How to translate text using browser tools
20 September 2019 Sounding Off: Relationships between Call Properties, Body Size, Phylogeny, and Laryngotracheal Form of Geckos
E. Alexander Rohtla, Anthony P. Russell, Aaron M. Bauer
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Gekkotan lizards are the most vocal of all squamates and a majority of species (exclusive of eublepharids and most diurnal geckos) are capable of producing both distress calls and more complex advertisement calls, the latter being typically restricted to males. Although the vocal characteristics of some individual species are known, this study presents the first effort to identify general patterns in vocalization and laryngotracheal morphology across a broad phylogenetic sampling of taxa. We recorded up to 10 parameters of sonographic data for 36 gecko species (25 of which were previously unsampled) in 6 of 7 gekkotan families. Advertisement calls are species and individual specific, and the dominant call frequency is negatively correlated with body size, supporting the interpretation that such calls provide information about identity and condition to conspecifics. Advertisement calls have a much narrower frequency bandwidth than distress calls, which are used to deter predators across a spectrum of auditory ranges. Features of laryngotracheal morphology, especially laryngeal size and tracheal dilation, are associated with acoustic properties of phonation, but do not exhibit a strong phylogenetic signal. Ancestral state reconstructions using parsimony and maximum-likelihood methods indicate that the gekkotan ancestor possessed a ring-shaped cricoid cartilage with distinct lateral processes and circumferentially continuous tracheal rings. With a substantial increase in the number and phylogenetic diversity of species sampled, our data largely confirm the generality of structural and functional patterns of gecko phonation previously inferred from focal studies of select taxa.

Reptiles generate a variety of sounds, such as hisses, chirps, and clicks emitted from the respiratory tract, as well as other sounds resulting from scale rubbing or other integumentary percussions (Gans and Maderson 1973). Distinct from most reptiles, however, geckos (Squamata: Gekkota), which comprise more than 1800 described species arrayed in 122 genera (Uetz et al. 2019), are known for frequency-modulated vocalizations resulting from air being expelled from the lungs and forcibly driven across elastinrich vocal cords housed within the cartilaginous larynx. Among squamates, gekkotans uniquely possess true vocal cords (Gans and Maderson 1973; Moore et al. 1991).

The structural basis for producing gekkotan vocalizations has been documented for some taxa and was described in detail for Gekko gecko by Moore et al. (1991), who laid the foundation for further comparative studies (Rittenhouse et al. 1997, 1998; Russell et al. 2000, 2014). The pyramidal gekkotan larynx lies posterior to the tongue and anterior to the trachea, with its intra-oral orifice, the aditus laryngis (glottis), positioned anteriorly and directed dorsally at rest. The main body of the larynx consists of a ring-shaped cricoid cartilage bearing two prominent lateral processes. Paired arytenoid cartilages articulate with the dorsolateral part of the cricoid, and extend anteriorly, supporting the lips of the aditus laryngis. Two skeletal muscles, the m. constrictor laryngis and m. dilator laryngis, control the orifice of the aditus laryngis, the configuration of the larynx, and the tension placed upon the vocal cords (Moore et al. 1991).

Gecko call types have been identified on the basis of their sound structure and the social context in which they are employed. The two most prominent, distress calls and advertisement calls, are of more universal distribution and form the focus of this investigation. Some species are known to emit several types of calls that are dependent on social situation, extending the call repertoire beyond the aforementioned dichotomy. Hemidactylus turcicus and Gehyra dubia possess repertoires of at least three (Marcellini 1974) and five (Phongkangsananan et al. 2014) call types, respectively. Intraspecifically, vocalizations of Ptenopus garrulus vary from population to population (Haacke 1969), suggesting that local dialects might exist as well.

Distress calls consist of a single, sometimes loud and explosive, chirp, frequently emitted when the animal is handled, or upon physical confrontation or other stress-inducing interactions (Bauer et al. 1992; Tang et al. 2001; Gramentz 2009a, 2010). Distress calls frequently contain spectral components that lie outside the frequency range of gekkotan hearing (Werner and Wever 1972; Brown 1985; Manley and Kraus 2010), and thus have been suggested to be a means of deterring non-gekkotan predators capable of hearing somewhere within the broad frequency range of the call (Frankenberg 1975; Brown 1985; Russell et al. 2014).

Advertisement calls, in contrast, are characterized by multiple similar chirps of short duration with one or more dominant frequencies. These fall within the range of maximal auditory sensitivity of geckos (Werner et al. 2008; Chen et al. 2016), are usually spontaneously emitted by males prior to the commencement of their nocturnal activity, and continue throughout the night (Tang et al. 2001; Gramentz 2010; Yu et al. 2011; Jono and Inui 2012; Chen et al. 2016). Diurnal geckos are considerably less vocal than their nocturnal relatives, and most species in the former group typically do not emit advertisement calls (Bauer 2013). Reports of female vocalizations are sparse, with the majority of known female vocalizations limited to distress calls. Female advertisement calls have been reported in only a few species, notably Gehyra dubia (Phongkangsananan et al. 2014) and all-female parthenogenetic species, such as Lepidodactylus lugubris (Zaworski 1987) and Hemidactylus garnotii (Frankenberg 1982a). Advertisement calls have been proposed to function in territoriality and/or mate attraction (Haacke 1969; Marcellini 1974; Hibbitts et al. 2007), but their precise roles remain untested.

Sonographic analysis of gecko vocalizations began with Haacke's (1969) investigation of the loud advertisement calls of Ptenopus garrulus. Since then, sporadic attention has been paid to the acoustic analysis of gecko vocalizations, generally by examining the call structure of single species in an attempt to determine the function and potential species specificity of the advertisement call. The calls of approximately 50 species—about 3% of all geckos—have been sonographically analyzed (Rittenhouse et al. 1998; Phongkangsananan et al. 2014; Appendix I).

Interspecific studies have revealed an association between the spectral frequency of vocalizations and body size, with larger species producing calls with a lower dominant frequency than smaller ones (Frankenberg 1975; Weber and Werner 1977; Brown 1985; Bauer et al. 1992). This relationship between call frequency and body size, however, has not been tested across a broad phylogenetic spectrum. Negative size-based relationships have also been reported intraspecifically. The dominant call frequencies of both Ptenopus garrulus garrulus (Hibbitts et al. 2007) and Gehyra dubia (Phongkangsananan et al. 2014) are negatively proportional to body size and, specific to P. g. garrulus, the best indicator of breeding success, suggesting that it might serve as an indicator of fitness (Hibbitts et al. 2007).

In light of the foregoing information we conducted a comparative study of gecko vocalizations. We combined the examination of distress and advertisement calls with an exploration of the morphology of vocal structure across a variety of taxonomic groups and size ranges, including the first sonographic analyses of vocalizations for several species. Phylogenetic comparisons of the vocal apparatus and hypotheses relating to the evolution of complex vocal anatomy and behavior (Rittenhouse et al. 1998; Russell et al. 2000) have previously been difficult to test because of poorly resolved gekkotan phylogenies. We employed a recently published, robust phylogeny of the gekkotan lineage (Gamble et al. 2015) to examine the evolution of laryngotracheal morphology across the Gekkota. We formulated five hypotheses for testing, as follows:

Hypothesis 1: The dominant frequency of advertisement calls is negatively proportional to body size. This contention is predicated upon the findings of Hibbitts et al. (2007) who showed that larger males of Ptenopus g. garrulus generate lower frequency calls than do smaller males, and of others who have noted this in interspecific comparisons (Frankenberg 1975; Weber and Werner 1977; Brown 1985; Bauer et al. 1992). This hypothesis has not previously been explored across a broad phylogenetic spectrum.

Hypothesis 2: Advertisement calls are used for intraspecific communication, whereas distress calls are used for interspecific communication. If this is true, we expect the frequency bandwidth (the range of frequencies over which a call transmits) to be greater for distress calls than advertisement calls. We further expect that the frequency bandwidth extends beyond the known range of gecko hearing in distress calls, but is restricted to it in advertisement calls.

Hypothesis 3: Advertisement calls are species specific. It has long been proposed that advertisement calls serve to attract mates (Marcellini 1974). We predict, therefore, that the advertisement calls of each species will contain unique acoustic properties and/or combinations of properties.

Hypothesis 4: Variability in laryngotracheal morphology is associated with acoustic properties of phonation. Rittenhouse et al. (1998) documented the highly derived and enlarged larynx of Ptenopus garrulus and asserted this to be the source of its exceptional vocal abilities. Later, Russell et al. (2014) proposed that the expanded chamber at the anteriormost region of the trachea of Uroplatus facilitates the production of loud, explosive distress calls. The relationship between vocal form and function has not been explored across a wide phylogenetic spectrum. With the use of qualitative comparisons and statistically based comparisons of quantitative measurements of call parameters and laryngotracheal morphology, we test whether correlations exist between vocal structures and acoustic properties.

Hypothesis 5: Elements of the morphology of the larynx and trachea can be used as reliable phylogenetic markers. We employ a recently constructed, robust phylogeny of the gekkotan lineage (Gamble et al. 2015), and trace laryngotracheal character evolution to test this.

Materials and Methods

Call Recording

Gecko vocalizations were recorded for 36 gecko species (Table 1) at a private gecko breeding facility near Tulsa, Oklahoma, USA from 10–21 June 2013. We subsequently omitted calls of the parthenogenetic Lepidodactylus lugubris, so our analyses of vocalizations are thus based upon 35 species. The ambient temperature was maintained at 28.3°C. Lighting was controlled by automatic timers. Sunrise and sunset were simulated by a 1-h period of graded light intensity in the morning and evening. The first lights turned on at about 0730 h and off at around 1930 h.

Table 1

Gekkotan species examined for this study, as listed by family: Py = Pygopodidae; C = Carphodactylidae; D = Diplodactylidae; E = Eublepharidae; S = Sphaerodactylidae; Ph = Phyllodactylidae; G = Gekkonidae. Species denoted by a pound sign (#) are those for which both advertisement and distress calls were recorded. Filled dots (•) represent adult males—these data were used to test hypotheses; open dots (*) represent females or juveniles—these data were not used in hypothesis testing (apart from the ancestral state reconstructions [ASR] for Lepidodactylus lugubris). Asterisks (*) in Columns 1 and 2 indicate taxa for which sonographic descriptions of advertisement and/or distress calls are provided for the first time. Columns numbered 1–12 indicate the species examined in the various parts of the study; the column headings referring to the five hypotheses tested, or parts thereof, as follows: 1 H1 AC DCF = Hypothesis 1, advertisement call, dominant call frequency; 2 H1 DC DCF = Hypothesis 1, distress call, dominant call frequency; 3 H2 AC FB = Hypothesis 2, advertisement call, frequency bandwidth; 4 H2 DC FB = Hypothesis 2, distress call, frequency bandwidth; 5 H3 AC = Hypothesis 3, species specificity of advertisement calls; 6 H3 AC Pac = species specificity of advertisement calls within the genus Pachydactylus; 7 H3 AC Cor = individual specificity of advertisement calls for three individuals of Correlophus ciliatus; 8 H3 AC Lim = species specificity of advertisement calls limited to only those parameters also used for distress call analysis; 9 H3 DC = species specificity of distress calls; 10 H4 AC = Hypothesis 4, correlations between laryngotracheal morphology and acoustic properties of advertisement calls; 11 H4 DC = Hypothesis 4, correlations between laryngotracheal morphology and acoustic properties of distress calls; 12 H5 ASR = Hypothesis 5, ancestral state reconstruction of laryngotracheal characters.

img-z3-2_175.gif

To collect advertisement calls, Zoom H2n handy recorders (Zoom North America) were placed in front of, atop, or inside the enclosures of identified callers and recorded through all, or part of, the night. One of us (EAR) was present at the facility from about 1900 to 0000 h each evening to identify each calling gecko.

Distress calls were recorded by day to minimize disrupting the nighttime activity of geckos, when most species produce advertisement calls. To record distress calls, we removed geckos from their enclosures by hand and stimulated them to vocalize by either tapping the snout (Bauer et al. 1992), pinching the toes (Brown 1985), and/or gently squeezing the body (Weber and Werner 1977), the latter approach being the most effective. Not all individuals emitted distress calls when prompted, but if they did calls were generally emitted within the first few seconds of capture. Individuals were captured and prompted to emit distress calls on several occasions, although never more than once per day.

Acoustic Analyses

Raven Pro v1.4 (Bioacoustic Research Program, Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY) was used for the spectral and temporal analysis of vocalizations. Adjusting contrast, color, and other visual settings in Raven Pro can result in inconsistent measurement of call durations and frequencies. To compensate for this, individual chirps were measured at the 5 and 95% intervals of sound energy for duration and frequency data (Charif et al. 2010). The following parameters were recorded, wherever possible, for all advertisement calls: number of phases (i.e., number of different chirp types), number of chirps per call, call duration (s), chirp duration at 90% of sound energy (s), interchirp duration (s), dominant chirp frequency at the 5th and 95th percentiles of sound energy (kHz; hereafter referred to as chirp frequency at 5% of sound energy [kHz] and chirp frequency at 95% of sound energy [kHz], respectively), chirp frequency bandwidth (kHz), and maximum power (dB; Gramentz 2009a; Tang et al. 2001; Yu et al. 2011; Jono and Inui 2012; Phongkangsananan et al. 2014; Chen et al. 2016).

Morphological Analyses

Digital photographs of each individual for which recordings were made were taken in dorsal view to ensure correct species identification. Individuals were placed in sealable plastic bags and mass was recorded with the use of a tared spring scale. Snout–vent length (SVL), head length (HL), head width (HW), and head depth (HD) were measured with the use of a ruler or calipers.

Adult specimens of 30 gekkotan species (Appendix II) that had been formalin-fixed and preserved in 70% ethanol were obtained from museum collections and their throat regions dissected (Table 1, Column 12). Of these species, 22 were represented in our recorded vocalizations (Table 1, Columns 1–11 vs. 12). A nonvocal outgroup species, Anolis carolinensis (Reptilia: Sauria: Dactyloidae), was also dissected for comparative purposes. Twelve species for which recordings were made were unavailable for dissection (Table 1, Columns 1–11 vs. 12). Specimens were dissected from six species for which no calls were recorded in this study (Table 1, Columns 1–11 vs. 12). In these instances, calls of congeners were recorded in this study (e.g., Gekko smithii was dissected because calls of G. vittatus and G. siamensis were recorded in this study, etc.) or descriptions of their calls (e.g., Delma fraseri and H. frenatus) have been published elsewhere (Gramentz 2010; Manley and Kraus 2010).

Larynges of museum specimens were examined in situ, and subsequently portions of the trachea and tongue were excised and extraneous tissue removed following the procedures of Rittenhouse et al. (1997, 1998) and Russell et al. (2000, 2014). Pertinent anatomy was examined and photographed using a Nikon SMZ1000 dissecting microscope with attached Nikon Digital Sight DS-U2 camera (Nikon Inc., Tokyo, Japan) and a metric rule. The following morphological measurements of the entire specimen and parts thereof were recorded using NIS-Elements D 3.1 (Nikon Inc., Tokyo, Japan; Table 2): SVL, HL, HD, HW, rostrum-aditus length (RAL), larynx length (LL), larynx width (LW), anterior tracheal width (ATW), posterior tracheal width (PTW), and anterior tracheal ring width (ATRW).

Table 2

Morphological measurements (reported as means, in mm) for lizard species examined. n = number of specimens examined per species. ATRW = anterior tracheal ring width; ATW = width of the anterior region of the trachea; GLL = length of the glottal lips; HD = head depth; HL = head length; HW = head width, taken at the widest point of the head; LL = anteroposterior length of the larynx; LW = mediolateral width of the larynx; PTW = width of the posterior region of the trachea; RAL = rostrum–aditus length; SVL = snout–vent length.

img-z5-2_175.gif

Hypothesis Testing

All statistical analyses were carried out using JMP v11.0 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). We examined all data for normality of distribution using the Shapiro–Wilk W test. For our analyses we included only calls for which all parameters could be measured. The values of all chirps in a call were averaged. Only adult male calls were used; female and juvenile calls were omitted. Morphological values were averaged per species. Sampling across the Gekkota as a whole remains poor and our questions seek to reveal lineage-wide trends. Thus, hypotheses relating to sonographic properties of calls were not analyzed using phylogenetic comparative methods.

To test Hypothesis 1, that call frequency is negatively correlated to body size, we employed two Model I regressions, one for advertisement calls and the other for distress calls, using SVL as the independent variable and average dominant call frequency (DCF) per species as the dependent variable. We used data from 17 species (2 diplodactylids; 1 phyllodactylid; 14 gekkonids; Table 1, Column 1) to determine whether the DCF of the advertisement calls is correlated with body size. We used data from 23 species (2 diplodactylids; 2 eublepharids; 1 sphaerodactylid; 1 phyllodactylid; 17 gekkonids; Table 1, Column 2) to determine whether the DCF of distress calls is correlated with body size.

To test Hypothesis 2, that advertisement calls are used for intraspecific communication and distress calls used for interspecific communication, we performed a one-tailed, unpaired t-test to compare frequency bandwidth (FB) of 285 advertisement calls recorded from 17 species and FB from 260 distress calls recorded from 23 species (Table 1, Columns 3, 4). For the eight species for which both advertisement and distress calls were recorded (Ptenopus carpi, P. kochi, Afroedura namaquensis, A. loveridgei, Chondrodactylus fitzsimonsi, Pachydactylus parascutatus, P. visseri, Hemidactylus turcicus), a one-tailed, paired t-test was conducted to compare average FB per species of both call types. Calls from only one individual were used when more than one individual per species was recorded. We used a nonparametric Mann–Whitney U to test for significant differences between the groups if the data were not normally distributed.

To test Hypothesis 3, that advertisement calls are species specific, we conducted principal-component analyses (PCAs) followed by discriminant function analyses (DFAs) for both advertisement and distress calls. In the first analysis, we tested for species specificity using 285 advertisement calls from 17 species (Table 1, Column 5) and 19 individuals (three Correlophus ciliatus) using the following parameters: number of phases, number of chirps, call duration (CaD), chirp duration (ChD), DCF, call frequency at 5% of the sound energy (CF5%), call frequency at 95% of the sound energy (CF95%), and maximum power (MP). In the second analysis, we tested for species specificity within the genus Pachydactylus using 127 advertisement calls from five species (Table 1, Column 6). Call structure was complex and fairly conserved for all Pachydactylus spp. recorded, allowing for more parameters to be analyzed than for the previous analysis, which employed calls from a wide range of species with highly varied call structure. The following parameters were used in the Pachydactylus analysis: number of chirps, CaD, intercall duration (ICaD), ChD, interchirp duration (IChD), CF5%, CF95%, DCF, and MP. In the third analysis we tested for individual specificity of the advertisement calls of Correlophus ciliatus (Table 1, Column 7) with the use of 56 advertisement calls from three individuals with the following parameters: number of chirps, CaD, ChD, IChD, CF5%, CF95%, DCF, and MP. In the fourth analysis, we tested for species specificity using 285 advertisement calls from 19 individuals of 17 species (Table 1, Column 8) by limiting the number of parameters to only those used for the distress-call analysis: ChD, CF5%, CF95%, DCF, and MP. In the fifth analysis, we tested for species specificity using 260 distress calls from 23 species (the same species examined for distress-call data in Hypothesis 1, above; Table 1, Column 9) using the following call parameters: ChD, CF5%, CF95%, DCF, and MP.

To test Hypothesis 4, that variability in laryngotracheal morphology is associated with acoustic properties of phonation, we ran a series of Model I regressions using measurements of the larynx and trachea as independent variables and acoustic measurements as dependent variables. Exploration of possible correlations between laryngotracheal morphology and the acoustic properties of advertisement calls employed data from 13 species (2 diplodactylids; 1 phyllodactylid; 10 gekkonids; Table 1, Column 10). We used data from 14 species (2 diplodactylids; 1 eublepharid; 1 sphaerodactylid; 1 phyllodactylid; 9 gekkonids; Table 1, Column 11) to investigate correlations between laryngotracheal morphology and acoustic properties of distress calls.

Three males of Correlophus ciliatus were recorded, so we averaged their acoustic and morphological values for each parameter. We log-transformed nonnormally distributed data sets until they were normally distributed, or used a nonparametric Spearman's rank correlation, as appropriate. The following measurements and proportions taken from preserved specimens were used as independent variables following Russell et al. (2014): LL, LW, GLL, larynx shape (LW/LL), relative larynx length (SVL/LL), relative larynx width (HW/LW), laryngeal position (SVL/RAL), relative anterior trachea width (SVL/ATW), relative posterior trachea width (SVL/PTW), trachea shape (ATW/PTW), relative tracheal ring width (SVL/ATRW), relative length of glottal lips (GLL/LL), and relative width of glottal lips (GLL/LW). The following acoustic measurements were used as dependent variables: DCF, CF5%, CF95%, FB, and MP.

To test Hypothesis 5, that the morphology of the larynx and trachea can be used as reliable phylogenetic markers, we performed ancestral state reconstructions (ASRs) of 7 gross morphological laryngotracheal characters from the 30 species for which museum specimens were examined (Table 1, Column 12) and one outgroup species, Anolis carolinensis. The characters and character states were derived from the gross morphological observations of the laryngotracheal morphology of Afro-Malagasy geckos of Russell et al. (2000). The following morphological characters were examined: (1) shape of the cricoid (ring, plate-like, or rounded), (2) orientation of the lateral process of the cricoid (anterolateral, lateral, or posterolateral), (3) shape of the trachea (uniform diameter, tapered, grooved, or dilated), (4) continuity of tracheal rings (continuous or not continuous), (5) orientation of the ventral fibers of the m. constrictor laryngis relative to the lateral processes of the cricoid (anterolateral, lateral, posterolateral, or fan-like), (6) fusion of the cricoid with the first tracheal ring (fused or not fused), and (7) relative size of the m. constrictor laryngis (small, moderate, or large; Table 3).

Table 3

Character matrix used for the ancestral state reconstruction of laryngotracheal morphology in gekkotans (outgroup = Anolis carolinensis). (a) Cricoid shape: (0) ring, (1) plate-like, (2) rounded. (b) Projection angle of the lateral processes of the cricoid cartilage: (0) anterolateral, (1) lateral, (2) posterolateral. (c) Trachea shape: (0) uniform diameter, (1) tapered, (2) grooved, (3) dilated. (d) Continuity of tracheal rings: (0) continuous, (1) not continuous. (e) Fusion of the cricoid and first tracheal ring(s): (0) fused, (1) not fused, (?) unknown. (f) Orientation of ventral fibers of m. constrictor laryngis: (0) anterolaterally, (1) laterally, (2) posterolaterally, (3) fan-like, (?) unknown. (g) Relative size of the m. constrictor laryngis: (0) small, (1) moderate, (2) large.

img-z6-2_175.gif

We performed ASRs in Mesquite v3.04 (Maddison and Maddison 2015) and traced the character states using the Trace Character History function across a recently published phylogeny that was pruned to include only the taxa for which laryngotracheal data were collected. Both parsimony and ML methods were used to trace character evolution. The parsimony ASRs incorporated an unordered states assumption, which counts one step for any character state change. A Markov k-state one-parameter model was incorporated for the ML ASRs which assumes that any particular change is equally probable.

The phylogeny used is a time-calibrated phylogeny incorporating representatives of 98% (120) of gekkotan genera estimated from a Bayesian analysis of the concatenated gene data set based upon the analysis of five nuclear genes and one mitochondrial fragment (Gamble et al. 2015). Most species, including the outgroup, and all gekkotan genera examined in this study are included in this tree.

Results

Calls

Advertisement and distress calls.—Advertisement calls were recorded for 19 males representing 17 species, 10 of which are sonographically presented for the first time (Table 1, Column 1 symbols with asterisks; 1 diplodactylid; 9 gekkonids; Table 4; Fig. 1). Most of these are monophasic (i.e., consist of one type of chirp), but Oedura marmorata, Hemidactylus turcicus, and Ptenopus garrulus emitted two chirp types, Chondrodactylus fitzsimonsi three types of chirps in three distinct calls, and Stenodactylus sthenodactylus produced four chirp types with a repertoire of three calls (Fig. 2). Most advertisement calls consisted of temporally discrete, individually countable chirps, although a few incorporated temporally fused, trilled chirps that were not segregable into discretely countable individual notes. Distress calls were recorded for 27 species and 31 individuals, of which those of 18 species are sonographically presented for the first time (Table 1, Column 2 symbols with asterisks; 1 diplodactylid; 2 eublepharids; 1 sphaerodactylid; 14 gekkonids; Table 5; Fig. 3).

Table 4

Mean and range values for advertisement calls of gekkotan species recorded in this study (see Table 1 for family designations). For species with a repertoire of calls, each call type is reported separately and denoted in parentheses. n = number of calls (if more than one individual was recorded the number of individuals is given in parentheses; see Fig. 2 for descriptions of call types). CaD = call duration; CF5% = call frequency at 5% of the sound energy; CF95% = call frequency at 95% of the sound energy; ChD = chirp duration; DCF = dominant call frequency; FB = frequency bandwidth; ICaD = intercall duration; IChD = interchirp duration; MP = maximum power.

img-z7-2_175.gif

Table 5

Mean and range values for distress calls of gekkotan species recorded in this study (see Table 1 for family designations). n = number of calls (if more than one individual was recorded the number of individuals is denoted in parentheses). CaD = call duration; CF5% = call frequency at 5% of the sound energy; CF95% = call frequency at 95% of the sound energy; DCF = dominant call frequency; FB = frequency bandwidth; J = juvenile; MP = maximum power.

img-z10-2_175.jpg

Fig. 1

Sonograms of advertisement calls from gekkotan species that are depicted for the first time (asterisks in Table 1, Column 1). Species are presented in the same order as in Table 1. (a) Correlophus ciliatus, (b) Afroedura namaquensis, (c) A. loveridgei, (d) Chondrodactylus fitzsimonsi, (e) Pachydactylus parascutatus, (f) P. scutatus, (g) P. montanus, (h) P. weberi, (i) P. visseri, (j) Microgecko persicus. Scales along the x-axes are not identical for all panels; all y-axes range from 0 to 20 kHz.

img-z8-1_175.jpg

Fig. 2

Advertisement calls of gekkotan species possessing polyphasic calls or call repertoires of more than one call type. (a) Oedura marmorata, (b) Hemidactylus turcicus, and (c) Ptenopus garrulus produced two types of chirp (labeled 1 and 2). (d) Chondrodactylus fitzsimonsi produced three call types, each possessing unique chirps, (di) a low-frequency “whoop” or bark, (dii) squeals, and (diii) low-frequency chirps of shorter chirp and interchirp duration than the previous call. (e) Stenodactylus sthenodactylus emitted four call types, represented by (ei) the most common emission consisting of 2–4 chirps, (eii) isolated chirps from the second call type, (eiii) entire call of the second call type, (eiv) polyphasic call. Scales along the x-axes are not identical for all panels; all y-axes range from 0 to 20 kHz.

img-z9-1_175.jpg

Fig. 3

Sonograms of distress calls from gekkotan species that are depicted for the first time (asterisks in Table 1, Column 2). Species are presented in the same order as listed in Table 1. (a) Oedura monilis male, (b) Correlophus ciliatus female, (c) Coleonyx variegatus male, (d) Goniurosaurus orientalis (juvenile), (e) Eublepharis hardwickii male, (f) Teratoscincus scincus male, (g) Afroedura namaquensis male, (h) Afroedura loveridgei male, (i) Homopholis wahlbergii male, (j) Chondrodactylus angulifer female, (k) Chondrodactylus angulifer male, (l) Chondrodactylus fitzsimonsi male, (m) Pachydactylus gaiasensis female, (n) Pachydactylus gaiasensis male, (o) Pachydactylus parascutatus male, (p) Pachydactylus visseri male, (q) Gekko vittatus male, (r) Gekko siamensis female, (s) Agamura persica male, (t) Cyrtodactylus pulchellus male, (u) Hemidactylus boavistensis male, (v) Hemidactylus ruspolii male, (w) Hemidactylus triedrus male.

img-z11-1_175.jpg

Frequency–size relationships of the calls.—Dominant call frequency (DCF) of advertisement call (W = 0.91, P = 0.11) and SVL (W = 0.92, P = 0.13) data were normally distributed, and exhibit a negative relationship (DCF = 5.36–0.04 × SVL, R2 = 0.55, P < 0.001; Fig. 4a). No correlation was detected for the DCF of distress calls (P = 0.59).

Fig. 4

Frequency characteristics of gecko calls. (a) Dominant call frequency of advertisement calls exhibits a negative relationship to SVL (P < 0.001; see Table 1, Column 1 symbols with asterisks). (b) Frequency bandwidth (kHz; reported as means ± 1 SE) of advertisement calls compared to distress calls for all species examined (bi; P < 0.001), and for the eight species for which both call types were recorded (bii; P = 0.02). See Table 1, Columns 3–4, and species names with pound signs (#).

img-z12-1_175.jpg

Frequency bandwidth of the calls.—Advertisement call data for all 17 species examined (Table 1, Column 1) were not normally distributed (n = 257, Shapiro–Wilk W = 0.89, P < 0.001), so a nonparametric Mann–Whitney U test was used. The distributions of the advertisement calls and the distress calls (Table 1, Column 2) differ significantly; the ranked sum mean frequency bandwidth was 199 for advertisement calls and 354 for distress calls (DF = 543, U = 11.45, P < 0.001; Fig. 4 bi). Distress calls have a greater frequency bandwidth (x̄ ± 1 SE = 7.69 ± 0.28 kHz) than do advertisement calls (x̄ = 3.18 ± 0.08 kHz) when tested using data for the eight species for which both call types were recorded (DF = 7, t = 2.84, P = 0.02; Table 1; Fig. 4bii).

Specificity of advertisement and distress calls.—In the analysis of advertisement calls for the 17 species for which these are available (Table 1, Column 5), the first two principal components account for 54.0% of the total variance (PC1 = 33.2% and PC2 = 20.8%). The discriminant function analysis (DFA) correctly classified 92% of all advertisement calls to the calling species (Fig. 5a). For the analysis of the advertisement calls of the five species of Pachydactylus (Table 1, Column 6), the first two principal components accounted for 54.6% of the total variance (PC1 = 39.9% and PC2 = 14.7%), and the DFA correctly classified 100% of all advertisement calls to the calling species (Fig. 5b). For the three individuals of Correlophus ciliatus (Table 1, Column 7) the first two principal components accounted for 49.9% of the total variance (PC1 = 28.6% and PC2 = 21.1%), and the DFA correctly classified 95% of all advertisement calls to the calling individual (Fig. 5c).

Fig. 5

Discriminant function analysis (DFA) of gecko calls in relation to call specificity. Encompassing circles represent the 95% confidence limit containing the true mean for that group. The scale of axes for panels a, b, and d are identical, but differ from those used in panels c and e (which are identical to each other); the legend for panels a, b, and d is also the same. (a) For the 17 species analyzed for 7 parameters of the advertisement call (Table 1, Column 5), the DFA correctly assigned 261 of 285 calls (92%) to the calling species. (b) With the use of 9 advertisement call parameters for 5 species of Pachydactylus (Table 1, Column 6), the DFA correctly assigned 127 of 127 advertisement calls (100%) to the calling species. (c) Based upon 8 advertisement call parameters for 3 individuals of Correlophus ciliatus (Table 1, Column 7), the DFA correctly assigned 53 of 56 advertisement calls (95%) to the calling individual. (d) For the 17 species analyzed for call parameters of the advertisement call, the DFA correctly assigned 248 of 285 advertisement calls (87%) when using only the call parameters used in the distress call DFA (Table 1, Column 8). (e) Based upon 5 call parameters of the distress call for males of 23 species (the same as those used for the advertisement call in panel [d]), the DFA correctly assigned 133 of 260 distress calls (51%) to the calling species. Al = Afroedura loveridgei; An = Afroedura namaquensis; Ap = Agamura persica; Ca = Chondrodactylus angulifer; Cc = Correlophus ciliatus; Cf = Chondrodactylus fitzsimonsi; Cp = Cyrtodactylus pulchellus; Cv = Coleonyx variegatus; Eh = Eublepharis hardwickii; Gv = Gekko vittatus; Hb = Hemidactylus boavistensis; Hr = Hemidactylus ruspolii; Htr = Hemidactylus triedrus; Htu = Hemidactylus turcicus; Hw = Homopholis wahlbergii; Mp = Microgecko persicus; Oma = Oedura marmorata; Omo = Oedura monilis; Pc = Ptenopus carpi; Pg = Pachydactylus gaiasensis; Pga = Ptenopus garrulus; Pgu = Ptyodactylus guttatus; Ph = Ptyodactylus hasselquistii; Pm = Pachydactylus montanus; Pp = Pachydactylus parascutatus; Ps = Pachydactylus scutatus; Pk = Ptenopus kochi; Pv = Pachydactylus visseri; Pw = Pachydactylus weberi; Rl = Rhacodactylus leachianus; Ss = Stenodactylus sthenodactylus; Ts = Teratoscincus scincus.

img-z14-1_175.jpg

When employing only the parameters used for the analysis of distress calls for 17 species (Table 1, Column 8), the first two principal components accounted for 67.1% of the total variance in advertisement calls (PC1 = 42.5% and PC2 = 24.6%), and the DFA correctly classified 87% of these advertisement calls to the calling species (Fig. 5d). For the distress calls of the 23 species recorded (Table 1, Column 9), when using the same five parameters examined for the advertisement calls of 17 species (Table 1, Column 8), the first two principal components accounted for 66.7% of the total variance (PC1 = 42.7% and PC2 = 24.0%), and the DFA correctly classified 51% of all distress calls to the calling species (Fig. 5e).

Laryngotracheal Morphology

We provide descriptions of the muscles and skeleton of the larynx, and the form of the trachea for an exemplar gecko, Hemidactylus turcicus, which is geographically widespread and highly vocal. These are compared to an outgroup species Anolis carolinensis (Reptilia: Sauria: Dactyloidae) which is a model organism for reptilian studies (Alföldi et al. 2011) and of comparable size to many geckos (SVL < 75 mm; Jensen 2008). Anolis has been reported to emit sounds comparable to gekkotan distress calls, but these are not regarded as being true vocalizations which pass through vocal cords and are relatively ineffective social signals (Milton and Jenssen 1979). To our knowledge, sounds that could be categorized as having a social function have not been reported for A. carolinensis.

These descriptions serve as a baseline for the comparisons made in this study, and augment those of Moore et al. (1991), Rittenhouse et al. (1997, 1998), and Russell et al. (2000, 2014) who provided anatomical descriptions of at least some part of the laryngotracheal anatomy of geckos. The primary attributes explored in this comparison (see Fig. 6) are the basic shape of the cricoid cartilage; the form the lateral processes of the cricoid cartilage; size of the glottal lips; size, origin, and insertion of the m. constrictor laryngis and m. dilator laryngis; occurrence of fusion of the cricoid with the first tracheal ring; and the presence of a cricoid fenestra. The general shape of the arytenoid cartilages is not easily discerned through dissection and is more clearly assessed through three-dimensional reconstruction from serial sections (Russell et al. 2000), which was not undertaken in this study.

Fig. 6

The larynx and trachea of male Hemidactylus turcicus (a,b) and Anolis carolinensis (c,d) in dorsal (a,c) and ventral (b,d) views. Abbreviations: ac, arytenoid cartilage; ad, aditus laryngis; cc, cricoid cartilage; gl, glottal lips; itrs, intertracheal ring space; lp, lateral process of cricoid cartilage; mcl, m. constrictor laryngis; mdl, m. dilator laryngis; tr, tracheal ring. Scale bar = 1 mm.

img-z15-1_175.jpg

Description of laryngeal structure.—The cricoid cartilage of Anolis carolinensis is round or ovoid in shape, with very small (essentially absent, as determined by examination of cleared and stained specimens) lateral processes that are not visible through the laryngeal musculature (Fig. 6c,d). Large paired, laterally curved arytenoids are clearly evident along the anterior surface of the cricoid and flare out anteriorly to circumscribe the aditus laryngis. The flat m. constrictor laryngis originates from the ventral midline of the cricoid and its fibers spread laterally in a fan-like pattern, to the lateral aspect of the cricoid. The m. dilator laryngis is extremely small and lies superficial to its ipsilateral m. constrictor laryngis, which originates at the posterolateral end of the larynx and runs along the lateral surface of the cricoid to insert on the arytenoid cartilage. Glottal lips were not readily observed. The cricoid is not fused with the first tracheal ring. There is no cricoid fenestra.

In comparison to Anolis, the cricoid cartilage of geckos bears hypertrophied lateral processes. The cricoid cartilage of H. turcicus is shaped similarly to that of most geckos for which descriptions have been furnished, being broad and ring-shaped, from which the dorsoventrally flattened lateral processes project (Fig. 6a,b). The anterior margin of each lateral process extends directly laterally from the cricoid, whereas the posterior margin extends anterolaterally, resulting in the lateral process having a shape resembling a right triangle with a rounded distal extremity. The paired arytenoid cartilages articulate anteriorly with the cricoid and circumscribe the aditus laryngis, the opening of which is situated along the anterodorsal surface of the cricoid. Each of the paired constrictor laryngis muscles is large, with its parallel fibers coursing laterally from their origin on the processus lingualis of the basihyoid (located ventral to the cricoid), around the anterior edge of the lateral process to insert on the dorsal midline of the central ring of the cricoid. Each of the paired m. dilator laryngis muscles lies superficial to its ipsilateral m. constrictor laryngis, originating at the apex of the lateral process of the cricoid, running along its anterior edge and inserting on the arytenoid cartilage. This muscle is much more conspicuous than that of A. carolinensis. The glottal lips of H. turcicus are inconspicuous. The cricoid fenestra is absent.

Noteworthy variations in cricoid structure among geckos.—Departures from the form of the cricoid cartilage exhibited by Hemidactylus turcicus are sporadically distributed among the taxa examined, with no obviously identifiable familial trends. The features of the cricoid that exhibit the greatest variation are the shape of the cricoid fenestra, the form of its central ring, and the orientation and shape of the lateral processes.

The dorsal cricoid fenestra of Chondrodactylus fitzsimonsi is square in outline and is deeply indented within the cricoid and demarcated by rounded edges. A similar, although relatively smaller and less deeply indented, dorsal cricoid fenestra is present in Gekko smithii. The dorsal cricoid fenestra of Ptyodactylus guttatus is rectangular, but it is small and triangular in P. togoensis. In Stenodactylus sthenodactylus the dorsal cricoid fenestra is reduced to a shallow midline groove.

In Lepidodactylus lugubris, the dorsal cricoid fenestra is teardrop shaped, with the apex directed posteriorly. A similar form is evident in the sphaerodactylid Teratoscincus scincus. In the eublepharid Goniurosaurus luii the dorsal cricoid fenestra is more ovoid in outline. In all three of these species, the dorsal cricoid fenestra extends posteriorly along the trachea, resulting in incomplete fusion of the anteriormost tracheal rings.

In the eublepharid Coleonyx variegatus, the central ring of the cricoid is slightly inflated posteriorly and is fused with the first tracheal ring. The anterior and posterior borders of its lateral processes deviate from being normal to the sagittal axis of the larynx, endowing them with a tapering form. This contrasts with the lateral processes of H. turcicus, in which only the posterior margin of the lateral process deviates from a line normal to the sagittal axis of the larynx. Ventrally, the cricoid ring of C. variegatus bears a conspicuous longitudinal groove that extends posteriorly onto the first few tracheal rings and houses the processus lingualis of the basihyoid.

The lateral processes of the cricoid of the carphodactylid Underwoodisaurus milii extend posterolaterally with respect to the sagittal axis of the cricoid, a condition unique among the species examined.

All three species of Ptenopus examined exhibit enlarged cricoid cartilages with enlarged lateral processes. In dorsal view the central ring of the cricoid extends posteriorly from the lateral processes to form a flat, triangular plate that sits atop its base (as noted for P. garrulus by Russell et al. 2000). In Stenodactylus sthenodactylus, the central ring of the cricoid also exhibits a highly differentiated shape, being expanded anteriorly and thus resembling a funnel, possibly as a result of an insensible fusion of the cricoid with the first few tracheal rings. The arytenoid cartilages of this species do not cover the majority of the central ring of the cricoid. The form of the cricoid of Agamura persica and Goniurosaurus luii is similar to that of S. sthenodactylus, although less markedly expanded.

Description of tracheal structure.—The trachea extends posteriorly from the larynx and its primary morphological variations concern its overall shape and the continuity of the tracheal rings. Tracheal morphology is similar for Anolis carolinensis and Hemidactylus turcicus, and in both species is of generally uniform diameter and supported by complete tracheal rings (Fig. 6). One notable difference, however, is the relatively greater robustness and anteroposterior width of the tracheal rings of A. carolinensis compared to its intertracheal ring spaces. The tracheal rings are of uniform width and are uninterrupted around their circumference. Tracheal morphology is relatively conserved in most of the gecko species examined, with it being either of uniform width or gently tapering anteriorly.

Noteworthy variations in tracheal structure among geckos.—In a few species highly deviant tracheal morphologies are evident in comparison to that described above. Such departures—like those noted above for the cricoid cartilage—are sporadically distributed across the taxa examined, and are not clustered in discrete regions of the phylogeny. Apart from those noted below, most species have complete tracheal rings throughout the length of the trachea. The dorsal cricoid fenestra extends posteriorly in Lepidodactylus lugubris, Goniurosaurus luii, and Teratoscincus scincus, which produces an incomplete fusion of the anteriormost tracheal rings with the cricoid. Ptyodactylus guttatus exhibits a high degree of branching of the tracheal rings, a feature only occasionally encountered in other species, although not evident in the congeneric P. togoensis. The tracheal folding in Ptenopus garrulus (Russell et al. 2000) was also observed in our dissected material.

The trachea of Chondrodactylus angulifer is markedly tapered, having a vastly greater anterior, compared to posterior, width, with the tracheal rings being incomplete dorsally in the expanded anterior region (cf. Russell et al. 2000), comprising the so-called “tracheal zipper” (Busing 1990). The trachea of C. fitzsimonsi also exhibits this tapering form, but funnels back to a narrower diameter just posterior to its junction with the cricoid. Similar tracheal tapering is also evident in Oedura marmorata, although in this species the trachea is more dilated. Only a slight tapering is evident in O. monilis.

Relationships Between Laryngotracheal Morphology and Phonation

Statistically significant relationships between laryngotracheal morphology and acoustic properties of advertisement calls were found for the following (Fig. 7; see  Table S1 (175_herp-75-03-06_s01.docx) in the Supplemental Materials available online): dominant call frequency and larynx length (Fig. 7a); dominant call frequency and larynx width (Fig. 7b); glottal lip length and dominant call frequency (Fig. 7c); call frequency at 5% of sound energy and larynx length (Fig. 7d); call frequency at 5% of sound energy and larynx width (Fig. 7e); glottal lip length and call frequency at 5% of sound energy (Fig. 7f); larynx shape (LW/LL) and maximum power (Fig. 7g); relative larynx width (HW/LW) and call frequency at 95% of sound energy (Fig. 7h); relative anterior trachea width (SVL/ ATW) and dominant call frequency (Fig. 7i); relative posterior trachea width (SVL/PTW) and call frequency at 5% of sound energy (Fig. 7j); relative posterior trachea width (SVL/PTW) and call frequency at 95% of sound energy (Fig. 7k); call frequency at 5% of sound energy and tracheal shape (ATW/PTW; when P. kochi is excluded; Fig. 7l).

Fig. 7

Features of laryngotracheal morphology for which there are statistically significant relationships with acoustic properties of advertisement calls (see Table 1, Column 10). Data for those features of laryngotracheal morphology that were not normally distributed were log-transformed prior to comparison with acoustic call properties, or a nonparametric Spearman's rank correlation was used, as appropriate. Detailed statistical results are provided in  Table S1 (175_herp-75-03-06_s01.docx) of the Supplemental Materials available online.

img-z16-1_175.jpg

Fewer significant relationships were found between laryngotracheal morphology and the acoustic properties of distress calls than for advertisement calls. Significant relationships were found for the following (Fig. 8; see  Table S1 (175_herp-75-03-06_s01.docx) in the Supplemental Materials available online): glottal lip length and maximum power (Fig. 8a), relative anterior tracheal ring width (SVL/ATRW) and call frequency at 5% of sound energy (Fig. 8b), relative posterior tracheal width (SVL/PTW) and frequency bandwidth (Fig. 8c), relative larynx width (HW/LW) and maximum power (Fig. 8d), relative anterior tracheal ring width (SVL/ATRW) and dominant call frequency (Fig. 8e), relative posterior tracheal width and call frequency at 95% of sound energy (Fig. 8f).

Fig. 8

Features of laryngotracheal morphology for which there are statistically significant relationships with acoustic properties of distress calls (see Table 1, Column 11). Data for those features of laryngotracheal morphology that were not normally distributed were log-transformed prior to comparison with acoustic call properties, or a nonparametric Spearman's rank correlation was used, as appropriate. Detailed statistical results are provided in  Table S1 (175_herp-75-03-06_s01.docx) in the Supplemental Materials available online.

img-z17-1_175.jpg

Ancestral State Reconstructions (ASRs)

The parsimony and maximum-likelihood (ML) ASRs were generally in agreement, although the ML dendrograms generally indicated lower certainty at deep nodes than did the parsimony reconstructions (Fig. 9).

Fig. 9

Ancestral state reconstructions of Gekkota based on parsimony (left) and maximum likelihood (right). (a) cricoid shape, (b) projection angle of the lateral processes of the cricoid cartilage, (c) trachea shape, (d) continuity of tracheal rings, (e) fusion of the cricoid and first tracheal ring(s), and (f) orientation of ventral fibers of m. constrictor laryngis, (g) relative size of the m. constrictor laryngis. Pie charts at each node represent the relative likelihood of a particular state. See Table 3 for definitions and matrix of character states.

img-z18-1_175.jpg

Form of the cricoid.—Both analyses indicate that the ancestor of geckos had a broad, ring-shaped cricoid, compared to the rounded cricoid present in the outgroup, and that the plate-like cricoid arose in the ancestor of Ptenopus (Fig. 9a).

Parsimony and ML methods lead to different conclusions about the ancestral state of the orientation of the lateral processes of the cricoid cartilage. The parsimony ASR indicates that the most recent common ancestor of Gekkota possessed lateral processes that project anterolaterally, and that laterally projecting lateral processes arose independently once in Diplodactylidae and once in Eublepharidae (Fig. 9b). The morphology of the lateral processes of the ancestor of the clade comprised of ([Gekkonidae + Phyllodactylidae] + Sphaerodactylidae) is equivocal, but there is strong support that the ancestor of ([Pachydactylus + Chondrodactylus] + Afroedura) possessed lateral processes that project anterolaterally, and that the ancestor of Ptenopus possessed lateral processes that project laterally. The ancestor of Carphodactylidae also had lateral processes that project posterolaterally. Contrastingly, the ML ASR indicates uncertainty about all of the attributes mentioned above for the ancestor of Gekkota, and weak support for all nodes apart from the observed states of the terminal taxa (Fig. 9b).

Form of the trachea.—Parsimony methods indicate that the ancestor of Gekkota possessed a trachea of uniform diameter, and that departures from this morphology arose independently in specific terminal taxa rather than being shared at deeper nodes (Fig. 9c). The tracheal shape of the ancestor of African gekkonids (Ptenopus, Afroedura, Chondrodactylus, and Pachydactylus), however, is equivocal. Again, ML methods provide weak support for the tracheal shape of the gekkotan ancestor (Fig. 9c). The only node strongly supported is that of the ancestor of Eublepharidae, which likely had a trachea of uniform width.

Parsimony and ML methods agree that the ancestral state for tracheal rings was a series of continuous units around the circumference of the trachea and that incomplete rings arose independently in several terminal taxa (Fig. 9d).

The parsimony analysis indicates that the cricoid cartilage is fused to the anteriormost tracheal ring in the ancestor of Gekkota and that separation occurred early in ([Carphodactylidae + Pygopodidae] + Diplodactylidae) lineage and also in Phyllodactylidae and the Hemidactylus + Cyrtodactylus clade (Fig. 9e). The ML ASR indicates uncertainty for the fusion of the cricoid cartilage and the anteriormost tracheal ring of the gekkotan ancestor. The only node strongly supported is that of the ancestor of Eublepharidae, which likely possessed a cricoid cartilage fused with the anteriormost tracheal ring (Fig. 9e).

Muscle form.—The parsimony ASR indicates weak support for most of the deep nodes, but strong support for the premise that the ancestor of the clade containing Gekkonidae + Phyllodactylidae possessed posterolaterally oriented fibers of the m. constrictor laryngis on the ventral surface of the cricoid, and for the ancestor of Gekkonidae and that of the African gekkonids possessing the same morphology (Fig. 9f). Morphologies deviating from this condition arose chiefly in terminal taxa. There is strong support that the ancestor of the clade containing Pygopodidae + Carphodactylidae, and Oedura also possessed this morphology. Laterally oriented fibers arose in the ancestor of the Chondrodactylus + Pachydactylus clade, and also in the ancestor of Hemidactylus. From the ML analyses, there is weak support for all nodes apart from the observed states of the terminal taxa (Fig. 9f).

The size of the m. constrictor laryngis in the gekkotan ancestor is equivocal based on the parsimony ASR, but indicates that the ancestor of the ([Gekkonidae + Phyllodactylidae] + Sphaerodactylidae) clade possessed an intermediate-sized m. constrictor laryngis (Fig. 9g). An enlarged m. constrictor laryngis arose in the ancestor of the ([{Hemidactylus + Cyrtodactylus} + {Agamura + Stenodactylus}] + [Lepidodactylus + Gekko]) clade, whereas a diminutive m. constrictor laryngis is likely the ancestral state for Eublepharidae. The ML ASR also supports a small m. constrictor laryngis as the ancestral state for Eublepharidae, but provides weak support at all other nodes (Fig. 9g).

Discussion

This study represents the most extensive comparative analysis of gecko vocalizations to date, being based upon vocalizations and morphological measurements of 36 species representing 21 genera and 6 families. Advertisement calls were recorded from males of 17 species and 1 all-female species. We analyzed distress calls for 28 species, and recorded both call types for males representing 8 species. For 25 of the 36 species recorded, sonographic data are reported for the first time (Table 1; Fig. 1).

Whereas all gekkotan families except pygopodids are represented in the calls we recorded, most of the species whose advertisement calls were recorded are gekkonids. Access to species maintained in captivity also provided us with the opportunity to assess which taxa do not emit calls, especially advertisement calls. Neither eublepharids nor diurnal species (apart from Ptyodactylus hasselquistii) were observed to produce advertisement calls, despite the fact that large numbers of diurnal species, chiefly in Phelsuma and Rhoptropus, were present. This finding is consistent with previous reports suggesting that neither diurnal species nor eublepharids typically emit spontaneous advertisement calls (Marcellini 1977; Frankenberg 1978a; Metallinou et al. 2015). These observations were made in captivity, however, and many diurnal taxa have yet to be assessed in field settings.

Outcomes of Hypothesis Testing

Hypothesis 1: Call frequency and body size.—The dominant call frequency of advertisement calls was found to be negatively proportional to body size (Fig. 4a). This size–frequency relationship has also been documented for a diversity of anuran species and presumably reflects a direct correlation between body size and the dimensions of the vocal folds (Gingras et al. 2013). No significant relationship was detected between the dominant call frequency of distress calls and body size. The frequency distribution of distress calls occupies a greater bandwidth than that of advertisement calls (Fig. 4b), and the sound energy of distress calls is more variable and diffuse (Figs. 13). It is likely that the dominant call frequency of distress calls is more variable within the sound energy range. As a result, the measurements for these calls are more variable and obviate a similar allometric relationship to that seen for advertisement calls.

One potential confounding factor when investigating ecological correlates across a set of closely related species is the possibility that any observed correlation is explainable as a result of the sharing of traits through phylogeny, rather than reflecting ecological adaptation (Felsenstein 1985; Maddison and Maddison 2005). Such a concern is lessened for this study because of the breadth of the phylogenetic and morphological sampling undertaken. The 17 species used in the regression analysis of advertisement calls comprise representatives of 3 families and 10 genera, and thus are not clustered in a localized area of the gekkotan tree. Additionally, although Gekkonidae was the most heavily sampled family, the taxa investigated represent a diverse range of body sizes (Table 2)—for example, Microgecko (SVL ≈ 28 mm, mass = 0.5 g), Afroedura (SVL = 50–65 mm, mass = 4.3–8.5 g), and Chondrodactylus (SVL = 74–95 mm, mass = 17–38 g). This rationale holds true for all hypotheses tested herein, including those investigating the morphological correlates of phonation.

For Hypothesis 1, we conclude that the dominant frequency of advertisement calls is negatively proportional to body size.

Hypothesis 2: Intra- and interspecific vocal communication.—Our results indicate that advertisement calls are used for intraspecific communication and that distress calls are used for interspecific communication, because the frequency bandwidth of the latter averages 7.69 kHz across all species, compared to the 3.18-kHz average frequency bandwidth for advertisement calls. Gekkotan ears (apart from those of pygopodids, many of which are sensitive to frequencies above 10 kHz; Manley and Kraus 2010) are relatively insensitive to sounds above 10 kHz (Werner et al. 2008). The high-frequency bandwidth of distress calls and the number of distress calls whose frequencies extend above 10 kHz indicate that much of their sound energy is inaudible to geckos, and that this call type is used for interspecific communication. One hypothesis is that such calls serve to deter predators that actively attack geckos, with the high-frequency bandwidth of the distress call being adaptive for startling and deterring predators with hearing sensitivities different from those of geckos (Russell et al. 2014).

For Hypothesis 2, we conclude that advertisement calls are used for intraspecific communication, whereas distress calls are used for interspecific communication. Below, we present further evidence in support of this conclusion.

Hypothesis 3: Species specificity of advertisement calls.—Strong support for advertisement calls being species specific is provided by our DFAs, indicating that such calls are used for intraspecific communication devoted to such aspects as mate attraction and/or territoriality. Our DFAs were able to identify the calling species correctly 92% of the time for advertisement calls when all measured parameters were used (Fig. 5a), and 87% of the time when the parameters were limited to only those that were also measured for distress calls (Fig. 5d). When compared to the 51% rate for correct identification of the calling species for distress calls (Fig. 5e), it is clear that advertisement calls incorporate more distinctive content than do distress calls.

When comparing the calls of five species of Pachydactylus (Fig. 5b), our DFA correctly identified 100% of advertisement calls to the calling species. Recently, it has been shown that geographically isolated and morphologically distinct populations of what was, at the time, regarded as Gekko gecko (the red and black tokays) have distinct components to their calls (Yu et al. 2011). Subsequently, Black Tokay Geckos were elevated to specific status (G. reevesii; Rösler et al. 2011). Our findings that advertisement calls are species specific corroborate those of Yu et al. (2011).

Yu et al. (2011) also reported individually specific components in the advertisement calls of the gekkonids G. gecko and G. reevesii. We found that advertisement calls of the diplodactylid Correlophus ciliatus also conveyed information as to the individual caller, as exemplified by the advertisement calls of three individuals, of which our DFA correctly identified 95% of calls to the calling individual (Fig. 5c). Because of the small number of individuals used in our investigation, the question of individual specificity of calls warrants further investigation.

For Hypothesis 3, our results are consistent with advertisement calls being species-specific forms of communication.

Hypothesis 4: Morphological effects on phonation.—Our morphological examination of laryngotracheal morphology revealed several correlations between morphology and call properties. Increasing the loudness (i.e., maximum power) of calls is likely the result of modifications of laryngotracheal morphology that increase the velocity and pressure of air flow (Russell et al. 2014). Only larynx shape (LW/LL) was positively correlated with the loudness of advertisements calls (Fig. 7g), whereas the loudness of distress calls was positively correlated with the log of the length of the glottal lips (Fig. 8a), and was negatively correlated with relative laryngeal width (HW/LW; Fig. 8d). These data indicate that geckos with larger (specifically, wider) larynges typically produce louder calls. Wider larynges would collect and pass more air during vocalization and forcibly expel it under high pressure through the aditus laryngis, thereby producing louder calls. All three species of Ptenopus that we examined exhibit an extremely large, triangular plate-shaped cricoid cartilage, which appears to be an autapomorphic feature of the genus (Fig. 9a), and they produce the loudest advertisement calls (up to 99.1 dB) of all species recorded. The calls of these species can be heard by humans at distances of several hundred meters (Haacke 1969). Although increased laryngeal size is correlated with increased call loudness, a negative correlation was detected for the frequency (DCF; CF5%, and CF95%) of advertisement calls (Fig. 7a,b,d,e).

Tracheal width relative to body size is positively associated with several frequency measurements (Fig. 7i–k). Russell et al. (2014) noted the highly aberrant tracheal dilatation of Uroplatus and related it to the explosive and energetic distress call of Uroplatus henkeli. Several species examined in this study possess similar tracheal morphologies. The trachea of Chondrodactylus angulifer is greatly expanded anteriorly and this species readily produces explosive distress calls resembling growls. The trachea of Oedura marmorata is dilated like that of Uroplatus, although not quite as distinctively, and its advertisement call resembles the distress call of Uroplatus by exhibiting an elongated, explosive series of chirps.

Apart from Ptyodactylus hasselquistii, no diurnally active species regularly vocalize, and no advertisement calls have been reported for eublepharid geckos (Marcellini 1977; Frankenberg 1978a; Metallinou et al. 2015). No notable features distinguish the larynx and trachea of the diurnally vocal species of Ptyodactylus from those of most crepuscular or nocturnally vocal gekkonids. Additionally, when compared to the moderate to extensive musculature of the gekkotan larynx, the nocturnal, but nonvocal, eublepharid geckos exhibit relatively weakly developed laryngeal musculature. Furthermore, the lateral processes of the cricoid cartilage project only moderately in the eublepharids examined. Russell et al. (2000) suggested that the evolution of laryngotracheal specialization (as seen in Ptenopus and Uroplatus), and probably the expression of vocal abilities, has occurred independently on several occasions within Afro-Malagasy gekkonids alone. Such a contention supports claims that vocal organs in reptiles are evolutionarily highly plastic and represent a random assemblage of morphologies with no central evolutionary tendency (Gans and Maderson 1973; Vogel 1976). This hypothesis concurs with the results of our ancestral state reconstructions.

For Hypothesis 4, our findings are consistent with the idea that variability in laryngotracheal morphology is associated with acoustic properties of phonation.

Hypothesis 5: Ancestral state reconstruction.—Ancestral state reconstructions using parsimony and maximum-likelihood (ML) methods did not always accord with one another. Apart from a few exceptions, ML generally provided weak support for most deep nodes. A lack of phylogenetic signal indicates highly plastic adaptive characters (Losos 2008), which would be expected if laryngotracheal morphology that affects phonation and vocalization is under intense sexual selection. In this case, mate choice might drive the evolution of laryngotracheal morphology, possibly resulting in many evolutionary reversals. Therefore, we would expect a high degree of uncertainty of character states at ancestral nodes, as is evident in the phylogenetic analyses presented here.

In applying Hypothesis 5 to a broad grouping of geckos, we refute the idea that the morphology of the larynx and trachea can be used as reliable phylogenetic markers. Highly derived laryngotracheal structure seems to be a reliable characteristic, however, for some taxa at the generic, or slightly more inclusive, level (Fig. 9).

Conclusions

Overall, we found that advertisement calls are species and individual specific, and that dominant call frequency is negatively correlated with body size. The frequency bandwidth of distress calls extends over a much broader range than the bandwidth of advertisement calls, presumably because they are directed in a nonspecific manner to assist in startling and deterring a variety of predators that potentially exhibit a broad spectrum of auditory ranges. These conclusions support previous hypotheses that have been proposed, but not previously tested, against a large data set representative of Gekkota as a whole.

The gross morphology of the larynx and trachea was found to be fairly consistent across species, although some deviations, such as overall laryngeal size and tracheal dilation, might influence phonation characteristics. Ancestral state reconstructions using parsimony and ML methods traced across a known gekkotan phylogeny revealed that the gekkotan ancestor possessed a ring-shaped cricoid cartilage with distinct lateral processes and tracheal rings that extend continuously around the circumference of the trachea. Reconstruction methods, however, were not always in agreement. Incongruent ancestral state reconstructions under the two analytical approaches might reflect the fact that laryngotracheal morphology does not exhibit strong phylogenetic signal. This interpretation is consistent with claims that reptilian vocal organs are readily influenced by selective forces and represent an assemblage of features that are molded more by function than by phylogeny.

Acknowledgments.

This research was supported by National Science Foundation grant DEB-1555968, the Gerald M. Lemole Endowed Chair Funds, and a 2013 Graduate Student Summer Research Fellowship through Villanova University. EAR was supported by the Academy of the New Church Professional Development Fund. APR was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Discovery Grant 9745-2008. Research protocols adhered to ASIH-HL-SSAR Guidelines for the Use of Live Amphibians and Reptiles in Field Research and Villanova's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol “Bauer2013”). We thank J. Boone for access to his collection of living geckos, K. Asplundh for his assistance collecting data from live geckos, R. Gardner for his assistance in the lab, and the museums and curators that generously provided access to specimens. Comments by anonymous reviewers and the editors helped improve earlier drafts of this manuscript.

Supplemental Material

Supplemental material associated with this article can be found online at  https://doi.org/10.1655/Herpetologica-D-1900021.S1.

Literature Cited

1.

Alföldi, J., F. Di Palma, M. Grabherr, . . . K. Lindblad-Toh. 2011. The genome of the green anole lizard and a comparative analysis with birds and mammals. Nature 477:587–591. Google Scholar

2.

Bauer, A.M. 2013. Geckos: The Animal Answer Guide. Johns Hopkins University Press, USA. Google Scholar

3.

Bauer, A.M., J. Doherty, and A.P. Russell. 1992. Vocalizations of the New Caledonian giant gecko, Rhacodactylus leachianus. Amphibia-Reptilia 13:412–417. Google Scholar

4.

Brillet, C., and M. Paillette. 1991. Acoustic signals of the nocturnal lizard Gekko gecko: Analysis of the ‘long complex sequence'. Bioacoustics 3:33–44. Google Scholar

5.

Brown, A.M. 1985. Ultrasound in gecko distress calls (Reptilia: Gekkonidae). Israel Journal of Zoology 33:95–101. Google Scholar

6.

Busing, M.G. 1990. Untersuchungen zur Morphologie der Trachea und der Lungen in der Systematik der Gekkonidae (Reptilia). Diplomarbeit, Universität Oldenburg, Germany. Google Scholar

7.

Charif, R.A., A.M. Waack, and L.M. Strickman. 2010. Raven Pro 1.4 User's Manual. Cornell Lab of Ornithology, USA. Google Scholar

8.

Chen, J., T. Jono, J. Cui, X. Yue, and Y. Tang. 2016. The acoustic properties of low intensity vocalizations match hearing sensitivity in the webbed-toed gecko, Gekko subpalmatus. PLoS ONE 11:e0146677. Google Scholar

9.

Deng, H.-h., Y. Peng, X.-d. Yu, and Y.-Z. Tang. 2011. Disparities of court call sequences between red and black tokays. Sichuan Journal of Zoology 30:338–343. Google Scholar

10.

Felsenstein, J. 1985. Phylogenies and the comparative method. American Naturalist 125:1–15. Google Scholar

11.

Frankenberg, E. 1974. Vocalization of males of three geographical forms of Ptyodactylus from Israel (Reptilia: Sauria: Gekkonidae). Journal of Herpetology 8:59–70. Google Scholar

12.

Frankenberg, E. 1975. Distress calls of gekkonid lizards from Israel and Sinai. Israel Journal of Zoology 24:43–53. Google Scholar

13.

Frankenberg, E. 1978a. Interspecific and seasonal variation of daily activity times in gekkonid lizards (Reptilia, Lacertilia). Journal of Herpetology 12:505–519. Google Scholar

14.

Frankenberg, E. 1978b. Calls of male and female tree geckos, Cytrodactylus kotschyi. Israel Journal of Zoology 27:53–56. Google Scholar

15.

Frankenberg, E. 1982a. Social behaviour of the parthenogenetic Indo-Pacific gecko, Hemidactylus garnotii. Journal of Comparative Ethology 59:19–28. Google Scholar

16.

Frankenberg, E. 1982b. Vocal behavior of the Mediterranean house gecko, Hemidactylus turcicus. Copeia 1982:770–775. Google Scholar

17.

Frankenberg, E., and Y.L. Werner. 1984. The defensive vocal “distress” repertoire of gekkonid lizards: Intra- and inter-specific variation. Amphibia-Reptilia 5:109–124. Google Scholar

18.

Gamble, T., E. Greenbaum, T.R. Jackman, A.P. Russell, and A.M. Bauer. 2015. Into the light: Diurnality has evolved multiple times in geckos. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 115:896–910. Google Scholar

19.

Gans, C., and P.F. Maderson. 1973. Sound producing mechanisms in recent reptiles: Review and comment. American Zoologist 13:1195–1203. Google Scholar

20.

Gingras, B., M. Boeckle, C.T. Herbst, and W.T. Fitch. 2013. Call acoustics reflect body size across four clades of anurans. Journal of Zoology 28:143–150. Google Scholar

21.

Glaw, F., and M. Vences. 1992. A Field Guide to the Amphibians and Reptiles of Madagascar. M. Vences & F. Glaw Verlags Gbr, Germany. Google Scholar

22.

Gramentz, D. 2003. Zur Stimme und Rufperiodik von Hemidactylus mabouia (Morea de Jonnès, 1818). Sauria 25:23–28. Google Scholar

23.

Gramentz, D. 2004. Der Schreckruf von Stenodactylus petrii Anderson, 1896. Sauria 26:13–16. Google Scholar

24.

Gramentz, D. 2005a. Der Schreckruf von Haemodracon reibeckii Peters, 1882 (Reptilia: Sauria: Gekkonidae). Gekkota 5:170–178. Google Scholar

25.

Gramentz, D. 2005b. On the defensive behavior and the distress call of Geckonia chazaliae Mocquard, 1895. Sauria 27:23–27. Google Scholar

26.

Gramentz, D. 2005c. Zur intraspezifischen bioakustischen Kommunikation von Hemidactylus brookii angulatus Hallowell, 1852. Sauria 27:41–46. Google Scholar

27.

Gramentz, D. 2005d. Zur intraspezifischen bioakustischen Kommunikation von Hemidactylus platycephalus Peters, 1854 (Reptilia: Sauria: Gekkonidae). Gekkota 5:155–169. Google Scholar

28.

Gramentz, D. 2007a. Zum bioakustischen Verhalten männlicher Thecadactylus rapicauda Houttuyn, 1782. Sauria 29:13–18. Google Scholar

29.

Gramentz, D. 2007b. Zur akustischen and visuellen Kommunikation von Cosymbotus platyurus (Schneider, 1792). Sauria 29:13–20. Google Scholar

30.

Gramentz, D. 2008. Zum bioakustischen Verhalten von Ptenopus carpi Brain, 1962. Sauria 30:43–46. Google Scholar

31.

Gramentz, D. 2009a. On the bioacoustic behavior of male Homopholis fasciata. Russian Journal of Herpetology 16:88–94. Google Scholar

32.

Gramentz, D. 2009b. On the distress calls of male Hemidactylus brookii parvimaculatus Deraniyagala, 1953 (Reptilia: Gekkonidae) from Sri Lanka. Taprobanica 1:19–23. Google Scholar

33.

Gramentz, D. 2009c. On the distress and threat call of Ptychozoon kuhli. Hamadryad 34:111–116. Google Scholar

34.

Gramentz, D. 2010. On the diurnal advertisement call frequency of Hemidactylus frenatus with additional remarks on the distress call and churr call. Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society 106:135–141. Google Scholar

35.

Gramentz, D., and M. Barts. 2004. Der Schreklat von Pachydactylus rugosus A. Smith, 1849. Sauria 26:23–26. Google Scholar

36.

Haacke, W. 1969. The call of the barking geckos (Gekkonidae: Reptilia). Scientific Papers of the Namib Desert Research Station 46:83–93. Google Scholar

37.

Hibbitts, T.J., M.J. Whiting, and D.M. Stuart-Fox. 2007. Shouting the odds: Vocalization signals status in a lizard. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61:1169–1176. Google Scholar

38.

Jensen, J.B. 2008. Amphibians and Reptiles of Georgia. University of Georgia Press, USA. Google Scholar

39.

Jono, T., and Y. Inui. 2012. Secret calls from under the eaves: Acoustic behavior of the Japanese house gecko, Gekko japonicus. Copeia 2012:145–149. Google Scholar

40.

Losos, J.B. 2008. Phylogenetic niche conservation, phylogenetic signal and the relationship between phylogenetic relatedness and the ecological similarity between species. Ecology Letters 11:995–1007. Google Scholar

41.

Maddison, D.R., and W.P. Maddison. 2005. MacClade 4: Analysis of Phylogeny and Character Evolution, Version 4.08a. Available at  http://macclade.org.MacClade, USA. Google Scholar

42.

Maddison, W.P., and D.R. Maddison. 2015. Mesquite: A Modular System for Evolutionary Analysis, Version 3.04. Available at  http://www.mesquiteproject.org.The Mesquite Project, USA. Google Scholar

43.

Manley, G.A., and J.E.M. Kraus. 2010. Exceptional high-frequency hearing and matched vocalizations in Australian pygopod geckos. Journal of Experimental Biology 213:1876–1885. Google Scholar

44.

Marcellini, D.L. 1974. Acoustic behavior of the gekkonid lizard, Hemidactylus frenatus. Herpetologica 20:44–52. Google Scholar

45.

Marcellini, D.L. 1977. Acoustic and visual-display behavior of gekkonid lizards. American Zoologist 17:251–260. Google Scholar

46.

Metallinou, M., J. Červenka, P.-A. Crochet, L. Kratochvíl, T. Wilms, P. Geniez, M.Y. Shobrak, J.C. Brito, and J. Carranza. 2015. Species on the rocks: Systematics and biogeography of the rock-dwelling Ptyodactylus geckos (Squamata: Phyllodactylidae) in North Africa and Arabia. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 85:208–220. Google Scholar

47.

Milton, T.H., and T.A. Jenssen. 1979. Description and significance of vocalizations by Anolis grahami (Sauria: Iguanidae). Copeia 1979:481–489. Google Scholar

48.

Moore, B.A., A.P. Russell, and A.M. Bauer. 1991. Structure of the larynx of the tokay gecko (Gekko gecko), with particular reference to the vocal cords and glottal lips. Journal of Morphology 210:227–238. Google Scholar

49.

Phongkangsananan, N., L. Schwarzkopf, and D.A. Pike. 2014. Chatty females and quiet males: Complex vocal communication in the northern dtella, Gehyra dubia. Herpetological Conservation and Biology 9:285–296. Google Scholar

50.

Regalado, R. 2003. Roles of visual, acoustic, and chemical signals in social interactions of the tropical house gecko (Hemidactylus mabouia). Caribbean Journal of Science 39:307–320. Google Scholar

51.

Rittenhouse, D.R., A.P. Russell, and A.M. Bauer. 1997. Laryngotracheal morphology of the Uroplatus fimbriatus complex (Reptilia: Gekkonidae): Yet another autapomorphic organ system in a group of highly aberrant geckos. African Journal of Herpetology 46:36–48. Google Scholar

52.

Rittenhouse, D.R., A.P. Russell, and A.M. Bauer. 1998. The larynx and trachea of the barking gecko, Ptenopus garrulus maculatus (Reptilia: Gekkonidae) and their relation to vocalization. South African Journal of Zoology 33:23–30. Google Scholar

53.

Rösler, H., A.M. Bauer, M.P. Heinicke, E. Greenbaum, T. Jackman, T.Q. Nguyen, and T. Ziegler. 2011. Phylogeny, taxonomy, and zoogeography of the genus Gekko Laurenti, 1768 with the revalidation of G. reevesii Gray, 1831 (Sauria: Gekkonidae). Zootaxa 2989:1–50. Google Scholar

54.

Russell, A.P., D.R. Rittenhouse, and A.M. Bauer. 2000. Laryngotracheal morphology of Afro-Madagascan geckos: A comparative survey. Journal of Morphology 245:241–268. Google Scholar

55.

Russell, A.P., H.A. Hood, and A.M. Bauer. 2014. Laryngotracheal and cervical muscular anatomy in the genus Uroplatus (Gekkota: Gekkonidae) in relation to distress call emission. African Journal of Herpetology 63:127–151. Google Scholar

56.

Tang, Y.-Z., L.-Z. Zhuang, and Z.-W. Wang. 2001. Advertisement calls and their relation to reproductive cycles in Gekko gecko (Reptilia, Lacertilia). Copeia 2001:248–253. Google Scholar

57.

Tremul, P.R. 2003. Vocalisation by the common eastern Australian gecko, Gehyra dubia. Memoirs of the Queensland Museum 49:485–486. Google Scholar

58.

Uetz, P., P. Freed, and J. Hošek. 2019. The Reptile Database. Available at  http://www.reptile-database.org/. Archived by WebCite at  http://www.webcitation.org/77jLYxHbG on 18 April 2019. Google Scholar

59.

Vogel, P. 1976. Der Stimmapparat der Reptilien, mit besonderer Berücksichtigung von Lacerta galloti. Verhandlungen der Naturforschenden Gesellschaft in Basel 84:135–153. Google Scholar

60.

Weber, E., and Y.L. Werner. 1977. Vocalizations of two snake-lizards (Reptilia: Sauria: Pygopodidae). Herpetologica 33:353–363. Google Scholar

61.

Werner, Y.L., and E.G. Wever. 1972. Function of the middle ear in lizards: Gekko gecko and Eublepharis macularius (Gekkonoidea). Journal of Experimental Zoology 179:1–16. Google Scholar

62.

Werner, Y.L., M. Rickert, and E. Schröder. 2001. Polysyllabic calls of diplodactyline geckos, genera Oedura and Rhacodactylus. Herpetological Natural History 8:37–48. Google Scholar

63.

Werner, Y.L., L.G. Montgomery, M. Seifan, and J.C. Saunders. 2008. Effects of age and size in the ears of gekkotan lizards: Auditory sensitivity, its determinants, and new insights into tetrapod middle-ear function. European Journal of Physiology 456:951–967. Google Scholar

64.

Yu, X., Y. Peng, A. Aowphol, L. Ding, S.E. Brauth, and Y.Z. Tang. 2011. Geographic variation in the advertisement calls of Gekko gecko in relation to variations in morphological features: Implications for regional population differentiation. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 23:211–228. Google Scholar

65.

Zaworski, J.P. 1987. Observations on unusual behaviour in the parthenogenetic gecko, Lepidodactylus lugubris Fitzinger. Bulletin of the Chicago Herpetological Society 22:76. Google Scholar

Appendices

Appendix I

Gekkotan species with previously published sonograms of vocalizations listed by family. Taxonomy has been updated as necessary.

img-z22-2_175.gif

Appendix II

Specimens examined morphologically in this study listed by family. AMB = Aaron M. Bauer private collection; CAS = California Academy of Sciences; FMNH = Field Museum of Natural History; JV = John Visser private collection (housed with AMB); LACM = Natural History Museum of Los Angeles County; MCZ = Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University; YPM = Yale Peabody Museum.

img-z23-2_175.gif
© 2019 by The Herpetologists' League, Inc.
E. Alexander Rohtla , Anthony P. Russell , and Aaron M. Bauer "Sounding Off: Relationships between Call Properties, Body Size, Phylogeny, and Laryngotracheal Form of Geckos," Herpetologica 75(3), 175-197, (20 September 2019). https://doi.org/10.1655/D-19-00021
Accepted: 7 June 2019; Published: 20 September 2019
KEYWORDS
Advertisement call
ancestral state reconstruction
Distress call
Lacertilia
larynx
morphology
sonogram
RIGHTS & PERMISSIONS
Get copyright permission
Access provided by
Back to Top