How to translate text using browser tools
1 September 2003 COMPARATIVE TESTS OF EVOLUTIONARY TRADE-OFFS IN A PALINURID LOBSTER ACOUSTIC SYSTEM
S. N. Patek, T. H. Oakley
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Communication structures vary greatly in size and can be structurally and behaviorally integrated with other systems. In structurally integrated systems, dramatic changes in size may impose trade-offs with the size of neighboring structures. In spiny lobsters (Palinuridae), there is a fivefold difference in size of the antennular plate, on which sound producing apparatus is located, such that the antennular plate reaches 38% carapace length in some sound producers (Stridentes) compared to only 4% carapace length in non-sound producing spiny lobsters (Silentes). We examined whether this major variation in antennular plate size imposes trade-offs with the adjoining antennae, specifically in the context that the signal producing structures and antennae are both used in predator defense. We recorded and analyzed lobster sounds in order to test whether size increases in the acoustic morphology were correlated with production of particular signal features. Antennal and antennular plate structures were measured across the family, including both Stridentes and Silentes. Phylogenetic comparative methods were used to test for correlated evolutionary change among the structures and signal features. We analyzed the phylogenetic relationships of the Palinuridae based on morphological characters and ribosomal DNA evidence (16S, 18S and 28S nuclear and mitochondrial ribosomal RNA gene regions). We found that the number of sound pulses was positively correlated with length of the sound producing apparatus. Opposite to the predicted trade-offs, we found that the size of the antennular plate was positively correlated with size of the surrounding antennae within Stridentes. Nevertheless, when Stridentes were compared to Silentes, the latter had relatively larger antennae for a given antennular plate size than did the sound producing taxa. These results suggest that body size does not limit size increases in acoustic structures within Stridentes, however the presence and associated constructional costs of a sound producing apparatus may impose a trade-off when taxa with and without the apparatus are compared. Alternatively, since both systems are used in predator defense, this pattern may indicate greater selection for antennal force production in Silentes, which lack the additional acoustic mode of predator defense.

In comparison to the extensive body of research about the evolutionary morphology of feeding and locomotion (e.g., Wainwright and Reilly 1994), the influence of morphology on the evolutionary variation of communication signals is relatively unexplored. However, this shortage of information about the evolution of signal producing morphology does not reflect a lack of importance of morphology to communication systems. For example, several studies (Cocroft and Ryan 1995; Podos 1997, 2001; Fitch 1999; Mahler and Tubaro 2001) provide solid evidence that signal producing structures play an important evolutionary role in acoustic communication systems. Because signal production typically employs specialized structures, variation in signal features often requires modification in these structures. Hence, limits on the variation of signal producing structures can be of central consequence to the evolutionary diversification of communication systems. Furthermore, because signal producing structures do not function in isolation (Nowicki and Podos 1993), interesting trade-offs often characterize their evolution.

Size is an essential, and costly, component of performance in communication structures as diverse as avian and anuran vocal systems, bird feathers, and fish tails (Ryan and Brenowitz 1985; Ryan 1986; Cocroft and Ryan 1995; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998). Evolutionary variation in the size of communication structures is important, not only for communication performance but also for the size interrelationships with other structures that are integrated with the communication system. Size variation of a communication structure can be influenced by the degree of coupling and integration of a system, both in terms of the system's internal integration (e.g., physiological and biomechanical levels of control) as well as its integration with other systems (e.g., shared functions and structures due to phylogenetic and ontogenetic coupling; Nowicki et al. 1992; Nowicki and Podos 1993). For example, in the structurally coupled bird song system, minor evolutionary size changes can have major influences on the production of particular signal features. The vocal tract of a bird resonates certain frequencies and functions as an air passageway (Nowicki 1987) whereas the beak modulates vocal tract resonances and processes food (Hoese et al. 2000; Westneat et al. 1993). In Darwin's finches, beak size is correlated with performance in processing specific food types while also correlated with frequency bandwidth and temporal rate of sound production. These shared functions have resulted in a trade-off whereby beak size limits the production of certain signal features and small size changes in the beak have major implications for the production of particular signal features (Podos 2001).

Size trade-offs among structures can be interpreted at multiple levels, including: (1) limits imposed by body size on the space available for larger structures, and (2) limits imposed by resource availability on the construction of larger structures and larger bodies. Space limits and construction costs are two different levels at which size trade-offs can be analyzed and are not mutually exclusive. Thus, the size of a structure may be limited by the available space within a given body size. At a different level of analysis, but consistent with an assumption of limited body size, is the assumption that increased size is limited by the costs and resources incurred by building larger structures. Here we provide detailed examples of these two levels of analyses and their relevance to evolutionary size variation in communication systems.

Structural interrelationships in terms of size and space have been addressed most extensively in the field of constructional morphology in which evolutionary size and shape change in one structure is correlated with size and shape changes of neighboring structures (for review, see Barel 1993; Barel et al. 1989; Lauder 1981). An underlying assumption of these studies is that a size increase in one structure is likely to impose size trade-offs with neighboring structures in order for all the structures to fit within the limited body space. In other words, it is unlikely that body size will increase to accommodate a larger structure; instead, it is more likely that structures will decrease in size to accommodate a larger neighboring structure. For example, the volume of the pharyngeal jaw apparatus varies by 55% across feeding morphs in the cichlid, Astatoreochromis alluaudi. Smits et al. (1996) demonstrated that neighboring structures reallocated space to accommodate the larger jaw apparatus. Even with this reallocation of space, there was a slight overall increase in size of the head to accommodate the remaining increased size of component parts.

In communication systems, overall body size has been shown to limit signal feature production and reception, because dominant frequencies in acoustic mating signals and hearing structures are often tightly correlated with body size (i.e., there is a minimum size at which low frequency signals can be produced and received; Ryan and Brenowitz 1985; Ryan 1986; McClelland et al. 1996, 1998). Although the acoustic signals of most taxa appear to be highly constrained by body size, several bird species have evolved elongated trachea that coil within the body cavity and thus permit production of lower formant frequencies without increasing body size (Fitch 1999).

Size trade-offs, analyzed at the level of resource costs for constructing larger structures, have been identified between adjacent structures in beetle horns and butterfly wings (Klingenberg and Nijhout 1998; Nijhout and Emlen 1998). Experimental removal of butterfly wing components caused an increase in size of the adjacent wing components (Nijhout and Emlen 1998). A similar trade-off was found within the beetle species, Onthophagus acuminatus: when populations were artificially selected for large horn size, the eyes, which are adjacent to the horns, decreased in size with increased horn size (Nijhout and Emlen 1998). In a cross-species comparison, Emlen (2001) demonstrated that the horn size of dung beetles (Onthophagus) imposed trade-offs with the size of the adjacent structures (e.g., eyes, wings) and that the degree of the trade-off depended on the distance, such that when the structures were closer together, there was a stronger negative correlation between the sizes of the two structures. The authors provide compelling evidence that these trade-offs are due to constructional costs of large structures during critical periods of development.

We introduce a new system—the spiny lobsters (Palinuridae)—in which we test whether there are size trade-offs between antennal structures used in different modes (force production and sound production) of the same behavioral function (predator deterrence); as we will discuss in the next section, both sound production and force production are expected to increase in performance with increased size. Although theories of evolutionary escalation of defenses with increased predation pressures predict overall increases in predator defenses (Vermeij 1987), and the above studies of trade-offs in communication systems suggest that trade-offs can occur between structures that share functions and resources, it is not known how the two processes combined might influence patterns of evolutionary trade-offs.

Background

Sound producing spiny lobsters (Stridentes: George and Main 1967; Parker 1883) have a large sound producing apparatus located on the antennular plate, which averages 28% carapace width and 19% carapace length and, in some species of Panulirus, reaches 38% carapace width and 27% carapace length (Figs. 1, 2). In contrast, non-sound-producing genera (Silentes: George and Main 1967; Parker 1883) lack a sound producing apparatus, but still have an antennular plate, which averages only 7% carapace width and 4% carapace length (Figs. 1, 2). Stridentes make a “rasp” sound by rubbing an extension off the base of each antenna (the plectrum) over the files located on the antennular plate below the eyes (Fig. 1) (Moulton 1957; Meyer-Rochow and Penrose 1976; Mulligan and Fischer 1977; Patek 2001a, 2002). The plectrum consists of a series of soft tissue ridges that rub over the macroscopically smooth surface of the file; “stick and slip” friction between these two surfaces results in a series of broad frequency bandwidth sound pulses called the rasp (Patek 2001b). In contrast, the Silentes have, in place of the plectrum, a joint articulation that restricts the joint to rotations with one degree of freedom (Patek 2002).

The Stridentes produce sound during interactions with predators (Lindberg 1955; Smale 1974; Meyer-Rochow and Penrose 1976; Mulligan and Fischer 1977) which most likely increases their chances of escape by causing the predator to pause momentarily (Alexander 1958; Masters 1979, 1980; Lewis and Cane 1990; Sargent 1990). Defensive signals capitalize on unexpectedness or novelty to deter predators (Edmunds 1974; Jetz et al. 2001). Predators more effectively learn to avoid dangerous prey when coupled with a conspicuous signal (Gittleman and Harvey 1980; Gamberale and Tullberg 1996; Speed 2000), and conspicuous signals can act as reliable indicators of defended prey (Sherratt 2002).

Antennae are spiny lobsters' primary weapons against predators (Lindberg 1955; Kanciruk 1980; Cobb 1981; Spanier and Zimmer-Faust 1988; Kelly et al. 1999). Spiny lobsters forcefully grasp and rake predators with the basal antennal segments; the antennae are sharp and covered with spines. Both Stridentes and Silentes have been observed in pod formations and spread their antennae as a “pincushion” of spines for protection against predators (Kanciruk 1980; Kelly et al. 1999). When in their burrows, spiny lobsters extend their antennae out of the burrows and, when walking in the open, they hold the antennae over the carapace and abdomen (Cobb 1981; Spanier and Zimmer-Faust 1988; Kelly et al. 1999).

The size of the antennae and sound producing apparatus are especially relevant in their respective roles in predator deterrence. An increase in size of the sound producing apparatus should permit production of louder, higher pulse number, or higher pulse rate signals that are more likely to startle a predator, much in the same way that bright colors, distinct color patterns, and noxious chemicals provide jarring and memorable signals to potential predators (Guilford 1990; Jetz et al. 2001; Sherratt 2002). Larger antennae are stronger and have the capacity to produce more force without structural failure (Wainwright et al. 1976; Alexander 1983). Furthermore, larger appendages allow space for greater muscle cross-sectional area which contributes to a greater capacity for generating force (although see Schenk and Wainwright 2001; Taylor 2001 for discussion of these size-muscle relationships). Hence, in both systems, increases in size are likely to increase predator deterrent performance.

A Priori Trade-Offs Hypotheses

With the extreme size range of antennular plate, up to 38% carapace width in Stridentes compared to 4% carapace width in Silentes (Fig. 1, 2), we developed the a priori hypothesis that the adjacent antennae would decrease in size to accommodate a larger antennular plate, rather than the alternative hypothesis that the overall body size would increase to accommodate a larger communication structure. We conducted three major phylogenetic comparative tests. First, we examined whether the size of the signal producing apparatus affected signal features, a necessary prerequisite for demonstrating the acoustic function of evolutionary size variation. We tested, both within and across species, whether a longer file resulted in longer signal duration, higher pulse number, or higher pulse rate. We found that file length was associated with pulse number and pulse rate, but not with rasp duration. Second, we tested the size trade-off hypothesis that the antennae would decrease in size with increasing antennular plate size, given the assumption that body size imposed an upper limit to evolutionary size variation (Table 1). The results did not suggest a trade-off, and instead showed the opposite—the sizes of both structures were positively correlated within the Stridentes and the assumption that body space imposed an upper limit to size increases was not supported. Third, we examined whether a gradeshift in antennal size occurred between Stridentes and Silentes due to the presence of the large sound producing apparatus. This gradeshift hypothesis was generally supported: when the effects of body size were removed, Silentes had larger antennae for a given antennular plate size than Stridentes.

Materials and Methods

Palinurid Phylogeny

To conduct a phylogenetic comparative study, it was first necessary to generate phylogenetic hypotheses for the relationships of palinurid taxa. Palinurid taxonomy is well described, with some contention as to the species level designations (e.g., Sarver et al. 1998). However, the genus and species-level evolutionary relationships remain mostly unresolved. Noncladistic methodology previously was used in dividing extant and fossil palinurid genera into the Silentes, Jasus and Projasus, and the Stridentes, Linuparus, Puerulus, Palinustus, Justitia, Palinurus, and Panulirus (Parker 1883; George and Main 1967). Baisre (1994) used larval and adult characters from representatives of each palinurid genus and constructed a phenogram using cluster analyses; he generally found support for relationships suggested in George and Main (1967). Monophyletic genera are consistently supported (George and Main 1967; Holthuis 1991) with the one exception of whether Jasus verreauxi belongs in the genus Jasus (Brasher et al. 1992; Ovenden et al. 1997). McWilliam (1995) examined relationships within the most speciose palinurid genus, Panulirus, and added larval characters to the adult characters used in George and Main (1967). Ptacek et al. (2001) used 16S rDNA and cytochrome oxidase subunit I (COI) to examine groupings of Panulirus species. These studies generally support the scheme suggested by George and Main (1967), although lacking strong resolution for the suggested subgroupings within Panulirus. A new palinurid genus and species, Palibythus magnificus, recently was discovered which exhibits a fully developed sound producing apparatus closely resembling those of the Stridentes (Davie 1990).

Outgroup choice

Tam and Kornfeld (1998) utilized 16S rDNA to analyze phylogenetic relationships across the Nephropidae (clawed lobsters) and included members of the Scyllaridae (slipper lobsters) and Palinuridae in the analysis. The results of this analysis suggest that scyllarids and palinurids are sister taxa relative to Nephropidae. Baisre's (1994) results also suggest a close relationship between the Scyllaridae and the Palinuridae, placing Scyllarus as a more appropriate sister taxon to the palinurids than the nephropids. However, there is some contention as to whether Palinurellus belongs to its own family, the Synaxidae, or whether it is a member of the Palinuridae (Baisre 1994; Davie 1990). Some of Baisre's results suggest that Palinurellus might be paraphyletic with the Scyllaridae and the Palinuridae, whereas Davie suggests that Palinurellus belongs within the Palinuridae. Given this ambiguity, we included both Scyllarus (Scyllaridae) and Palinurellus (Synaxidae or Palinuridae) in the analyses, and designated Scyllarus as the outgroup.

Morphological characters

Most morphological characters were collected at the National Museum of Natural History (NMNH), Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC. Specimen identification was verified using Holthuis (1991). The species used and specimen identifications are listed in Appendix 1. We initially defined and coded 93 characters from the above specimens in both males and females whenever possible. We then removed apomorphic and constant characters and used the remaining 79 unordered characters in the analysis (Appendix 2). The character matrix is provided in Appendix 3.

Phylogenetic analyses were conducted using PAUP 4.0b6 and 4.0b10 (Swofford 2001). None of the characters were weighted and a priori character transformations were not determined. We calculated the shortest tree(s) using a heuristic search algorithm (Swofford et al. 1996) and ran 1000 bootstrap replicates (Felsenstein 1985a; Swofford et al. 1996).

Molecular characters

We examined rDNA from both nuclear (18S and 28S) and mitochondrial (16S) genomes. This choice of multiple genes, evolving at different rates, represents an attempt to resolve genus level divergences at several time scales (e.g., Hillis and Dixon 1991). rDNA sequences were obtained for at least one species within each genus in the Palinuridae. Sequence data were acquired from GenBank when available. Tissue sources and GenBank Accession numbers are listed in Appendix 1.

DNA was extracted and purified from muscle tissue using a G NOME extraction kit (BIO101, Vista, CA). 18S and 28S rDNA gene regions were amplified using standard PCR reactions: 35–40 cycles of 94° for 15 sec, 50–65° for 1 min 30 sec, 72° for 2 min 30 sec followed by 72° for 10 min. 16S was amplified in the same way except that the annealing temperature was 40–50°. The reactions were run on either a GeneAmp PCR System 9600 (Perkin Elmer, Wellesley, MA) or a Hybaid PCRExpress (Needham Heights, MA). Universal primers were used for 16S and 18S gene regions (Hillis and Dixon 1991). 28S primers were used as suggested in Jarman et al. (2000). The same primers were used for sequencing reactions.

PCR products were purified using Shrimp Alkaline Phosphatase Exonuclease I protocol (USB Corporation) with one cycle of 37° for 30 min and 80° for 15 min. For a small subset of specimens, when amplifying 28Sddff and 28Svx, cloning reactions were used to isolate PCR products (TOPO XL PCR Cloning Kit, Invitrogen Corp., Carlsbad, CA). Cloning products were purified using Qiaquick PCR Purification Kit (Qiagen, Inc., Valencia, CA). Sequencing reactions were run using ABI Prism BigDye Terminator Cycle Sequencing Ready Reaction Kit (Perkin Elmer, Applied Biosystems) and analyzed on an ABI Prism 3700 DNA Analyzer (Perkin Elmer, Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA). In all cases, except 28See and 28Smm (in which the sequences were too short to overlap), sequences were obtained in both 5′ and 3′ directions. Base spectrographs were checked visually with Sequencher 3.1.1. We aligned sequences using Clustal X (Thompson et al. 1997) and then checked visually to find areas of ambiguous alignment. Areas of ambiguous alignment were deleted from the analyses to avoid erroneous nonhomologous base comparisons (e.g., Swofford et al. 1996).

We analyzed each dataset separately because available taxa varied across datasets and combined analyses did not increase resolution of phylogenetic relationships (Patek 2001a). We performed maximum-likelihood analysis of each gene region. First we determined the best fit DNA substitution model for each gene region from 56 possible evolutionary models using likelihood ratio tests implemented by Modeltest vers. 3.04 (Posada and Crandall 1998). Next, trees were calculated using the best fit maximum-likelihood models and bootstrap values were calculated. We also calculated the most probable trees assuming the best fit maximum-likelihood model and using a Bayesian statistical framework that approximates posterior probabilities using a Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) search algorithm (Huelsenbeck et al. 2000a,b; Lewis 2001). For each molecular dataset, we ran one million replicates, with four chains, sampled every 100 replicates, and printed every 1000 replicates using the MrBayes vers. 1.10 software implementation of this approach (Huelsenbeck and Hall 2000). A consensus tree was calculated based on the last 1000 trees generated.

Trees Used in Phylogenetic Comparative Analyses

Phylogenetic comparative analyses were based on trees obtained from 18S, 16S, and morphological data sets (28S results provided minimal resolution; see Results). Because phylogenetic resolution and available taxa varied among data partitions, we performed comparative analyses on phylogenies from different datasets in order to find correlational patterns that were common to all analyses. First we used the most parsimonious tree based on morphological characters. Second, for the 18S and 16S molecular datasets to be used in phylogenetic comparative analyses, we used the posterior probability distribution of trees estimated by Bayesian methods. A Bayesian approach more effectively represents the range of likely tree topologies and branch lengths than the single most parsimonious or maximum-likelihood tree calculations (Huelsenbeck et al. 2000a,b; Lewis 2001). Using MrBayes (Huelsenbeck and Hall 2000; Huelsenbeck and Ronquist 2001), we ran 10,000 replicates with four chains, sampled every 10 replicates, and printed every 100 replicates; the last fifty trees from each analysis were used for calculations of independent contrasts (see below).

Sound Acquisition and Analysis

Rasp sounds were recorded from eight species representing five palinurid genera. Lobsters were handheld in most experiments since rasp sounds are naturally elicited during interactions with potential predators. Palinurus elephas specimens were collected by Cleggan Lobster Fisheries, Ltd. (Galway, Ireland). Palinustus waguensis and Panulirus japonicus specimens were collected by the Seto Marine Biological Laboratories staff and local fishermen in Shirahama, Japan. Justitia japonica, Linuparus trigonus, Panulirus homarus, and Panulirus longipes were recorded at the Okinawa Expo Aquarium, Okinawa, Japan. Panulirus argus specimens were collected using FL DEP permit 99S–428 at Long Key, Florida. Specimens from Ireland and Japan were purchased from fishermen or shared from other researchers and did not require the use of a permit.

A hydrophone (20 Hz–15,000 Hz; HTI-94-SSQ, High Tech, Inc., Gulfport, MS) was placed 0.3 m deep and at least 0.15 m from the lobster when possible (during international travel, some tanks were too small to place the hydrophone at this depth). Rasps were recorded on audio tape (Sony TCD5M audio cassette recorder). We first filtered the signal with a Krohn Hite (Brocton, MA) model 3500 band pass filter (20 Hz and 15000 Hz), then with a low pass 15,000 Hz filter (Stanford Research Systems, Sunnyvale, CA, Model S640). We digitally sampled the filtered signals at 37383.2 points/sec and analyzed them using Signal 3.0 software (Beeman 1992).

We measured the number of pulses, duration, and the pulse rate (number of pulses per second) for each rasp (Fig. 3). The rasps were visually represented using a Kay Elemetric Digital Sona Graph, 20–15,000 Hz frequency range (band pass filtered using Krohn Hite model 3550 filter), and 300 Hz frequency resolution. We analyzed at least ten rasps per individual.

Phylogenetic Comparative Tests of Trade-Offs Hypothesis

The trade-offs hypothesis was evaluated by examining correlated evolutionary change between antennal morphology, signal producing morphology, and signal features (Table 1). First, we tested whether mean and maximum values of rasp duration, pulse number, and pulse rate (as measured above) were positively correlated with file length. Second, we tested whether file length was positively correlated with antennular plate size (the structure on which the file is located; Fig. 1) so that, third, we could test whether there was a negative correlation between the size of the sound producing apparatus (as represented by the antennular plate size) and size of the surrounding antennae. Finally, we tested whether there was a gradeshift of antennal bending strength for a given antennular plate size, between Stridentes and Silentes.

In 41 species, male and female when possible, we measured the dimensions of the file, antennular plate and antennae (Fig. 1; Appendix 1). Male and female values were not significantly different (Patek 2001a), thus species values were averaged across males and females. For each individual, we measured body size as the carapace length (base of rostrum to posterior margin of cephalothorax). Morphometric data were log transformed prior to analysis to remove the dependence of variance on the mean values (Sokal and Rohlf 1981). Antennular plate length was measured from posterior to anterior along the midline. The width of the antennular plate was measured at the proximal most edge of the antennular plate. Antennal width was measured from the lateral joint articulation to the medial junction of the antenna where it meets the cephalothorax (in Silentes, the medial joint articulation). Antennular plate length and width were significantly positively correlated (see Results), so antennular plate length was used as a measure of antennular plate size in subsequent analyses because it had a higher level of measurement accuracy than antennular plate width.

The capacity for antennae to generate force hinges upon their ability to resist failure, specifically failure while bending (buckling). Strength in bending thus provides a measure of the capacity to generate force across species. Strength in bending in a thin walled cylinder is proportional to: I/R = (π/4)*(R4r4)/R, where I is the second moment of area, R is the outer radius of the cylinder, and r is the inner radius of the cylinder (Currey 1970; Wainwright et al. 1976; Alexander 1983) (Fig. 3). We measured R as the radius of the first segment of the antennae. We calculated r as R minus the cuticle thickness. Because most of the 41 species were museum specimens, it was not possible to cut the antennae to measure cuticle thickness. Using a personal collection, we were able to measure cuticle thickness of six species from both Silentes and Stridentes genera. We measured cuticle thickness in the dorsal and ventral surfaces of the antennae and used an average of these values for calculation of r across all species. Silentes cuticle thickness averaged 1.5% of carapace length whereas Stridentes averaged 1% carapace length. If systematic biases resulted from this estimation, strength in bending would be slightly underestimated in Silentes. Thus, our comparative tests would err in the direction against our predicted trade-offs that Silentes have stronger antennae. This calculation of strength in bending assumes that the materials are isotropic and the structure is perfectly cylindrical. Because we used this calculation for comparison across species, and actual values are not being reported, the violations of these assumptions should not systematically alter the observed patterns. We conducted additional comparative analyses using antennal diameter in place of I/R to verify that statistical significance was not unduly influenced by errors in cuticle thickness estimates.

We corrected for phylogenetic nonindependence of species data points by calculating independent contrasts (Felsenstein 1985b) using Compare version 4.4 (Martins 2001). Branch lengths only were calculated for the molecular datasets; branch lengths were set equal to one for morphological datasets, and, as recommended by the program documentation, branch lengths at unresolved nodes were set equal to a small value. Compare software does not allow missing data, hence each dataset used a different subset of taxa due to missing data or taxa in some datasets. We accounted for body size by inputting the morphological variables and body size measures into Compare software to calculate the regression line. We then calculated the residuals for each variable in contrasts space and used those residuals as the body size corrected values for further independent contrasts tests (Garland et al. 1992). Rasp signal features were not always significantly correlated with body size (Patek 2001a), thus these values were not corrected for body size. We calculated average Pearson product moment correlation coefficients for contrast values calculated across 50 Bayesian trees in the molecular datasets and the most parsimonious trees in the morphological phylogeny. We used two tailed tests and set the significance level at P < 0.05.

To test whether Silentes and Stridentes have different antennal bending strength for a given antennular plate size, we used a modification of a nonparametric rank test suggested in Garland et al. (1993). We regressed size-corrected contrasts of antennal bending strength on antennular plate size (also size-corrected with independent contrasts) and calculated the residuals from the regression line. For rank tests, the absolute values of the residuals were used. We identified the independent contrast calculated between the Stridentes clade and Silentes clade. The probability of the ranking of this contrast value was calculated as the rank score divided by the number of residuals. The test was conducted on the 12 most parsimonious trees of the morphological dataset and ten trees of the 16S Bayesian dataset (chosen arbitrarily as the last ten trees generated by MrBayes). The 18S dataset was too small to provide statistical power for this test.

Results

Palinurid Phylogeny

A heuristic search of the 79 morphological characters yielded twelve most parsimonious trees (tree length: 224; consistency index: 0.4911; retention index: 0.8014) (Fig. 4A). These and subsequent tree descriptions exclude uninformative characters.

We obtained 21 new complete 18S rDNA sequences, 11 new complete 16S rDNA sequences, and 44 new complete and partial 28S rDNA sequences (GenBank Accession numbers are listed in Appendix 1). Technical difficulties precluded obtaining all gene regions for all species. Also listed are sequences that were used in these analyses but which were obtained from GenBank.

The 18S dataset consisted of an edited, unambiguous alignment of 1852 base pairs and yielded 297 most parsimonious trees (informative characters: 109; tree length: 282; CI: 0.7374; RI: 0.8120) (Fig. 4B). For 16S tree searches, we assumed the best-fit model and parameter values as: Tamura-Nei (1993) model; NST = 6; Gamma shape = 0.9171; proportion of invariant sites = 0.6793.

16S rDNA analyses resulted in 57 most parsimonious trees (126 informative characters: tree length, 554; CI: 0.415; RI: 0.682) based on an edited, unambiguous alignment of 407 base pairs (Fig. 4C). For 16S tree searches, we assumed the best-fit model and parameter values as: HKY model; NST = 2; Gamma shape = 0.5547; proportion of invariant sites = 0.4717.

28S rDNA is a very large region so we sequenced four smaller areas within the 28S gene (ddff, ee, mm, and vx) and concatenated these regions into a single analysis. Three most parsimonious trees were found with an alignment of 2274 characters (139 informative characters; tree length: 725; CI: 6330; RI: 0.6022) (Fig. 4D). For 28S tree searches, we assumed the best-fit model and parameter values as: Tamura-Nei + Invariant sites + Gamma rate heterogeneity; NST = 6; Gamma shape = 0.6441; proportion of invariant sites = 0.4325.

Bootstrap values and Bayesian consensus values were consistent across the four analyses in supporting a Jasus + Projasus clade, the monophyly of the Panulirus genus (with the exception of the 18S dataset in which P. homarus, inflatus, and versicolor fell outside the genus) and the Palinurus genus. In the three maximum-likelihood tree topologies (Fig. 4B–D), the Silentes occur within the Stridentes. The morphological phylogeny (Fig. 4A) suggests a monophyletic clade of Stridentes. Bootstrap and Bayesian values do not preferentially support either of these topologies. Support is found for inclusion of Palinurellus in the family Palinuridae and, based on the 16S and 18S datasets, may form a clade with Jasus and Projasus species. Palibythus magnificus falls within the Palinuridae in all datasets.

Phylogenetic Comparative Tests of Correlated Signal and Morphological Change

First, we tested whether file length was correlated with signal features. The file/antennular plate complex noticeably varies across the palinurids, ranging from no file in Silentes to an elongate file in the Panulirus genus (Figs. 1, 2). Rasp duration, number of pulses, and pulse rate (pulses/second) were documented across eight palinurid species (Table 2). Average pulse number was positively correlated with file length across a size range of Panulirus argus individuals (Table 3, Fig. 5). Across palinurids in the morphological and 16S datasets, file length and average rasp duration were negatively correlated; pulse rate was positively correlated with file length; maximum number of pulses and file length were positively correlated (Table 3 and Fig. 5). No significant relationships were found when using the 18S dataset, possibly caused by extremely long branch lengths of Panulirus homarus that forced this species outside of the Panulirus clade for this particularly small dataset (seven taxa) (Fig. 4B).

File length was tightly correlated with antennular plate size, hence antennular plate size was used as a measure of the size of the sound producing mechanism (Table 4; Fig. 6). Antennular plate length and width were positively correlated (Table 4). Finally, we tested whether the size of signal producing morphology (antennular plate length) correlated negatively with bending strength of the surrounding antennae, as predicted by the trade-offs hypothesis. Antennular plate length and bending strength (I/R) were positively correlated in one of three datasets across all palinurids (Table 4; Fig. 6) and positively correlated in all datasets across Stridentes (Table 4; Fig. 6). Antennular plate length and antennal width followed the same pattern as those found for I/R (Table 4).

The comparison of antennular bending strength for a given antennular plate size between Stridentes and Silentes (both variables were corrected for body size using the independent contrasts method as discussed above) (Fig. 7), yielded significant differences in all 16S trees (10 trees; 28 residuals; average P = 0.036) and nonsignificant results in the morphological trees (12 trees; 40 residuals; average P = 0.11). In the 16S trees, the contrast value between Stridentes and Silentes always ranked first. The contrast value ranked third in ten of the morphological trees; the contrast in the remaining two trees ranked fourth and twenty-first.

Discussion

Two distinct patterns emerged from the comparative analyses of antennal strength and antennular plate size. Within Stridentes, antennal strength and antennular plate size were positively correlated, suggesting that body space did not impose an upper size limit to the structures. However, when antennal strength and antennular plate size were compared between Stridentes and Silentes, there was a distinct separation of the clades: with the effects of body size removed, for a given antennular plate size, Silentes had antennae with higher bending strength (Fig. 7D), suggesting that a size trade-off, in the form of a gradeshift, may indeed exist at this level of comparison. We begin by discussing the function of size changes in the sound producing apparatus, specifically the correlation found between the size of acoustic morphology (file length) and signal features (pulse number and rate). Then, we discuss the findings from the trade-off tests and consider the implications that body size did not appear to impose an upper limit within Stridentes, but may have been important at the broader scale when comparing the presence and absence of the sound producing apparatus between Silentes and Stridentes. In interpreting these results, we consider the potential role of developmental resource competition in the observed size trade-offs in spiny lobsters. We also propose two alternative explanations for the observed patterns, including the mechanics of sound production and the different selective pressures experienced by lobsters with two modes of predator defense (Stridentes: sound production and force production) versus one mode of defense (Silentes: force production).

Signal Feature and File Length Variation

Both between species and within Panulirus argus, the number and rate of pulses increased with file length (Fig. 5; Table 3), and hence increased with the overall size of the antennular plate (Fig. 6; Table 4). Intuitively, as the file length increased, the plectrum could move a greater distance, and hence a higher number of pulses could be produced. In contrast, a somewhat counterintuitive result was the weaker yet statistically significant finding that rasp duration decreased with increasing file length. These results were unexpected given that rasp duration equals pulse number divided by pulse rate. As such, in order for rasp duration to decrease, one would expect that pulse number would decrease and/or pulse rate increase. We offer two possible interpretations of these results.

The first interpretation is that there were behavioral differences in the use of the sound producing apparatus. Specifically, plectra (Fig. 1) could be moved simultaneously to maximize the pulse rate with an overall decrease in rasp duration. Alternatively, plectrums could be moved sequentially to maximize rasp duration with an overall decrease in pulse rate. Because these two strategies were not distinguished in the analyses, our findings that file length was correlated with decrease in average rasp duration and increase in average pulse rate suggest that the strategy of simultaneous plectrum movement was used more often than the sequential activation. In either strategy, the maximum number of pulses should increase with file length, which was supported in our results. Interestingly, these two types of behavioral strategies have been observed in pulsed frog sounds (Cocroft and Ryan 1995).

A second possibility is that the properties of frictional stick and slip sound production may have varied independently from file length across species due to frictional load, elasticity of component structures, and surface asperities (Patek 2002; Persson 2000; Rabinowicz 1995). These physical parameters determine the critical load and velocity for alternating sliding and sticking friction, hence affecting the pulse rate, pulse duration, and slip duration.

Size Trade-Offs

Counter to our a priori size trade-off hypothesis, both antennae and antennular plate size were positively correlated in Stridentes. The nearly twofold difference in antennular plate size within the Stridentes and no proportional decrease in size of the surrounding antennae suggested that body space did not impose a limit on the combined size of these structures. However, when Stridentes and Silentes were compared, there was general support for a gradeshift in antennal size relative to antennular plate size (Fig. 7). Because the comparison between Stridentes and Silentes involved an even greater, fivefold size differential in antennular plate size, space limits may have been more apparent at this level of analysis.

These kinds of size trade-offs can be interpreted at the level of available body space (Barel et al. 1989; Smits et al. 1996) as well as at the level of the costs and resources required to build the structures during development (Klingenberg and Nijhout 1998; Nijhout and Emlen 1998). The extreme range of antennular plate size in spiny lobsters also suggested a system in which construction costs during development should be considered, particularly the resource intensive costs in constructing the sound producing apparatus, including the file, plectrum, and additional musculature (Patek 2002).

Indeed, the life history of spiny lobsters suggests that constructional costs and resource limitations during development could limit size increases and cause the gradeshift observed between Stridentes and Silentes. For resource competition between body parts to occur, resources must be limited in the time period when the structure develops (Emlen 2001). In spiny lobsters, the apparatus is first visible, but not functional, in the late phyllosoma stage when the larvae are still planktonic (Meyer-Rochow and Penrose 1974). The phyllosoma larvae molt into puerulus larvae and swim toward the continental slope. Notably, resources are limited because the pueruli are not known to feed during this time period and probably rely on stored nutrients (Booth and Phillips 1994; Meyer-Rochow and Penrose 1974). The larvae eventually settle on the substrate and molt into postpuerulus larvae, at which time the file grows in length, file surface features (shingles) increase in number, and the plectrum becomes fully differentiated (Meyer-Rochow and Penrose 1974). Meyer-Rochow and Penrose (1974) recorded sounds produced by the apparatus in postpuerulus larvae (incidental sounds were recorded in the pueruli). During these resource-limited time periods, the larger antennular plate size observed in Stridentes could impose resource costs not present in Silentes, thus causing a size trade-off with the surrounding antennae.

As an alternative explanation to resource-based size trade-offs, the observed gradeshift may be due biomechanical and physiological limits imposed by muscle attachment area and construction of the sound producing antennal joint. The antennal muscles attach over a substantial surface area of the dorsal and ventral cephalothorax (Patek 2002; Paterson 1968). Although greater antennal size provides space for larger muscles, these larger muscles must have sufficient space for attachment in the cephalothorax. Furthermore, as mentioned in the introduction (Fig. 1), Stridentes have lost their medial joint articulation such that the plectrum (a modification of the joint articulation) can translate across the file to make sound (Patek 2002). As a result, the Stridentes have a sliding joint articulation with multiple degrees of freedom, whereas the first antennal joint in Silentes is limited to one degree of freedom of movement. Increases in antennal diameter may be more effective for increasing strength and force in Silentes, but at the cost of manipulability for Stridentes. The differences in joint mechanics could lead to decreased stability of the antennae during force production in Stridentes, thus limiting them to smaller antennae.

Ecological factors may also have played a role in patterns of antennal strength across these palinurid taxa; specifically, higher predation pressures in two palinurid genera may provide an alternative explanation to the observed gradeshift. Theories of escalation propose that predator defenses increase in taxa that move to warmer, more productive waters (Vermeij 1987). Response to predation includes a suite of predator defenses, including increased exoskeletal strength, and targeted defenses, such as toxicity. In Figure 7D, the Stridentes and Silentes have distinctly separate distributions of strength in bending relative to antennular plate length. However, Silentes and Panulirus species have overlapping areas of strength in bending (Fig. 7A, D). Interestingly, two separate invasions of relatively predator-rich habitats indicate that predation has been an important selective force in palinurid evolution (Brasher et al. 1992; George 1997; George and Main 1967; McWilliam 1995; Ovenden et al. 1997; Pollock 1990, 1992, 1993; Ptacek et al. 2001). First, the Stridentes genus Panulirus invaded shallow, tropical waters (George 1997; George and Main 1967). The movement of taxa toward warmer waters and complex reef systems was likely accompanied by increased predation pressures and hence increased selection on predator defenses (Vermeij 1987; Pollock 1995). Secondly, major shifts in Jasus (Silentes) distribution may also have led to increased predation pressure and some members of Jasus and Panulirus now inhabit similar ecological habitats (George 1997; Pollock 1995). The similar distribution of strength in bending between Panulirus and Jasus (Fig. 7D) suggests responses to predation by increasing the strength and potential force production of their antennal weapons. With a suitable dataset on the ecology and predation pressures across lobster species, phylogenetic comparative analyses could be used to test whether a statistical association indeed exists between these factors.

Size and predation pressures are also germane to explanations of the observed gradeshift, particularly when the evolutionary origins of communication structures are considered. The evolutionary origins of communication structures often involve “co-option” of existing motor or structural systems (Bailey 1991; Bradbury and Vehrencamp 1998; Lee 1995; Nowicki et al. 1992; Spurway and Haldane 1953); this can result in the communication structure being used in a similar behavioral context as the pre-existing motor system (e.g. spiny lobster antennae are used as defensive weapons and, in a subset of taxa, in acoustic defense). This poses a theoretical twist to the size trade-offs described in previous studies (Emlen 2001; Nijhout and Emlen 1998; Podos 1997, 2001): the communication structure and the structure with which it shares space and function may both increase in performance through increase in size. In other words, size increases of one structure could potentially decrease the size and performance of the other structure, even though they both are used for related behavioral functions. Thus, we add a new dimension to the question of evolutionary trade-offs by examining correlated evolutionary size changes when shared morphological systems are used in related behavioral functions and when both systems increase in performance through increases in size.

Hence, an alternative explanation to size trade-offs for the observed distribution of antennal strength may arise from the shared structure and function of the antennal region. Specifically, the Stridentes and Silentes may respond differently to selection for predator defenses. In particular, the presence of an additional mode of predator defense in Stridentes may result in relatively less selection for antennal strength compared to Silentes. If Jasus and Panulirus clades both encountered increased predation at different time periods in evolutionary history, the selection for predator defenses might have operated differently on taxa with one major defense (antennal weapons) versus two major defenses (antennal weapons plus defensive sound production). Are taxa with antennal weapons plus an aposematic sound producing apparatus better at deterring predators than taxa only having antennal weapons? If yes, then Silentes may be under greater pressure to increase the size and bending strength of their antennae. Thus, one would expect the observed pattern that for a given antennular plate size, Silentes have stronger antennae (Fig. 7), not due to size trade-offs, but instead due to different predator deterrent performance in these two clades.

Conclusions

Morphology plays a key role in the evolutionary origins of communication structures as well as the nutritional and performance limits imposed by the shared structures of other behavioral systems. These interactions are complex and require multiple levels of analysis—within species, within developmental time periods, and across clades. Spiny lobsters offer a particularly interesting system for studies of structural limits on communication evolution because of their relatively simple sound producing mechanism and small number of taxa. The presence of shared structures that are used in different modes of the same behavioral function adds an additional dimension, peculiarly common to communication systems, in which shared structural trade-offs might also be interpreted as shared functional trade-offs. An understanding of correlated evolutionary changes in morphology and behavior is fundamental to explaining morphology's evolutionary role in the remarkable diversity of animal communication systems.

Acknowledgments

S. Nowicki, C. Nunn and J. Podos provided key insights and suggestions. We greatly appreciate the comments by P. Wainwright, A. Summers, and the anonymous reviewers of this paper. Thank you to C. Cunningham and B. Kong for advice and assistance when working in the Cunningham molecular systematics laboratory. C. Nunn provided valuable training in the use of phylogenetic comparative methods. M. Beebee, C. Cunningham, W. Hoese, W. Kier, M. McHenry, D. McShea, S. Peters, K. Smith, S. Wainwright, and Nowicki laboratory members offered excellent advice and comments. Thank you to M. Childress and M. Ptacek for their exceptional generosity in providing preserved lobster tissue from the Lobster Phylogeny Project. For help in specimen acquisition and recording sounds, we thank K. Adachi, D. Brown, T-Y Chan, P. Davie, J. Fouere, T. Hamano, N. Higashi, Y. Murata, H. Ono, G. Paulay, M. Retamal, J. Shields, S. Uchida, K. Wada, A. Williams, S. Yamato, the Okinawa Expo Aquarium, and Seto Marine Laboratory of Kyoto University. Funding to SNP was provided by the Philanthropic Education Organization, the Smithsonian Institution, Duke University Graduate School, and the National Science Foundation (DIG No. 9972597 and GRF).

Literature Cited

1.

A. J. Alexander 1958. On the stridulation of scorpions. Behaviour 12:333–352. Google Scholar

2.

R. M. Alexander 1983. Animal mechanics. Blackwell Scientific, Boston, MA. Google Scholar

3.

W. J. Bailey 1991. Acoustic behaviour of insects: an evolutionary perspective. Chapman and Hall, New York. Google Scholar

4.

J. A. Baisre 1994. Phyllosoma larvae and the phylogeny of the Palinuroidea (Crustacea: Decapoda): a review. Aust. J. Mar. Freshwater Res 45:925–944. Google Scholar

5.

C. D N. Barel 1993. Concepts of an architectonic approach to transformation morphology. Acta Biotheor 41:345–381. Google Scholar

6.

C. D N. Barel, G. C. Anker, F. Witte, R. J C. Hoogerhoud, and T. Goldschmidt . 1989. Constructional constraints and its ecomorphological implications. Acta Morphol. Neerl.-Scand 27:83–109. Google Scholar

7.

K. Beeman 1992. SIGNAL User's manual. Engineering Design, Belmont, MA. Google Scholar

8.

J. D. Booth and B. F. Phillips . 1994. Early life history of spiny lobster. Crustaceana 66:271–294. Google Scholar

9.

J. W. Bradbury and S. L. Vehrencamp . 1998. Principles of animal communication. Sinauer Associates, MA. Google Scholar

10.

D. J. Brasher, J. R. Ovendon, and R. W G. White . 1992. Mitochondrial DNA variation and phylogenetic relationships of Jasus spp. (Decapoda: Palinuridae). J. Zool. Lond 227:1–16. Google Scholar

11.

J. S. Cobb 1981. Behaviour of the western Australian spiny lobster, Panulirus cygnus George, in the field and laboratory. Aust. J. Mar. Freshwater Res 32:399–409. Google Scholar

12.

R. B. Cocroft and M. J. Ryan . 1995. Patterns of advertisement call evolution in toads and chorus frogs. Anim. Behav 49:283–303. Google Scholar

13.

J. D. Currey 1970. Animal Skeletons. Crane, Russak and Company, New York. Google Scholar

14.

P. J F. Davie 1990. A new genus and species of marine crayfish, Palibythus magnificus, and new records of Palinurellus (Decapoda: Palinuridae) from the Pacific Ocean. Invertebr. Taxon 4:685–695. Google Scholar

15.

M. Edmunds 1974. Defence in animals: a survey of anti-predator defenses. Longman Group, Essex, U.K. Google Scholar

16.

D. J. Emlen 2001. Costs and the diversification of exaggerated animal structures. Science 291:1534–1536. Google Scholar

17.

J. Felsenstein 1985a. Confidence limits on phylogenies: an approach using the bootstrap. Evolution 39:783–791. Google Scholar

18.

J. Felsenstein 1985b. Phylogenies and the comparative method. Am. Nat 125:1–15. Google Scholar

19.

W. T. Fitch 1999. Acoustic exaggeration of size in birds via tracheal elongation: comparative and theoretical analyses. J. Zool 248:31–48. Google Scholar

20.

G. Gamberale and B. S. Tullberg . 1996. Evidence for a peak-shift in predator generalization among aposematic prey. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 263:1329–1334. Google Scholar

21.

T. Garland Jr., A. W. Dickerman, C. M. Janis, and J. A. Jones . 1993. Phylogenetic analysis of covariance by computer simulation. Syst. Biol 42:265–292. Google Scholar

22.

T. J. Garland, P. H. Harvey, and A. R. Ives . 1992. Procedures for the analysis of comparative data using phylogenetically independent contrasts. Syst. Biol 41:18–32. Google Scholar

23.

R. W. George 1997. Tectonic plate movements and the evolution of Jasus and Panulirus spiny lobsters (Palinuridae). Mar. Freshwater Res 48:1121–1130. Google Scholar

24.

R. W. George and A. R. Main . 1967. The evolution of spiny lobsters (Palinuridae): a study of evolution in the marine environment. Evolution 21:803–820. Google Scholar

25.

J. L. Gittleman and P. H. Harvey . 1980. Why are distasteful prey not cryptic? Nature 286:149–150. Google Scholar

26.

T. Guilford 1990. The evolution of aposematism. Pp. 23–61 in D. L. Evans and J. O. Schmidt, eds. Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators. State Univ. of New York Press, Albany, NY. Google Scholar

27.

D. M. Hillis and M. T. Dixon . 1991. Ribosomal DNA: Molecular evolution and phylogenetic inference. Q. Rev. Biol 66:411–453. Google Scholar

28.

W. J. Hoese, J. Podos, N. C. Boetticher, and S. Nowicki . 2000. Vocal tract function in birdsong production: experimental manipulation of beak movements. J. Exp. Biol 203:1845–1855. Google Scholar

29.

L. B. Holthuis 1991. Marine lobsters of the world. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome. Google Scholar

30.

J. P. Huelsenbeck and B. Hall . 2000. MrBayes: a program for the Bayesian inference of phylogeny,. Department of Biology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY. Google Scholar

31.

J. P. Huelsenbeck and F. Ronquist . 2001. MRBAYES: Bayesian inference of phylogenetic trees. Bioinformatics 17:754–755. Google Scholar

32.

J. P. Huelsenbeck, B. Rannala, and B. Larget . 2000a. A Bayesian framework for the analysis of cospeciation. Evolution 54:352–364. Google Scholar

33.

J. P. Huelsenbeck, B. Rannala, and J. P. Masly . 2000b. Accomodating phylogenetic uncertainty in evolutionary studies. Science 288:2349–2350. Google Scholar

34.

S. N. Jarman, S. Nicol, N. G. Elliot, and A. McMinn . 2000. 28S rDNA evolution in the Eumalacostraca and the phylogenetic position of Krill. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol 17:26–36. Google Scholar

35.

W. Jetz, C. Rowe, and T. Guilford . 2001. Nonwarning odors trigger innate color aversions—as long as they are novel. Behav. Ecol 12:134–139. Google Scholar

36.

P. Kanciruk 1980. Ecology of juvenile and adult Palinuridae (spiny lobsters). Pp. 59–96 in J. S. Cobb and B. F. Phillips, eds. The biology and management of lobsters. Vol. 2. Academic Press, New York. Google Scholar

37.

S. Kelly, A. B. MacDiarmid, and R. C. Babcock . 1999. Characteristics of spiny lobster, Jasus edwarsii, aggregations in exposed reef and sandy areas. Mar. Freshwater Res 50:409–416. Google Scholar

38.

C. P. Klingenberg and H. F. Nijhout . 1998. Competition among growing organs and developmental control of morphological asymmetry. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265:1135–1139. Google Scholar

39.

G. V. Lauder 1981. Form and function: structural analysis in evolutionary morphology. Paleobiology 74:430–442. Google Scholar

40.

S. Y. Lee 1995. Cheliped size and structure: the evolution of a multifunctional decapod organ. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol 193:161–176. Google Scholar

41.

E. E. Lewis and J. H. Cane . 1990. Stridulation as a primary antipredator defense of a beetle. Anim. Behav 40:1003–1004. Google Scholar

42.

P. O. Lewis 2001. Phylogenetic systematics turns over a new leaf. Trends Ecol. Evol 16:30–37. Google Scholar

43.

R. G. Lindberg 1955. Growth, population dynamics, and field behavior in the spiny lobster, Panulirus interruptus (Randall). Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool 59:157–248. Google Scholar

44.

B. Mahler and P. L. Tubaro . 2001. Relationship between song characters and morphology in New World pigeons. Biol. J. Linn. Soc 74:533–539. Google Scholar

45.

E. P. Martins 2001. COMPARE. Computer programs for the statistical analysis of comparative data. Distributed by the author, Department of Biology, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, via the WWW at  http://compare.bio.indiana.edu/Google Scholar

46.

W. M. Masters 1979. Insect disturbance stridulation: its defensive role. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 5:187–200. Google Scholar

47.

W. M. Masters 1980. Insect disturbance stridulation: characterization of airborne and vibrational components of sound. J. Comp. Physiol 135:259–268. Google Scholar

48.

B. E. McClelland, W. Wilczynski, and M. J. Ryan . 1996. Correlations between call characteristics and morphology in male cricket frogs (Acris crepitans). J. Exp. Biol 199:1907–1919. Google Scholar

49.

B. E. McClelland, W. Wilczynski, and M. J. Ryan . 1998. Intraspecific variation in laryngeal and ear morphology in male cricket frogs (Acris crepitans). Biol. J. Linn. Soc 63:51–67. Google Scholar

50.

P. S. McWilliam 1995. Evolution of the phyllosoma and puerulus phases of the spiny lobster genus Panulirus White. J. Crustacean Biol 15:542–557. Google Scholar

51.

V. B. Meyer-Rochow and J. D. Penrose . 1974. Sound production and sound emission apparatus in puerulus and postpuerulus of the western rock lobster. J. Exp. Zool 189:283–289. Google Scholar

52.

V. B. Meyer-Rochow and J. D. Penrose . 1976. Sound production by the western rock lobster Panulirus longipes (Milne Edwards). J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol 23:191–209. Google Scholar

53.

J. Moulton 1957. Sound production in the spiny lobster Panulirus argus (Latreille). Biol. Bull 113:286–295. Google Scholar

54.

B. E. Mulligan and R. B. Fischer . 1977. Sounds and behavior of the spiny lobster Panulirus argus. Crustaceana 32:185–199. Google Scholar

55.

H. F. Nijhout and D. J. Emlen . 1998. Competition among body parts in the development and evolution of insect morphology. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci 95:3685–3689. Google Scholar

56.

S. Nowicki 1987. Vocal tract resonances in oscine bird sound production: evidence from birdsongs in a helium atmosphere. Nature 325:53–55. Google Scholar

57.

S. Nowicki and J. Podos . 1993. Complexity, coupling, and contingency in the production of birdsong. Pp. 159–186 in P. P. G. Bateson and P. H. Klopfer, eds. Perspectives in ethology: behavior and evolution. Plenum Press, New York. Google Scholar

58.

S. Nowicki, M. Westneat, and W. Hoese . 1992. Birdsong: motor function and the evolution of communication. Semin. Neurosci 4:385–390. Google Scholar

59.

J. R. Ovenden, J. D. Booth, and A. J. Smolenski . 1997. Mitochondrial DNA phylogeny of red and green rock lobsters (genus Jasus). Mar. Freshwater Res 48:1131–1136. Google Scholar

60.

T. J. Parker 1883. On the structure of the head in Palinurus with special reference to the classification of the genus. Nature 29:189–190. Google Scholar

61.

S. Patek 2001a. Signal producing morphology and the evolution of palinurid lobster communication. Ph.D. diss. Duke University, Durham, NC. Google Scholar

62.

S. N. Patek 2001b. Spiny lobsters stick and slip to make sound. Nature 411:153–154. Google Scholar

63.

S. N. Patek 2002. Squeaking with a sliding joint: mechanics and motor control of sound production in palinurid lobsters. J. Exp. Biol 205:2375–2385. Google Scholar

64.

N. F. Paterson 1968. The anatomy of the cape rock lobster, Jasus lalandii (H. Milne Edwards). Ann. S. Afr. Muse 51:1–232. Google Scholar

65.

B. N J. Persson 2000. Sliding friction: physical principles and applications. Springer-Verlag, Berlin. Google Scholar

66.

J. Podos 1997. A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51:537–551. Google Scholar

67.

J. Podos 2001. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin's finches. Nature 409:185–188. Google Scholar

68.

D. E. Pollock 1990. Palaeoceanography and speciation in the spiny lobster genus Jasus. Bull. Mar. Sci 46:387–405. Google Scholar

69.

D. E. Pollock 1992. Palaeoceanography and speciation in the spiny lobster genus Panulirus in the Indo-Pacific. Bull. Mar. Sci 51:135–146. Google Scholar

70.

D. E. Pollock 1993. Speciation in spiny lobsters—clues to climatically-induced changes in ocean circulation patterns. Bull. Mar. Sci 53:937–944. Google Scholar

71.

D. E. Pollock 1995. Evolution of life-history patterns in three genera of spiny lobsters. Bull. Mar. Sci 57:516–526. Google Scholar

72.

D. Posada and K. A. Crandall . 1998. Modeltest: testing the model of DNA substitution. Bioinformatics 14:817–818. Google Scholar

73.

M. B. Ptacek, S. K. Sarver, M. J. Childress, and W. F. Herrnkind . 2001. Molecular phylogeny of the spiny lobster genus Panulirus (Decapoda: Palinuridae). Mar. Freshwater Res 52:1037–1047. Google Scholar

74.

E. Rabinowicz 1995. Friction and wear of materials. John Wiley and Sons, New York. Google Scholar

75.

M. J. Ryan 1986. Factors influencing the evolution of acoustic communication: biological constraints. Brain Behav. Evol 28:70–82. Google Scholar

76.

M. J. Ryan and E. A. Brenowitz . 1985. The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am. Nat 126:87–100. Google Scholar

77.

T. D. Sargent 1990. Startle as an anti-predator mechanism, with special reference to the underwing moths, (Catocala). Pp. 229–249 in D. L. Evans and J. O. Schmidt, eds. Insect defenses: adaptive mechanisms and strategies of prey and predators. State University of New York Press, Albany, NY. Google Scholar

78.

S. Sarver, J. Silberman, and P. Walsh . 1998. Mitochondrial DNA sequence evidence supporting the recognition of two subspecies or species of the Florida spiny lobster Panulirus argus. J. Crustacean Biol 18:177–186. Google Scholar

79.

S. C. Schenk and P. C. Wainwright . 2001. Dimorphism and the functional basis of claw strength in six brachyuran crabs. J. Zool. Lond 255:105–119. Google Scholar

80.

T. N. Sherratt 2002. The coevolution of warning signals. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269:741–46. Google Scholar

81.

M. Smale 1974. The warning squeak of the Natal rock lobster. S. Afr. Assoc. Mar. Biol. Res. Bull 11:17–19. Google Scholar

82.

J. D. Smits, F. Witte, and F. G. VanVeen . 1996. Functional changes in the anatomy of the pharyngeal jaw apparatus of Astatoreochromis alluaudi (Pisces, Cichlidae), and their effects on adjacent structures. Biol. J. Linn. Soc 59:389–409. Google Scholar

83.

R. R. Sokal and F. J. Rohlf . 1981. Biometry. 2d ed. Freeman, New York. Google Scholar

84.

E. Spanier and R. K. Zimmer-Faust . 1988. Some physical properties of shelter that influence den preference in spiny lobsters. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol 121:137–149. Google Scholar

85.

M. P. Speed 2000. Warning signals, receiver psychology and predator memory. Anim. Behav 60:269–278. Google Scholar

86.

H. Spurway and J. B S. Haldane . 1953. The comparative ethology of vertebrate breathing. I. Breathing in newts, with a general survey. Behaviour 6:8–31. Google Scholar

87.

A. Summers 2001. The lobster's violin. Nat. Hist 110:26–27. Google Scholar

88.

D. L. Swofford 2001. Phylogenetic analysis using parsimony (and other methods). Sinauer Associates, Sunderland MA. Google Scholar

89.

D. L. Swofford, G. J. Olsen, P. J. Waddell, and D. M. Hillis . 1996. Molecular systematics. Pp. 407–514 in D. M. Hillis, C. Moritz and B. K. Mable, eds. Molecular systematics. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA. Google Scholar

90.

Y. K. Tam and I. Kornfield . 1998. Phylogenetic relationships of clawed lobster genera (Decapoda: Nephropidae) based on mitochondrial 16S rRNA gene sequences. J. Crustacean Biol 18:138–146. Google Scholar

91.

K. Tamura and M. Nei . 1993. Estimation of the number of nucleotide substitutions in the control region of mitochondrial DNA in humans and chimpanizees. Mol. Biol. Evol 10:512–526. Google Scholar

92.

G. Taylor 2001. The evolution of armament strength: evidence for a constraint on the biting performance of claws of durophagous decapods. Evolution 55:550–560. Google Scholar

93.

J. D. Thompson, T. J. Gibson, F. Plewniak, F. Jeanmougin, and D. G. Higgins . 1997. The Clustal X windows interface: flexible strategies for multiple sequence alignment aided by quality analysis tools. Nucleic Acids Res 24:4876–4882. Google Scholar

94.

G. J. Vermeij 1987. Evolution and escalation: an ecological history of life. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ. Google Scholar

95.

P. C. Wainwright and S. M. Reilly . 1994. Ecological morphology: integrative organismal biology. The Univ. of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL. Google Scholar

96.

S. A. Wainwright, W. D. Biggs, J. D. Currey, and J. M. Gosline . 1976. Mechanical design in organisms. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ. Google Scholar

97.

M. W. Westneat, J. H. Long, W. Hoese, and S. Nowicki . 1993. Kinematics of birdsong: functional correlation of cranial movements and acoustic features in sparrows. J. Exp. Biol 182:147–171. Google Scholar

Appendices

Appendix 1

Appendix 1 Species included in morphological, morphometric, and molecular analyses. “Morphology” column includes specimen identifications for taxa used in morphological phylogeny and morphometric analyses. “Molecular” column lists identifications of tissue sources. Genbank accession numbers are listed for rDNA sequences including both original sequence data and previously available sequences. U and AF indicate GenBank acquisition numbers; * indicates new sequence; SI, Smithsonian Institution; W, Queensland Museum; numbered identifications in the “Molecular” column are from the Lobster Phylogeny Project

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-ta01.gif

Appendix 2

Characters used in the morphological phylogeny (some characters modified from George and Main 1967; Holthuis 1991).

  1. Number of spines on antennular plate: 0 spines (0); 2 spines (1); 4 spines (2).

  2. Bare and smooth lateral margin of carapace covering branchiostegites with distinct medial margin: absent (0); present (1).

  3. Postcervical and intestinal teeth on dorsal surface of carapace: absent (0); present (1).

  4. Number of median teeth or tubercles on dorsal median ridge of carapace between cervical and post cervical groove: none (0); 1–3 teeth (1); 4–6 teeth (2).

  5. Number of median teeth on median ridge in dorsal intestinal region: none (0); 1–3 teeth (1); 4–6 teeth (2).

  6. Postorbital spine: absent (0); present (1).

  7. Median ridge posterior to cervical groove: absent (0); present (1).

  8. Spinose median ridge from postcervical groove to posterior margin of carapace: absent (0); present (1).

  9. Submedian row of spines posterior to cervical groove (not on a ridge): absent (0); present (1).

  10. Submedian ridges posterior to cervical groove: absent (0); present (1).

  11. Lateral ridges posterior to cervical groove: absent (0); present (1).

  12. Rostrum: present (0); absent (1).

  13. Two processes of opthalmic sternite: absent (0); present (1).

  14. Frontal horns: absent (0); fused (1); widely separated (2).

  15. Shape of frontal horns: absent/spine only (0); truncated and crenulate (George and Main 1967) (1); taper to a point (2); bulbous with spine (3).

  16. Median spine on anterior margin of carapace between frontal horns: absent (0); present (1).

  17. Antennal flagella: reduced (small diameter and limp) (0); straight and inflexible (1); flexible (2).

  18. Antennular flagella: shorter than antennular peduncle (0); longer than peduncle (1).

  19. Medial, ventral, posterior spine on basal antennal segment: absent (0); present (1).

  20. Median carina on abdominal somites: present (0); absent (1).

  21. Two anterior/posterior lines of small spines on abdominal somites 2–4 on each side of the median carina: absent (0); present (1).

  22. Two prominent spines at posterolateral edge of abdominal somite 6: absent (0); present (1).

  23. Two submedian pairs of spines on abdominal somite 6: absent (0); present (1).

  24. Structure of abdominal and pleural grooves: none (0); shallow with smooth margins (1); deep with squamate margins (2); deep with sharp margins (3).

  25. Length of anterior groove relative to posterior groove on abdominal somites 2–5: fewer than two grooves (0); anterior shorter than posterior groove (1).

  26. Spines anterior and posterior to transverse groove on abdominal somites 2–4: absent (0); present (1).

  27. Abdominal somite 1 sculpture: no sculpturation (0); 1st somite smooth, then 2–5 somites sculptured (1); 1st somite faintly ridged, then 2–5 somites sculptured (2); 1–5 somites sculptured (3).

  28. Number of rows/layers of squamae anterior to transverse groove on abdominal somites 2–6: none (0); 1 row (1); 2 rows (2); 3–5 rows (3).

  29. Squamae posterior to transverse groove on abdominal somites 2–5: none (0); squamae anterior and posterior to groove (1); squamae anterior and not posterior to groove (2).

  30. Pubescent line on dorsal surface of abdominal somites (not in groove): absent (0); present (1).

  31. Transverse groove on abdominal somite 2 meets anterior groove on pleuron: absent (0); present (1).

  32. Transverse groove on abdominal somite 2 approaches, but does not meet, anterior groove on pleuron: absent (0); present (1).

  33. Transverse groove on abdominal somite 2 meets posterior groove on pleuron: absent (0); present (1).

  34. Transverse groove on abdominal somites 3–4 meets anterior groove on pleura: absent (0); present (1).

  35. Transverse groove on abdominal somites 3–4 approaches anterior groove on pleura: absent (0); present (1).

  36. Transverse groove on abdominal somites 3–4 meets posterior groove on pleura: absent (0); present (1).

  37. Anterior deep, distinct furrow on pleuron 2: absent (0); present (1).

  38. Posterior deep, distinct furrow on pleuron 2: absent (0); present (1).

  39. Anterior deep, distinct furrow on pleuron 3–5: absent (0); present (1).

  40. Posterior deep, distinct furrow on pleuron 3–5: absent (0); present (1).

  41. Teeth and/or spines on posterior margin pleura on abdominal somites: absent (0); present (1).

  42. Expansion of distal segment of endopod of female pleopod 2: absent (0); vestigial (1); reduced (½ length of exopod) (2); long (greater than ½ length of exopod) (3).

  43. Expansion of penultimate segment of endopod of female pleopod 2: absent (0); vestigial (1); pleopod-like (2).

  44. Endopod on pleopod 2 in males: absent (0); present (1).

  45. Pleopod 1in females: absent (0); vestigial (1); present (2).

  46. Shape of the chitinous base of telson: rounded base (0); rounded base with indentations along lateral pieces (1); posteriorly extending lateral margins (2).

  47. Sharp points on anterior and posterior sides of mandibles (not occluding): absent (0); present (1).

  48. Laterally positioned, anteriorly/posteriorly directed ridges on epistome articulations: absent (0); granulation (1); anterior tooth (2).

  49. Median furrow on epistome: absent (0); present (1).

  50. Position of mandibular complex relative to epistome: posterior to (0); adjacent/overlapping (1).

  51. Distal process of palp: reduced (round and small) (0); elongate (1).

  52. Anterior width of supralabral ridge relative to posterior width of labrum: supralabral ridge greater than ½ width of labrum (0); supralabral ridge less than or equal to ½ width of labrum (1).

  53. Row of single nodes/tubercles on midline/axis of sternum: absent (0); present (1).

  54. Row of paired nodes/tubercles on midline/axis of sternum: absent (0); present (1).

  55. Row of single spines on axis/midline of sternum: absent (0); present (1).

  56. Small spines on the axial to the anterior edge of sternal segments: absent (0); present (1).

  57. Prominent spines on the sternum medial and adjacent to pereiopod attachment: absent (0); present (1).

  58. Location of toothed, median chitinous margin of male genital aperture: absent (0); toothed anteriorly (1); toothed anteriorly and posteriorly (2).

  59. Male genitals on chitinous tube-like extension off base of fifth walking legs: absent (0); present (1).

  60. Anterolaterally/posteroventrally directed ridge on medial side of genital pore: absent (0); present (1).

  61. Flagellum on the exopod of the 2nd maxilliped: present (0); reduced (1); absent (2).

  62. Exopod on third maxilliped: present with flagellum (0); present without flagelum (1); absent (2).

  63. Rows of spines on the ventral surface of the third maxillipeds: absent (0); 1 row (1); 2 rows (2); 3 rows (3).

  64. Pereiopods: smooth or with short hairs (0); woolly (1).

  65. Dactyl hair arrangement on pereiopods 1–2: none (0); diffuse on ventral side (1); anterior/posterior rows on ventral side (2).

  66. Dactyl hair arrangement pereiopod 4: none (0); diffuse on ventral side (1); anterior and posterior rows and dorsal side (2).

  67. Distal ventral median spine on propodus: absent (0); present (1).

  68. Anterodorsal spine on carpus of first pereiopod: absent (0); present (1).

  69. Spinules (tiny spines) on the ventral margin of merus and ischium of pereiopods: absent (0); present (1).

  70. Spine on the dorsal, distal median side of the merus on first pereiopod: absent (0); present (1).

  71. Row of small spines on ventral, distal, anterior side of merus on first pereiopod (not part of the joint): absent (0); present (1).

  72. Single spine on ventral, distal, anterior side of merus on first pereiopod (not joint): absent (0); present (1).

  73. Spine on ventral, distal antero/median side of ischium on the first pereiopod: absent (0); present (1).

  74. Lateral groove on merus of pereiopods 2–5: absent (0); present (1).

  75. Plectrum nail extension: none (0); tissue (1); narrow soft chitinous extension (2); wide band chitinous extension (3); prominent chitinous nail (4).

  76. Medial process of first segment of antennae: absent (0); thin rounded process (1); wide flattened process (2).

  77. Antennular plate: absent (0); reduced (1); present (2).

  78. Median, longitudinal groove on antennular plate: present (0); absent (1).

  79. Sound-producing apparatus on antennular plate: absent (0); present (1).

Appendix 3

Appendix 3 Matrix of character states used in the morphological phylogeny

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-ta03.gif

Fig. 1.

(A) In an oblique view of Panulirus argus, a Stridentes species, the plectra rub over the files to produce sound. The plectrum is a medial extension off the base of each antenna and the files are located on each side of the antennular plate. The flap extends posteriorly off of the plectrum and is not necessary for sound production (Patek 2002). (B) In a schematic, anterior view of a Silentes species, each antenna has two joint articulations, one laterally with the cephalothorax and one medially with the antennular plate. (C) In a schematic, anterior view of a Stridentes species, the medial joint articulation of each antenna is replaced by the plectrum. The plectrum slides over the file which is part of the antennular plate. Image (A) adapted from Summers (2001)

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f01.jpg

Fig. 2.

The Stridentes and Silentes genera of the Palinuridae. Photographs show an anterior view of each lobster with eyes and antennal bases in the center of each photograph. Jasus edwarsii (A) and Projasus bahamondei (B) do not produce sound; the antennular plate is small and joint articulations are present at the medial and lateral edges of the basal antennal segment. Stridentes include: (C) Justitia longimanus, (D) Linuparus sordidus, (E) Palibythus magnificus, (F) Palinurus gilchristi, (G) Palinustus waguensis, (H) Panulirus ornatus and (I) Puerulus angulatus. Note that Justitia, Palinurus, and Panulirus species have a flap attaching to the posterior edge of the plectrum. Linuparus has a thick layer of tissue attaching to the posterior edge of the plectrum. One antenna of Palibythus magnificus has been removed. Specimens illustrated in B, C, D, F, I are from the National Museum of Natural History (Smithsonian Institution, Washington DC). Specimen illustrated in E is from the Queensland Museum. A, G, and H were photographed live

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f02.jpg

Fig. 3.

Measurements taken in calculation of bending strength and rasp signal features. (A) An oscillograph of a typical palinurid rasp which consists of a series of pulses. The measurements of each rasp included the duration (time finish minus time start), pulse duration as indicated on the graph, and pulse rate (number of pulses divided by rasp duration). (B) Calculation of strength in bending in a thin-walled cylinder. In lobsters, R represents the radius of the antenna and r is calculated as R minus the average thickness of the cuticle

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f03.gif

Fig. 4.

Morphological and molecular phylogenetic trees. The morphological phylogeny (A) is shown as a majority rule consensus tree with numbers indicating bootstrap values from 1000 replicates. The topology of the 18S (B), 16S (C), and 28S (D) molecular phylogenies are based on the maximum-likelihood model described in the text. The numbers above each node are from 1000 heuristic bootstrap replicates/500 (18S dataset) and 250 (16S and 28S datasets) maximum-likelihood bootstrap replicates. Below each node are consensus tree values from 1000 trees of the 1 million Bayesian replicate analysis/100 trees of the 10,000 Bayesian replicate analysis (see text for details). For the 28S dataset, the number below each node is the 1 million Bayesian replicate analysis. “n” indicates that values were below 50% for nodes in which at least one test yielded support over 50%. Empty boxes indicate Silentes palinurids and filled boxes indicate Stridentes. Scyllarus arctus is the outgroup

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f401.gif

Fig. 4.

Continued.

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f402.gif

Fig. 5.

Relationship between rasp signal features and file length. The left column includes data from a size range of Panulirus argus. The right column depicts independent contrast values from comparative analyses across the palinurids. Average rasp duration (seconds) is plotted against file length (A, B). Average pulse number increases with file length in Panulirus argus (C) whereas maximum pulse number increases with file length across palinurids (D). Average pulse rate (pulses/second) increases with file length within and across species (E, F). The contrast plots represent one tree from the 16S Bayesian analyses with a correlation coefficient closely matching the average correlation coefficient across all the trees

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f05.gif

Fig. 6.

Representative results depicting the relationships between the size of the sound producing apparatus and surrounding antennae. Independent contrast plots are taken from one representative tree from the 16S Bayesian analyses with a correlation coefficient closely matching the average correlation coefficient across all the trees. Within Stridentes, antennular plate length increases with file length (A). Antennular plate width increases with antennular plate length in Stridentes (B) and across all palinurids (not depicted). Strength in bending increases with antennular plate length in Stridentes (C). A similar positive trend occurs across all palinurids (D); the arrow points to the contrast value calculated from a comparison of the basal node of the Stridentes clade to the Silentes

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f06.gif

Fig. 7.

Species values show the distinct separation of antennular plate length and strength in bending between Silentes and Stridentes. All values have been log-transformed. Data points in (A) and (B) represent multiple individual values for each species; in (C) and (D) data points represent average values for each species. A gradeshift in strength in bending relative to body size is apparent when Silentes (solid circles, dashed regression line) are compared to Stridentes (hollow circles, solid regression line) (A). (B) Stridentes (hollow circles) have larger antennular plates than Silentes (solid circles) for a given body size. Strength in bending is greater for a given antennular plate length in Silentes than Stridentes when body size is not taken into account (C). When the effects of body size are removed through phylogenetically independent contrasts (see Methods), Silentes (solid circles) have greater I/R than Stridentes (hollow circles and triangles) for a given antennular plate size (D). Furthermore, as indicated by the horizontal line, Silentes have higher I/R than most Stridentes even when the effects of body size are removed; only the Panulirus species (Stridentes: triangles) have I/R at the levels of Silentes (D). The same patterns are observed when antennal diameter is used in place of I/R

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-f07.gif

Table 1.

Predicted size trade-offs between the sound producing and force generating antennal structures. A plus sign indicates that both parameters increase; a minus sign indicates that as one parameter increases, the other parameter decreases

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-t01.gif

Table 2.

Descriptive statistics for rasp temporal features across the palinurids. Panulirus argus values were recorded from four individuals, Palinurus elephas values were from two individuals, the remaining species were recorded from one individual

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-t02.gif

Table 3.

Statistical correlations between file length and rasp signal features. Correlations measured across a size range of Panulirus argus are labeled “intraspecific” in the table. The morphological phylogeny dataset consisted of two trees. 16S and 18S datasets used 50 Bayesian trees. Correlation coefficients are labeled as * P < 0.05 and ** P < 0.01

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-t03.gif

Table 4.

Correlations among component structures of the antennal/antennular plate complex. Morphological phylogeny consisted of twelve and four most parsimonious trees for all taxa and Stridentes, respectively. Correlation coefficients were averaged across 50 Bayesian trees in 16S and 18S datasets. Degrees of freedom (df ) is equal to one minus the number of taxa used in each analysis. Correlation coefficients are labeled as * P < 0.05 and ** P < 0.01

i0014-3820-57-9-2082-t04.gif
S. N. Patek and T. H. Oakley "COMPARATIVE TESTS OF EVOLUTIONARY TRADE-OFFS IN A PALINURID LOBSTER ACOUSTIC SYSTEM," Evolution 57(9), 2082-2100, (1 September 2003). https://doi.org/10.1554/02-608
Received: 14 October 2002; Accepted: 16 April 2003; Published: 1 September 2003
KEYWORDS
Comparative methods
constraints
functional morphology
Palinuridae
phylogeny
predation
sound production
RIGHTS & PERMISSIONS
Get copyright permission
Access provided by
Back to Top