is likely that expressive suppression, by preventing verbal communication about emotions, limits the search for affective support and helps produce feelings of hopelessness [58].
On the other hand, cognitive reappraisal is an emotional regulation strategy that consists of changing the assessment or meaning of the stressful event, which decreases discomfort. Coincidentally, a direct relationship with general well-being and PTG and inverse relationship with depressive and post-traumatic symptoms is observed. In previous studies, a direct relationship with psychological well-being [59] and PTG [60] was observed.
We observed an absence of significant relationship of expressive suppression with general well-being at T1, which contradicts some previous findings [5] but coincides with the findings of Garabito et al. (2020) in a Chilean population [6]. The lack of association between emotional suppression and depressive symptoms is also inconsistent with previous findings [5,6]. It is possible that this lack of association is due to cultural issues such as, for example, a formal education that encourages reading that privileges the cognitive over the emotional and teaches to postpone any current negative effect for the promise of future happiness [61].
The ERQ showed the ability to longitudinally predict growth and PTSS associated with lifestyle changes during the COVID-19 pandemic. Expressive suppression directly predicted PTSS and inversely predicted PTG. Other cross-sectional studies during the pandemic also showed a positive relationship of suppression with emotional distress [12]; the present study reinforces this idea by finding the same relationship in a longitudinal assessment. On the other hand, the cognitive reappraisal showed direct prediction of PTG and inverse prediction of PTSS. In another study conducted during the pandemic, reappraisal was associated with a lower level of anxiety in confined people [62]. In this sense, it is possible to consider that the pandemic has generated intense negative emotions in a significant part of the population due to confinement, changes in lifestyle and the direct threat to physical health. Apparently, expressing these emotions instead of repressing them and considering a more acceptable meaning for the changes generated by the pandemic could fulfill a potentially protective function in the face of COVID-19.
The present study also discovered gender differences, where men showed greater use of expressive suppression compared to women. This result can be explained by the process of socialization and the cultural norms transmitted from childhood that seek to establish social gender roles, which lead boys to suppress or inhibit their emotional expression [63,64] which, according to Walker et al. (2011) [65], would be influenced by the environment, culture, parenting styles and sexual stereotypes to which children are exposed from their proximal environment, as it happens in Chile [66]. A study carried out in Chile by Espinoza and Silva (2014) warned that young Chilean men have been socialized in a cultural system that favors the reproduction of a hegemonic model of masculinity based on a constant comparison and competition between emotional endurance and physical strength [67]. In another study conducted in Chile, it was observed that the formal education of boys and girls excludes readings that promote emotions such as sadness and anger, which could convey the idea that the expression of these emotions is not allowed [61].
In addition to the previous points, it is observed that the older the age, the greater the use of emotional suppression and cognitive reappraisal. In this regard, Urry and Gross (2010) report that as age increases, the emotional regulation strategies that are used around the relationship with others improve [68]. In addition, at an older age, a greater willingness to attend and remember the positive stimuli over the negative ones is observed [69]. Phillips et al. (2006), for their part, add that the most efficient use of these emotional regulation strategies is due to a selective dampening of negative emotions such as anger, a better adaptation of strategies to situations and a more frequent use of positive reevaluation, even in cases experienced as a negative situation [70].
A limitation of this study is the diversity of populations included in the analyses, which in turn occupied different sampling methods, which could alter the overall results presented in this article. For example, only one subsample was evaluated longitudinally, allowing for temporal stability and predictive validity analyses; however, this was an unintentional unrepresentative sample so the generalization of these findings should be taken with caution. The gender disproportion in this study, in which more women than men answered the surveys, is also a limitation. This also affects the generalizability of the results. A third limitation is the absence of data to control some confounding factors for the interpretation of the findings, such as the presence of mental disorders, neurological conditions, and possible infection with COVID-19, among others.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the ERQ showed a two-dimensional factorial structure, factorial invariance between men and women, adequate internal consistency and temporal stability, convergent validity with depressive symptoms, PTSS, PTG and general well-being, and predictive validity of PTSD and PTG six months after exposure to a stressful event such as the COVID-19 pandemic. In addition, men showed greater use of expressive suppression than women and older participants showed greater use of both strategies than younger ones. These findings contribute to the understanding of the phenomenon of emotional regulation in general and in the Chilean population in particular; for example, the verification of its predictive capacity of mental health consequences would allow its use as a screening instrument to detect more vulnerable people after exposure to a stressful event. The adequate psychometric performance of the scale allows us to conclude that it can continue to be used to evaluate emotional regulation strategies in people exposed to a highly stressful event in this culture.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph20043452/s1, ANNEX S1: EMOTION REGULATION QUESTIONNAIRE (ERQ).

Author Contributions

F.E.G. proposed the research idea, directed the study, wrote the article, and performed the data analysis and the final review. P.V.-B. and P.C.-P. wrote the article and participated in the final review. M.A. contributed to data analysis and wrote the article and final review. P.R. and M.V.-D. participated in the methodology and wrote the final review. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the FONDECYT/ANID under Grant number 1220231 for Felipe E. García; FONDECYT/ANID under Grant number 11221092 for Mariela Andrades; and FONDECYT/ANID under Grant number 1210489 for Mauricio Valdivia-Devia. The objective of ANID was to stimulate and promote the development of basic scientific and technological research.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Concepción, in its resolution CEBB 1150-2022. Locally, the correct treatment of the information was sought for the protection of personal data, established in Chilean law 19.628 of 1999, and the protection of confidentiality and statistical secrecy.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

  1. Thompson, R.A. Emotion Regulation: A Theme in Search of Definition. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1994, 59, 25–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Barnhofer, T.; Crane, C.; Brennan, K.; Duggan, D.S.; Crane, R.S.; Eames, C.; Radford, S.; Silverton, S.; Fennell, M.J.V.; Williams, J.M.G. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) reduces the association between depressive symptoms and suicidal cognitions in patients with a history of suicidal depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2015, 83, 1013–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Compare, A.; Zarbo, C.; Shonin, E.; Van Gordon, W.; Marconi, C. Emotional Regulation and Depression: A Potential Mediator between Heart and Mind. Cardiovasc. Psychiatry Neurol. 2014, 2014, 324374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Gross, J.J. The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1998, 2, 271–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Gross, J.J.; John, O.P. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 348–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. García, F.E.; Garabito, S.; Neira, M.; Puentes, E. Ruptura de pareja en adultos jóvenes y salud mental: Estrategias de afrontamiento ante el estrés del término de una relación [Couple breakup in young adults and mental health: Coping strategies in the face of stress at the end of a relationship]. Psychol. Av. Discip. 2020, 14, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. García, F.E.; Andrades, M.; Rodríguez, F.; Díaz, Z.; Alarcón, S.; Fonseca, I.; Lucero, C. Sintomatología y crecimiento postraumático en estudiantes de educación superior en Chile durante la pandemia por COVID-19 [Post-traumatic symptoms and posttraumatic growth in higher education students in Chile during the COVID-19 pandemic]. Actual. Psicol. 2022, 36, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bustos, C. Cuando se Acaba el Amor: Estrategias de Afrontamiento, Duelo por Pérdidas Amorosas y Crecimiento Postraumático en Estudiantes Universitarios. Tesis de Pregrado, Universidad San Francisco, Quito, Colombia. 2011. Available online: http://repositorio.usfq.edu.ec/handle/23000/604 (accessed on 5 July 2022).
  9. Moore, S.A.; Zoellner, L.A.; Mollenholt, N. Are expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms? Behav. Res. Ther. 2008, 46, 993–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Zoellner, T.; Maercker, A. Posttraumatic growth in clinical psychology—A critical review and introduction of a two component model. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 26, 626–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zhang, L.; Lu, J.; Li, B.; Wang, X.; Shangguan, C. Gender differences in the mediating effects of emotion-regulation strategies: Forgiveness and depression among adolescents. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2020, 163, 110094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bramanti, S.M.; Trumello, C.; Lombardi, L.; Babore, A. COVID-19 and chronic disease patients: Perceived stress, worry, and emotional regulation strategies. Rehabil. Psychol. 2021, 66, 380–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Balzarotti, S.; John, O.P.; Gross, J.J. An Italian Adaptation of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2010, 26, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Melka, S.E.; Lancaster, S.L.; Bryant, A.R.; Rodriguez, B.F. Confirmatory factor and measurement invariance analyses of the emotion regulation questionnaire. J. Clin. Psychol. 2011, 67, 1283–1293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Gómez-Ortiz, O.; Romera, E.M.; Ortega-Ruiz, R.; Cabello, R.; Fernández-Berrocal, P. Analysis of Emotion Regulation in Spanish Adolescents: Validation of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 2016, 6, 1959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Masumoto, K.; Taishi, N.; Shiozaki, M. Age and Gender Differences in Relationships Among Emotion Regulation, Mood, and Mental Health. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 2016, 2, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Preece, D.A.; Becerra, R.; Robinson, K.; Gross, J.J. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric Properties in General Community Samples. J. Pers. Assess. 2020, 102, 348–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Pérez-Sánchez, J.; Prieto, G.; De Salamanca, U. Propiedades Psicométricas de las Puntuaciones de los Test Más Empleados en la Evaluación de la Regulación Emocional. Pap. Psicólogo 2020, 41, 116–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Stanford Psychophysiology Laboratory. Berkeley Expressivity Questionnaire. Available online: https://spl.stanford.edu/sites/g/files/sbiybj19321/files/media/file/spanish.pdf (accessed on 3 June 2018).
  20. Gargurevich, R.; Matos, L. Propiedades psicométricas del Cuestionario de Autorregulación Emocional adaptado para el Perú. Rev. Psicol. 2010, 12, 192–215. [Google Scholar]
  21. Cabello, R.; Salguero, J.M.; Fernández-Berrocal, P.; Gross, J.J. A Spanish Adaptation of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 29, 234–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Navarro, J.; Vara, M.D.; Cebolla, A.; Baños, R.M. Validación psicométrica del cuestionario de regulación emocional (ERQ-CA) en población adolescente española. Rev. Psicol. Clínica Niños Adolesc. 2018, 5, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Moreta, R.; Dominguez, S.; Sánchez, S.; López, J.; Molina, M. Análisis multigrupo por sexo y fiabilidad del Cuestionario de Regulación Emocional (ERQ) en jóvenes ecuatorianos. Avaliaçao Psicol. Interam. J. Psychol. Assess. 2021, 20, 220–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Moreta-Herrera, R.; Perdomo-Pérez, M.; Vaca-Quintana, D.; Sánchez-Vélez, H.; Camacho-Bonilla, P.; de la Bandera, F.V.; Dominguez-Lara, S.; Caycho-Rodríguez, T. New Psychometric Evidence of a Bifactor Structure of the Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) in Ecuadorian College Students. Psychol. Russ. State Art 2022, 15, 120–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Olalde-Mathieu, V.E.; Licea-Haquet, G.; Reyes-Aguilar, A.; Barrios, F.A. Psychometric properties of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire in a Mexican sample and their correlation with empathy and alexithymia. Cogent Psychol. 2022, 9, 2053385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Delvalle, M.V.; Andrés, M.L.; Urquijo, S.; Zamora, E.V.; Mehta, A.; Gross, J.J. Argentinean Adaptation and Psychometric Properties of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ). Psychol. Rep. 2022, 125, 2733–2759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Pineda, D.; Valiente, R.M.; Chorot, P.; Piqueras, J.A.; Sandín, B. Invarianza factorial y temporal del Cuestionario de Regulación Emocional (ERQ). Rev. Psicopatología Psicol. Clínica 2018, 23, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Pinto, A.L.D.C.B.; Pasian, S.R.; Malloy-Diniz, L.F. Gender invariance and psychometric properties of a Brazilian version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ). Trends Psychiatry Psychother. 2021, 43, 92–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Kwon, H.; Yoon, K.L.; Joormann, J.; Kwon, J.-H. Cultural and gender differences in emotion regulation: Relation to depression. Cogn. Emot. 2013, 27, 769–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Liddell, B.J.; Williams, E.N. Cultural Differences in Interpersonal Emotion Regulation. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Zubieta, E.; Fernández, I.; Vergara, A.I.; Martínez, M.D.; Candia, L. Cultura y emoción en América [Culture and emotion in America]. Boletín Psicol. 1998, 61, 65–89. Available online: https://www.ehu.eus/documents/1463215/1504238/Zubietaetal98.pdf (accessed on 15 July 2022).
  32. Fernández, P.; Ramos, N.; Extremera, N. Inteligencia emocional, supresión crónica de pensamiento y ajuste psicológico. Boletín Psicol. 2001, 70, 79–95. Available online: https://emotional.intelligence.uma.es/documentos/PDF13supresion_cronica_de_pensamientos.pdf (accessed on 15 July 2022).
  33. Andrades-Tobar, M.; García, F.E.; Concha-Ponce, P.; Valiente, C.; Lucero, C. Predictores de síntomas de ansiedad, depresión y estrés a partir del brote epidémico de COVID-19. Rev. Psicopatol. Psicol. Clínica 2021, 26, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Cerna, C.; García, F.E.; Téllez, A. Brief mindfulness, mental health, and cognitive processes: A randomized controlled trial. PsyCh J. 2020, 9, 359–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Keyes, C.L.M.; Wissing, M.; Potgieter, J.P.; Temane, M.; Kruger, A.; van Rooy, S. Evaluation of the mental health continuum–short form (MHC–SF) in setswana-speaking South Africans. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 2008, 15, 181–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Aragonés, J.; Olivos, P.; Lima, M. Conectividad con la naturaleza y bienestar hedónico y eudaimónico. In Proceedings of the XI Congreso de Psicología Ambiental, Almería, Spain, 9–11 February 2011. [Google Scholar]
  37. Petrillo, G.; Capone, V.; Caso, D.; Keyes, C.L.M. The Mental Health Continuum–Short Form (MHC–SF) as a Measure of Well-Being in the Italian Context. Soc. Indic. Res. 2015, 121, 291–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Connor, K.M.; Davidson, J.R.T. SPRINT: A brief global assessment of post-traumatic stress disorder. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2001, 16, 279–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Leiva-Bianchi, M.; Gallardo, I. Validation of the short posttraumatic stress disorder rating interview (SPRINT-E) in a sample of people affected by F-27 Chilean earthquake and tsunami. An. Psicol. 2013, 29, 328–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Cann, A.; Calhoun, L.G.; Tedeschi, R.G.; Taku, K.; Vishnevsky, T.; Triplett, K.N.; Danhauer, S.C. A short form of the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory. Anxiety Stress. Coping 2010, 23, 127–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. García, F.E.; Wlodarczyk, A. Psychometric Properties of the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory–Short Form Among Chilean Adults. J. Loss Trauma 2016, 21, 303–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Radloff, L.S. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1977, 1, 385–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gempp, R.; Avendaño, C.; Muñoz, C. Normas y punto de corte para la Escala de Depresión del Centro para Estudios Epidemiológicos (CES-D) en población juvenil chilena. Ter. Psicológica 2004, 22, 145–156. [Google Scholar]
  44. Larrain, M.E.; Andes, U.D.L.; Zegers, B.; Orellana, Y. Traducción y Adaptación de una Escala Para Evaluar Generatividad en Abuelos en Santiago de Chile. Psykhe 2017, 26, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Mardia, K.V. Applications of some measures of multivariate skewness and kurtosis in testing normality and robustness studies. Sankhya Indian J. Stat. 1974, 36, 115–128. [Google Scholar]
  46. Bentler, P. EQS 6 Structural Equations Program Manual; Multivariate Software: Encino, CA, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  47. Rodríguez-Ayán, M.N.; Ruiz Diaz, M.Á. Atenuación de la asimetría y de la curtosis de las puntuaciones observadas mediante transformaciones de variables: Incidencia sobre la estructura factorial. Psicológica Rev. Metodol. Psicol. Exp. 2008, 29, 205–228. Available online: http://www.uv.es/revispsi/articulos2.08/6RODRIGUEZ.pdf (accessed on 20 July 2022).
  48. Hu, L.T.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cheung, G.W.; Rensvold, R.B. Evaluating Goodness-of-Fit Indexes for Testing Measurement Invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2002, 9, 233–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 21.0; IBM Corp.: Armonk, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  51. Arbuckle, J.L. Amos (Version 20.0) [Computer Program]; IBM SPSS: Chicago, IL, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  52. Elosua, P. Evaluación progresiva de la invarianza factorial entre las versiones original y adaptada de un cuestionario sobre autoconcepto. Psicothema 2005, 17, 356–362. [Google Scholar]
  53. Beauducel, A.; Herzberg, P.Y. On the Performance of Maximum Likelihood Versus Means and Variance Adjusted Weighted Least Squares Estimation in CFA. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2006, 13, 186–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Dunn, T.J.; Baguley, T.; Brunsden, V. From alpha to omega: A practical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency estimation. Br. J. Psychol. 2014, 105, 399–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Cohen, J.R.; Swerdlik, M.E. Psychological Testing and Assessment: An Introduction to Tests and Measurement, 7th ed.; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  56. Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation: Taking stock and moving forward. Emotion 2013, 13, 359–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Fernández, P.; Extremera, N.; Ramos, N. Inteligencia emocional y depresión. Encuentros Psicol. Soc. 2003, 1, 251–254. [Google Scholar]
  58. Campos, M.; Iraurgui, J.; Páez, D.; Velasco, C. Afrontamiento y regulación emocional de hechos estresantes: Un meta-análisis de 13 estudios. Boletín Psicol. 2004, 82, 25–44. [Google Scholar]
  59. King, R.B.; Rosa, E.D.D. Are your emotions under your control or not? Implicit theories of emotion predict well-being via cognitive reappraisal. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019, 138, 177–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Orejuela-Dávila, A.I.; Levens, S.M.; Sagui-Henson, S.J.; Tedeschi, R.G.; Sheppes, G. The relation between emotion regulation choice and posttraumatic growth. Cogn. Emot. 2019, 33, 1709–1717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Errázuriz, V.; García-González, M. “More person, and, therefore, more satisfied and happy”: The affective economy of reading promotion in Chile. Curric. Inq. 2021, 51, 229–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Xu, C.; Xu, Y.; Xu, S.; Zhang, Q.; Liu, X.; Shao, Y.; Xu, X.; Peng, L.; Li, M. Cognitive Reappraisal and the Association Between Perceived Stress and Anxiety Symptoms in COVID-19 Isolated People. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Brody, L.R.; Hall, J.A. Gender, Emotion, and Socialization. In Handbook of Gender Research in Psychology, 1st ed.; Chriler, J., McCreary, D., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010; pp. 429–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Chaplin, T.M. Gender and Emotion Expression: A Developmental Contextual Perspective. Emot. Rev. J. Int. Soc. Res. Emot. 2015, 7, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Walker, S.P.; Wachs, T.D.; Grantham-McGregor, S.; Black, M.M.; Nelson, C.A.; Huffman, S.L.; Baker-Henningham, H.; Chang, S.M.; Hamadani, J.D.; Lozoff, B.; et al. Inequality in early childhood: Risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet 2011, 378, 1325–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Chile Crece Contigo. Available online: https://www.crececontigo.gob.cl/wp-content/uploads/2015/11/13-INFORME-FINAL_Evaluacion-PADB.pdf (accessed on 26 November 2022).
  67. Espinoza-Tapia, R.E.; Segovia, J.L.S.S. Emociones, corporeidad y socialización de género en la subjetivación de la masculinidad de jóvenes chilenos: Una aproximación intertextual desde el modelo de mapas corporales. Salud Soc. 2014, 5, 300–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Urry, H.L.; Gross, J.J. Emotion regulation in older age. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2010, 19, 352–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Martins, B.; Sheppes, G.; Gross, J.J.; Mather, M. Age differences in emotion regulation choice: Older adults use distraction less than younger adults in high-intensity positive contexts. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2018, 73, 603–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Phillips, L.H.; Henry, J.D.; Hosie, J.A.; Milne, A.B. Age, anger regulation and well-being. Aging Ment. Health 2006, 10, 250–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Model of two correlated factors; the values are standardized.
Figure 1. Model of two correlated factors; the values are standardized.
Ijerph 20 03452 g001
Table 1. Factorial invariance models between women (n = 990) and men (n = 553).
Table 1. Factorial invariance models between women (n = 990) and men (n = 553).
Models: χ2 Δχ2 CFI ΔCFI TLI RMSEA
M1: Configural 2087.59 0.96 0.94 0.045
M2: Metric 2181.08 −93.49 0.95 −0.01 0.94 0.043
M3: Strong 2735.50 −743.52 0.94 −0.01 0.94 0.045
M4: Strict 2924.60 −5660.10 0.94 0.00 0.94 0.044
M1 = not constrained; M2 = M1 + invariant factor loadings; M3 = M2 + invariant intercepts; M4 = M3 + invariant error variances and covariances. Note: CFI comparative fit index, TLI Tucker–Lewis index, RMSEA root mean square error of approximation.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the study variables.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the study variables.
Variable Min Max M SD Skewness Kurtosis
General well-being T1 22 84 62.52 11.31 −0.54 0.15
Depressive symptoms T1 4 57 32.92 11.28 −0.19 −0.34
Posttraumatic stress symptoms T1 5 48 26.66 8.06 0.21 −0.44
Posttraumatic growth T1 0 60 28.72 12.69 0.33 −0.60
ERQ—reappraisal T1 6 42 30.29 7.01 −0.64 0.36
ERQ—suppression T1 4 28 15.44 5.79 −0.01 −0.62
Posttraumatic stress symptoms T2 12 47 28.06 8.19 0.10 −0.65
Posttraumatic growth T2 10 60 32.13 11.93 0.15 −0.85
ERQ—reappraisal T2 6 42 28.90 6.49 −0.67 0.93
ERQ—suppression T2 4 28 15.34 5.66 −0.06 −0.76
ERQ = Emotion regulation questionnaire.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations between emotional regulation strategies and other variables.
Table 3. Bivariate correlations between emotional regulation strategies and other variables.
Variable ERQ Reappraisal ERQ Suppression
ERQ—reappraisal T2 0.69 *** 0.01
ERQ—suppression T2 −0.02 0.70 ***
General well-being T1 0.21 *** 0.01
Depressive symptoms T1 −0.26 ** 0.11
Posttraumatic stress symptoms T1 −0.20 *** 0.26 ***
Posttraumatic growth T1 0.31 *** −0.06
Posttraumatic stress symptoms T2 −0.22 *** 0.25 ***
Posttraumatic growth T2 0.23 *** −0.14 ***
** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Multiple linear regression on posttraumatic stress symptoms in T2 (n = 509).
Table 4. Multiple linear regression on posttraumatic stress symptoms in T2 (n = 509).
Model Variable Unstandardized
Coefficients
Typified
Coefficients
t-Value p-Value
B SE β
1 (Constant) 8.366 0.932 8.979 <0.001
Posttraumatic stress symptoms 0.708 0.032 0.70 22.019 <0.001
2 (Constant) 11.735 1.641 7.151 <0.001
Posttraumatic stress symptoms 0.652 0.033 0.65 19.959 <0.001
ERQ—reappraisal −0.166 0.040 −0.13 −4.184 <0.001
ERQ—suppression 0.194 0.046 0.13 4.212 <0.000
Table 5. Multiple linear regression on posttraumatic growth in T2 (n = 509).
Table 5. Multiple linear regression on posttraumatic growth in T2 (n = 509).
Model Variable Unstandardized
Coefficients
Typified
Coefficients
t-Value p-Value
B SE β
1 (Constant) 11.153 0.980 11.378 <0.001
Posttraumatic growth 0.684 0.030 0.72 23.111 <0.001
2 (Constant) 9.092 1.984 4.582 <0.001
Posttraumatic growth 0.631 0.030 0.66 21.028 <0.001
ERQ—reappraisal 0.268 0.058 0.15 4.875 <0.001
ERQ—suppression −0.267 0.064 −0.13 −4.186 <0.000
ERQ = Emotion regulation questionnaire.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

García, F.E.; Vergara-Barra, P.; Concha-Ponce, P.; Andrades, M.; Rincón, P.; Valdivia-Devia, M. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric Properties and Prediction of Posttraumatic Consequences during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Chilean Adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 3452. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043452

AMA Style

García FE, Vergara-Barra P, Concha-Ponce P, Andrades M, Rincón P, Valdivia-Devia M. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric Properties and Prediction of Posttraumatic Consequences during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Chilean Adults. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(4):3452. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043452

Chicago/Turabian Style

García, Felipe E., Pablo Vergara-Barra, Pablo Concha-Ponce, Mariela Andrades, Paulina Rincón, and Mauricio Valdivia-Devia. 2023. "The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire: Psychometric Properties and Prediction of Posttraumatic Consequences during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Chilean Adults" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 4: 3452. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043452

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop