Skip to main content
Scheduled maintenance on Monday, June 3rd, with potential service disruption. Find out more.
Intended for healthcare professionals
Restricted access
Research article
First published June 1990

The Purposeful-Action Sequence and the "Illusion of Control": The Effects of Foreknowledge and Target Involvement on Observers' Judgments of Others' Control Over Random Events

Abstract

This study examined how features of both the situation and the target's behavior may lead observers to infer that a target has exerted control over a random event-in this case a roll of the dice in a game of backgammon. Observers read stories about a game of backgammon in which the male target either strongly wished for one particular outcome of the roll of the dice or was indifferent and in which either the target rolled the dice himself or a third party rolled the dice for him. Results showed that when the target desired a particular consequence of the dice roll in advance and rolled the dice himself, observers were more likely to see him as having exerted influence over the dice roll, were willing to wager more that he could roll the same number again, and rated luck as less influential in producing the dice roll outcome. These results are discussed from the perspective of illusory perceptions of control that might explain why observers inferred an actor's intentions from the actor's production of an event generally regarded as random.

Get full access to this article

View all access and purchase options for this article.

1.
1 A similar concept has been described by others as skill orientation (Langer, 1975; Langer & Roth, 1975). We use the term purposeful-action sequence because we mean it to apply to the broad class of situations in which a person purposefully brings about an outcome. In some of those situations, such as opening a door, phenomenologically the ordinary person would not say that skill was involved, simply purpose.
2.
2 These cues are the same ones previously used by Wortman (1975).
3.
3 These subjects also participated in another study, reported elsewhere (Fleming & Darley, 1989, Experiment 1). The subjects who participated in the present study always did so following completion of the other experiment. Subjects were randomly assigned to treatment conditions in this experiment, irrespective of the condition to which they had been assigned in the first study. In order to ensure that participation in the first experiment did not exert any systematic influence on subjects' responses in this study, the treatment condition assignment from the earlier experiment was used as a blocking factor in all of the preliminary analyses from the present experiment. Results of these preliminary analyses showed that subjects' prior treatment condition assignment exerted no effects on the results from this study (all Fs < 1.50). As a result of these analyses, it was concluded that the two experiments were statistically independent, and the prior treatment condition variable was dropped from further analyses; it will not be reported in the present results.
Finally, because some of the subjects who participated in the present research had previously watched a videotape in which dice rolling was used, while others had not, dice rolling might have been more salient for some of these subjects than it was for others. This may suggest a potential problem in interpreting the results from the present research, especially because the vignettes that were used here concerned backgammon, a game in which dice rolling plays a major role. To minimize these potential differential salience effects, we assigned subjects to treatment conditions in the present study irrespective of their prior treatment condition assignment, and equal numbers of subjects from the various treatment conditions in the first experiment were assigned to each condition in the present study. As a result, any differential salience produced by the videotapes used in the earlier experiment was evenly distributed among the treatment conditions in this experiment. Because we were interested primarily in the relative differences among the treatment conditions in this study, rather than absolute estimates of influence, the potential impact of differential salience was considered inconsequential.
4.
4 Three of the items from the first questionnaire would have been redundant and were not included in the second set of measures.
5.
5 We found a similar pattern of results when we examined each question separately. Specifically, all of the five questions yielded significant main effects for both the foreknowledge and the target involvement manipulations, Fs(1, 118) ranging from 3.18 to 87.18, ps ranging from .05 to .0001, and Fs(1, 118) ranging from 5.90 to 45.60, ps ranging from .02 to .0001, respectively. The Foreknowledge × Target Involvement interaction was significant for Questions 2 and 4 (Fs[1, 118] = 4.29 and 10.18, ps < .02 and .001, respectively), marginal for Questions 1 and 3 (Fs[1, 118] = 2.25 and 1.89, ps < .11 and .16, respectively), and nonsignificant for Question 5 (F[1,118] = 1.35. In addition, our planned comparison prediction (that the planned-ahead/rolled-own target would be rated as exerting significantly more control over the outcomes of the dice rolls than the other targets) was significant for all five items, with Fs(1, 118) ranging from 22.31 to 133.34, all ps < .0001.

References

Fleming, J. H., & Darley, J. M. (1989). Perceiving choice and constraint The effects of contextual and behavioral cues on attitude attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 27-40.
Henslin, J. M. (1967). Craps and magic. American Journal of Sociology, 73, 316-330.
Jenkins, H. M., & Ward, W. C. (1965). Judgments of contingency between responses and outcomes; Psychological Monographs, 79(1, Whole No. 594).
Jones, E. E., & Nisbett, R. E. (1972). The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press.
Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311-328.
Langer, E. J. (1977). The psychology of chance. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 7, 185-207.
Langer, E. J., & Roth, J. (1975). Heads I win, tails it's chance: The illusion of control as a function of the sequence of outcomes in a purely chance task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 951-955.
Lerner, M. J., & Matthews, G. (1967). Reactions to suffering under conditions of indirect responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 319-325.
Strickland, L. H., Lewicki, R. J., & Katz, A. M. (1966). Temporal orientation and perceived control as determinants of risk-taking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 143-151.
Wortman, C. B. (1975). Some determinants of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 282-294.
Wortman, C. B. (1976). Causal attributions and personal control. In J. H. Harvey, W. I. Ickes, & R. F. Kidd (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
EMAIL ARTICLE LINK
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published: June 1990
Issue published: June 1990

Rights and permissions

Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

John H. Fleming
University of Minnesota
John M. Darley
Princeton University

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.

VIEW ALL JOURNAL METRICS

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 61

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 15 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 0

  1. Superior-Subordinate Divergence in Controllability Judgments
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  2. Skill-Based Electronic Gaming Machines: a Review of Product Structures...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  3. 自主选择偏好:表现、机制与应用
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  4. From dictators to avatars: Furthering social and personality psycholog...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  5. From incidental harms to moral elevation: The positive effect of exper...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  6. How do gamblers maintain an illusion of control?
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  7. Expressing Gambling-Related Cognitive Biases in Motor Behaviour: Rolli...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  8. Recommending Action More Than Inaction When Either Decision Fails: Whi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  9. Illusory control as a function of the motivation to avoid randomly det...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  10. Illusory Personal Control as a Determinant of Bet Size and Type in Cas...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  11. Need State and the Illusion of Control
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  12. Designing Effective Promotional Games: Opportunities and Problems
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

Get access

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:

SPSP members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.

SPSP members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub