Skip to main content
Intended for healthcare professionals
Open access
Research article
First published online March 25, 2015

Social Influence on Risk Perception During Adolescence

Abstract

Adolescence is a period of life in which peer relationships become increasingly important. Adolescents have a greater likelihood of taking risks when they are with peers rather than alone. In this study, we investigated the development of social influence on risk perception from late childhood through adulthood. Five hundred and sixty-three participants rated the riskiness of everyday situations and were then informed about the ratings of a social-influence group (teenagers or adults) before rating each situation again. All age groups showed a significant social-influence effect, changing their risk ratings in the direction of the provided ratings; this social-influence effect decreased with age. Most age groups adjusted their ratings more to conform to the ratings of the adult social-influence group than to the ratings of the teenager social-influence group. Only young adolescents were more strongly influenced by the teenager social-influence group than they were by the adult social-influence group, which suggests that to early adolescents, the opinions of other teenagers about risk matter more than the opinions of adults.
Risk assessment is part of people’s daily lives. Should they cross a street on a red light? Should they make a phone call while driving or not wear a seatbelt for a short drive? People are constantly making decisions about whether they should or should not take such risks. Adolescence is a developmental period that has been associated with heightened risk-taking behavior, such as alcohol and tobacco use, unsafe sexual behavior, and dangerous driving (Eaton et al., 2012; Steinberg, 2008). Risky decision making is linked to a situation’s perceived riskiness (Mills, Reyna, & Estrada, 2008; Spurrier & Blaszczynski, 2014); for decades, heightened risk taking in adolescence was explained by the notion that adolescents were more likely than adults to perceive situations as controllable or less likely to perceive situations as harmful (Elkind, 1967; Lapsley, Milstead, Quintana, Flannery, & Buss, 1986). However, recent studies dispute the notion that adolescents are unaware of potential risks and feel invulnerable. For example, compared with adults, adolescents sometimes overestimate rather than underestimate risk (Millstein & Halpern-Felsher, 2002), and they perceive certain situations as more risky and harmful than adults do (Cohn, Macfarlane, Yanez, & Imai, 1995). Thus, a reduced perception of risk may not be the critical factor in adolescent risk taking.
Social influence is a factor in adolescent risk taking: Adolescents are more likely to engage in risky behavior (including dangerous driving, substance abuse, and shoplifting) when they are with their peers than when they are alone (Dishion & Tipsord, 2011; Gardner & Steinberg, 2005; Simons-Morton, Lerner, & Singer, 2005). Adolescents spend more time with their peers than with their families (Brown, 1990) and are particularly sensitive to peer rejection (Peake, Dishion, Stormshak, Moore, & Pfeifer, 2013; Sebastian et al., 2011; Somerville, 2013). Social influence is the subject of a large body of research that has demonstrated how readily participants are affected by the opinions, judgments, and behavior of other people (Klucharev, Hytönen, Rijpkema, Smidts, & Fernández, 2009; Walker & Andrade, 1996; Zaki, Schirmer, & Mitchell, 2011). Despite extensive research on social influence in adults, developmental patterns of social influence, especially social conformity in adolescence, are not fully understood.
Deutsch and Gerard (1955) identified two types of conformity in adults. First, conformity can result from the pursuit of accuracy (informational influence); studies have shown that adults tend to follow other people as a guideline in novel or unfamiliar situations (Hilmert, Kulik, & Christenfeld, 2006; Jetten, Hornsey, & Adarves-Yorno, 2006). Second, conformity can arise from the pursuit of acceptance (normative influence); adults yield to social influence to avoid rejection (Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). Normative conformity might be prompted by an awareness of social norms; that is, people make decisions on the basis of expected or acceptable behavior in social interactions (Cialdini & Trost, 1998). We were interested in social influence on adolescent risk perception, and in the current study, we specifically investigated whether the degree of social conformity varied depending on the origin of the social influence.
We investigated the development of social influence on risk perception from late childhood through adulthood. Five hundred and sixty-three participants rated the riskiness of everyday situations and were then shown how other people, either teenagers or adults, perceived the riskiness of the same situation (these other ratings were actually generated randomly). The participants were then asked to rate the situation again. We investigated two hypotheses: (a) Risk perception is influenced by the risk ratings of others and this social-influence effect decreases with age (social-influence hypothesis; Engelmann, Moore, Capra, & Berns, 2012; Walker & Andrade, 1996); and (b) this socially driven change in risk perception depends on whether the social-influence group consists of teenagers or adults (peer-influence hypothesis). Regarding this second hypothesis, we predicted that adults would be more influenced by the ratings of other adults than by the ratings of teenagers and, in contrast, that adolescents would be more influenced by other teenagers than by adults (Somerville, 2013; Stallen, Smidts, & Sanfey, 2013). This prediction is based on the proposal that the pursuit of social acceptance by peers is a significant determinant of adolescent decision making (Blakemore & Mills, 2014; Reyna & Farley, 2006).

Method

Participants

Participants were visitors to the Science Museum in London on 14 days in April 2013. Data from 563 visitors (mean age = 23.4 years, SD = 12.2, age range = 8–59 years; 313 females, 250 males) were included in the analyses. Data from 63 additional participants were excluded because their responses were incomplete or they reported developmental conditions, including autism, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, epilepsy, dyslexia, dyspraxia, dyscalculia, or depression. Participants were divided into five age groups as in similar prior studies (Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010): 106 children (mean age = 9.6 years, age range = 8–11 years; 54 girls, 52 boys), 68 young adolescents (mean age = 12.9 years, age range = 12–14 years; 39 girls, 29 boys), 52 midadolescents (mean age = 16.6 years, age range = 15–18 years; 30 girls, 22 boys), 136 young adults (mean age = 24.7 years, age range = 19–25 years; 77 women, 59 men), and 201 adults (mean age = 37.1 years, age range = 26–59 years; 113 women, 88 men). Twenty-nine percent of participants (2% of children, 11% of young adolescents, 30% of midadolescents, 40% of young adults, and 36% of adults) were nonnative speakers of English, but all spoke English fluently. Informed consent was obtained from parents for participants under 18 years old and from adults themselves. The study was approved by the University College London ethics committee.

Design

We used a 3 × 5 factorial design with the within-subjects factor social-influence group (teenagers, adults, and control) and the between-subjects factor age group (children, young adolescents, midadolescents, young adults, and adults).

Risk-perception task

In the risk-perception task, participants were presented with risky scenarios (see the Supplemental Material available online). The scenarios were generated on the basis of the following criteria: (a) They included a potential immediate health risk, such as an accident (e.g., crossing a street while texting, cycling without a helmet, driving without a seatbelt, climbing on a roof); (b) they elicited a relatively large amount of variance across individuals in terms of perceived risk; (c) they were generally considered to have moderate or medium risk; and (d) they included a variety of situations and avoided repetition. Stimuli consisted of single statements and were presented both aurally and visually. The auditory stimuli were spoken by a female English speaker and recorded in a soundproof chamber. After recording, stimuli were digitized (sampling rate = 44.1 kHz; bit depth = 16; monaural) and normalized. Statements were simultaneously presented aurally and displayed at the top of a screen, and they were illustrated with an image depicting the situation without providing too much contextual information (Fig. 1). The images were included to make the stimuli more appealing for museum visitors.
Fig. 1. Illustration of the trial sequence. Participants were asked to imagine that someone was engaged in an activity (in this example, crossing the street on a red light). They then rated the activity’s risk by using a computer mouse to move a slider on a visual analogue scale. There was no time restriction for this first rating. After making this rating, participants were shown (for 2 s) a risk rating of the same situation that was ostensibly provided by a group of either adults or teenagers (the social-influence conditions) or was the participant’s own rating (control condition). The ratings from the social-influence groups were actually randomly generated. Finally, participants were asked to rate the same situation again. There was no time restriction for the second rating. The next trial started after 1 s.
Participants read and listened to instructions before the main task, which started only after they pressed a button to indicate they had understood the instructions. The task was programmed using Cogent 2000 (University College London Laboratory of Neurobiology; http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_2000.php) and run in MATLAB (Version R2012b; The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA). The entire task took around 12 min to complete.
Before the trials began, the participant’s age was displayed for 3 s alongside a message stating “Calculating your age group. . . .” The intention of this manipulation was that each participant would associate him- or herself with a social-influence group of a particular age (teenagers or adults). In addition, participants over 18 years old were explicitly told that the adult group picture represented a group of people their age, and participants between 12 and 18 years old were explicitly told that the teenager picture represented a group of people their age. Note that children’s ratings were not included for three reasons: First, our hypothesis pertained to adolescent risk perception and whether this was influenced differentially by risk ratings from other teenagers versus those from adults; second, we wanted to constrain the complexity of the experimental design; and third, we wanted to keep the study as short as possible because there were time limits for testing in the Science Museum.
On each trial, participants were asked to imagine that someone was engaged in the activity presented. The participants rated the activity’s risk by using a computer mouse to move a slider to the left side (low risk) or to the right side (high risk) of a colorful visual analogue scale. The slider initially appeared at a random position on the scale on each trial to avoid any consistent anchoring bias. There was no time restriction for the first rating. After making the first rating, participants were shown a risk rating of the same situation by either adults or teenagers (i.e., the social-influence group) for 2 s. These ratings were ostensibly from other participants; in fact, however, they either were randomly generated (adult social-influence condition, teenager social-influence condition) or were the participants’ own ratings (control condition). This minor deception was approved by the ethics committee. After this, the participant was asked to rate the same situation again. There was no time restriction for the second rating.
Participants provided the second rating in each trial, and then the next trial started after 1 s. A total of 79 different situations were generated; 18 (6 per social-influence condition) were randomly selected for each participant. The order of trials was pseudorandomized within each participant, such that there were never more than five consecutive trials from a single social-influence condition. In the control condition, instead of being shown the ratings of other people, participants were shown their first ratings and were then asked to rate the situations again. The purpose of the control condition was to check that there was no systematic difference between the age groups in terms of remembering their first rating and to find out the degree to which the participants in different age groups shifted their answers under no social influence. The timings of the stimulus presentation were identical for each trial in all three of the social-influence conditions.

Statistical analysis

Risk-perception analysis

Differences in the first risk ratings across age groups were analyzed using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post hoc pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni-adjusted alpha levels).

Social-influence analysis

We used linear mixed-effects models to investigate the degree to which participants changed their risk ratings in the direction of other people’s ratings (social-influence hypothesis) and whether the extent of this change depended on whether the social-influence group consisted of adults or teenagers (peer-influence hypothesis). These models incorporated: (a) fixed effects that reflected average effects within and differences between the three experimental conditions and (b) random effects that took into account individual variability in the effect of participants’ first rating on their second rating and individual variability in susceptibility to social influence.
The linear mixed-effects model was used to assess the dependence of a participant’s second rating on two main predictors: (a) the first rating and (b) the difference between the rating provided by the social-influence group and the participant’s first rating (Δrating). The latter predictor was used to assess the degree to which participants were influenced by the rating provided by the social-influence group. We were particularly interested in whether the effect of the provided rating was dependent on its source (i.e., teenager or adult social-influence group) and the participants’ ages. Therefore, the model also included interactions between Δrating and age, Δrating and social-influence group, and Δrating, age, and social-influence group.
Fixed effects were included for all the main and interaction factors in the model. In addition, the model included subject-specific random slopes for the first rating and Δrating. Effect coding was used for the social-influence groups (1 = adults; −1 = teenagers), whereas dummy coding was used for age group (with young adults as the baseline group). The model did not include an intercept, because an intercept not identical to 0 would mean that participants’ second rating always increased (or decreased). For the same reason, no main effects of social-influence group or age group were included. Preliminary analyses indicated that these effects were indeed redundant. For a more detailed description of the linear mixed-effects model analysis, see the Supplemental Material.

Results

Risk perception

A one-way ANOVA was used to test whether the first risk rating differed across age groups. There was a main effect of age group on risk rating, F(4, 558) = 15.4, p < .001. Children rated the situations as more risky (M = 6.20, SD = 1.26) than did every other age group—children compared with young adolescents (M = 5.57, SD = 0.99): t(173) = 3.47, p < .01; children compared with midadolescents (M = 5.18, SD = 1.08): t(157) = 5.00, p < .001; children compared with young adults (M = 5.12, SD = 1.02): t(241) = 7.39, p < .001; and children compared with adults (M = 5.51, SD = 1.15): t(305) = 4.82, p < .001. In addition, adults rated the situations as more risky than did young adults, t(334) = −3.20, p < .05 (see Fig. 2a). No other pairwise comparison was significant—young adolescents compared with midadolescents: t(118) = 2.06, p = .58; young adolescents compared with young adults: t(202) = 3.03, p = .07; young adolescents compared with adults: t(266) = 0.40, p = 1.0; midadolescents compared with young adults: t(186) = 0.363, p = 1.0; and midadolescents compared with adults: t(250) = −1.86, p = .59.
Fig. 2. Results. The graph in (a) shows mean first risk rating as a function of age group: children (ages 8–11), young adolescents (ages 12–14), midadolescents (ages 15–18), young adults (ages 19–25), and adults (ages 26–59). The possible range of rating was from 0 (low risk) to 10 (high risk). Error bars represent ±1 SE. Asterisks indicate statistically significant differences between groups (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001; Bonferroni corrected). The scatter plots (b) illustrate the relationship between Δrating (the difference between the provided rating and the first rating) and change in rating (the difference between the second rating and the first rating) separately for each of the five age groups. Each symbol represents a single trial. Note that Δrating for the control condition was always zero. The graph in (c) presents the slopes for the average change in risk rating predicted by the difference between the provided rating and the first rating (Δrating). Results are shown separately for the adult social-influence condition and the teenager social-influence condition, for each age group. The slopes were calculated using estimates of the linear mixed-effect model analysis.
Figure 2b shows scatter plots of raw data depicting the change in rating for all conditions and all age groups. The figure shows the decrease in social influence with age and illustrates that Δrating in the control condition was necessarily zero because the provided rating was simply the first rating. The linear mixed-effects model was used to estimate the second rating as a function of (a) the first rating, (b) Δrating, and (c) the interactions involving age group and social-influence group. Full results of the main linear mixed-effects analysis are reported in Tables 1 and 2. Omnibus F tests are comparable with those in traditional ANOVAs and show significance of all the components of the main linear mixed-effects model.
Table 1. Main Linear Mixed-Effects Model Predicting Change in Rating: Omnibus-Test Results
Predictor F test p
First rating F(1, 666.32) = 32,129.69 < .001
ΔRating F(1, 604.61) = 607.97 < .001
ΔRating × Age F(4, 548.04) = 53.69 < .001
ΔRating × Social Influence F(1, 9637.45) = 4.52 .034
ΔRating × Age × Social Influence F(4, 9643.51) = 4.17 .002
Table 2. Main Linear Mixed-Effects Model Predicting Change in Rating: Fixed Effects
Fixed effect b SE t test p
First rating 0.98 0.01 t(665.48) = 179.25 < .001
ΔRating 0.12 0.01 t(565.93) = 8.53 < .001
ΔRating × Children 0.21 0.02 t(544.55) = 9.99 < .001
ΔRating × Young Adolescents 0.15 0.02 t(559.85) = 6.14 < .001
ΔRating × Midadolescents 0.06 0.03 t(541.26) = 2.28 .023
ΔRating × Adults −0.05 0.02 t(545.82) = −2.69 .007
ΔRating × Social Influence 0.02 0.01 t(9619.13) = 2.70 .007
ΔRating × Children × Social Influence 0.01 0.01 t(9653.10) = 0.71 .478
ΔRating × Young Adolescents × Social Influence −0.04 0.01 t(9633.64) = −3.44 .001
ΔRating × Midadolescents × Social Influence −0.01 0.01 t(9609.38) = −0.78 .434
ΔRating × Adults × Social Influence −0.01 0.01 t(9641.82) = −0.85 .393
Note: Effect coding was used for social-influence group (1 = adults; −1 = teenagers), whereas dummy coding was used for age group; young adults was used as the baseline group. The estimated standard deviations of the random effects were 0.14 (Δrating) and 0.08 (first rating). The standard deviation of the residuals was 1.02.

Social-influence hypothesis

We used linear mixed-effects models to investigate the extent to which participants changed their risk ratings in the direction of others’ ratings. The extent of such changes did differ between age groups, as revealed by a significant two-way interaction between age group and Δrating, F(4, 548.04) = 53.69, p < .001. The slopes of the individual effects revealed that the slope for the first rating was close to 1, such that the other effects could clearly be interpreted as reflecting their influence on the change in rating. The slope of Δrating reflects its effect in the young adult (i.e., baseline) group and indicates that, on average, young adults (ages 19–25) changed their ratings in accordance with the ratings from the social-influence group, t(565.93) = 8.53, p < .001; the magnitude of this change was equal to about 12% of the difference between that rating and their first rating. This magnitude of the change for young adults differed from that in the other age groups: 33% for children (ages 8–11), 27% for young adolescents (ages 12–14), 18% for midadolescents (ages 15–18), and 7% for adults (ages 26–59).
In follow-up models, we repeated the analysis using each of the other age groups as the baseline group to assess whether the social-influence effect was significant for all age groups. These analyses showed that the effect of social influence was significant in all age groups—children: t(554.46) = 20.82, p < .001; young adolescents: t(573.69) = 13.47, p < .001; midadolescents: t(545.64) = 8.00, p < .001; young adults: t(565.93) = 8.53, p < .001; and adults: t(568.48) = 6.16, p < .001. Thus, all age groups changed their second ratings in the direction of the ratings provided by the social-influence group. This finding supports the social-influence hypothesis. However, the degree of social influence in young adults was different from that in every other age group: Children, t(544.55) = 9.99, p < .001; young adolescents, t(559.85) = 6.14, p < .001; and midadolescents, t(541.26) = 2.28, p < .05, were more strongly influenced by the provided ratings than were young adults, whereas older adults, t(545.82) = −2.69, p < .01, were less strongly influenced than were young adults (Fig. 2c).

Peer-influence hypothesis

We investigated whether the extent of the change in risk ratings in the direction of others’ ratings depended on whether the social-influence group consisted of adults or teenagers. We found a significant three-way interaction of social-influence group, age group, and Δrating, F(4, 9643.51) = 4.17, p < .002, which indicates that social influence depended on social-influence group (teenagers or adults) and that this effect differed among the age groups. The effect of social-influence group was significant in young adults, who changed their ratings more after seeing the ratings from the adult social-influence group than after seeing ratings from the teenager social-influence group, t(9619.13) = 2.70, p < .01. The effect of social-influence group in young adults did not differ significantly from the effect of social-influence group in children, midadolescents, or adults. In contrast, the effect of social-influence group was significantly different in young adolescents, t(9633.64) = −3.44, p < .001 (Fig. 2c). Follow-up models for the peer-influence hypothesis showed that children and adults, like young adults, changed their ratings to a significantly greater extent when the social-influence group was adults rather than teenagers—children: t(9678.63) = 3.36, p < .001; adults: t(9605.25) = 1.951, p = .051. The effect in midadolescents was not significant, t(9605.25) = 0.77, p > .44, which indicates that midadolescents did not differentiate between the sources of social influence. Young adolescents, in contrast to all other age groups, were significantly more influenced by the teenager social-influence group than by the adult social-influence group, t(9640.28) = −2.33, p < .05.

Discussion

In the present study, we demonstrated that risk perception was influenced by the risk ratings of other people and this social-influence effect decreased with age (social-influence hypothesis). Furthermore, this socially driven change in risk perception was dependent on the age of the participant and on whether the social-influence group consisted of teenagers or adults (peer-influence hypothesis). Specifically, although most age groups changed their ratings more toward those of the adult social-influence group than toward those of the teenager social-influence group, young adolescents showed the reverse effect, being more influenced by the teenager social-influence group than by the adult social-influence group.
The analysis of the first rating indicated that children (ages 8–11) rated the situations as significantly more risky than did adolescents and adults. The risk-ratings-by-age function followed a roughly U-shaped pattern (see Fig. 2a): There was a decrease in risk ratings between late childhood and midadolescence, followed by an increase between young adulthood and adulthood. Children might rate situations as more risky than other age groups for a number of reasons, including having less experience of risky situations (Halpern-Felsher et al., 2001) or being at greater risk in risky situations because of their physical or cognitive disadvantage (Anderson, 2002) relative to adolescents or adults. A similar decline in risk perception from late childhood to adulthood has been reported previously (Millstein & Halpern-Felsher, 2002), although a recent study found that young adults had the greatest likelihood of perceiving risky activities as a “good idea” (Shulman & Cauffman, 2014). Thus, this analysis fails to support the notion that adolescents (relative to other age groups) feel invulnerable to harm; this finding is in line with results from previous studies that found no evidence that adolescents are more likely than adults to underestimate or overestimate risk (Millstein & Halpern-Felsher, 2002).
A large body of literature has demonstrated that other people’s behavior can have a significant impact on one’s own behavior (Berns, Capra, Moore, & Noussair, 2010; Zaki et al., 2011). People change their behavior to fit in with other people. This social conformity effect has been studied extensively, and some research has suggested that children and adolescents show a higher susceptibility to social influence than adults do (Costanzo & Shaw, 1966; Hoving, Hamm, & Galvin, 1969). However, previous findings of age effects on social conformity are inconsistent (Costanzo & Shaw, 1966; Engelmann et al., 2012; Hoving et al., 1969; Iscoe, Williams, & Harvey, 1963; Walker & Andrade, 1996). A decrease in conformity from childhood to adolescence was found in several studies using monetary games (Engelmann et al., 2012) and perceptual judgment tasks (Hoving et al., 1969; Walker & Andrade, 1996), whereas in other studies using similar perceptual judgment tasks (Costanzo & Shaw, 1966; Hoving et al., 1969; Iscoe et al., 1963) and decision making in hypothetical scenarios (Berndt, 1979), the researchers reported an increase, with a peak in conformity in early adolescence to midadolescence. It has been suggested that ambiguous stimuli evoke stronger conformity effects, whereas with unambiguous stimuli, conformity declines with age (Walker & Andrade, 1996).
In the current study, participants were told that each trial had no correct or incorrect response. We found that all age groups showed a significant social-influence effect: All groups changed their risk ratings in the direction of the provided ratings. Children (age 8–11) exhibited the highest level of social conformity, indicated by a larger change in their risk ratings after being presented with other people’s ratings (Fig. 2c), and the social-influence effect decreased steadily between late childhood and adulthood. It is not possible to determine the underlying reason for the conformity we observed in our study. The provided ratings came from a group of people whom the participants had never met and never anticipated meeting; thus, there were no social consequences of conforming or not. It is possible that the social-influence effect found here is associated with a true reevaluation of risk; that is, participants may have taken into account the ratings of the social-influence group and changed their risk perception accordingly (informational conformity) rather than pursuing acceptance or simply being aware of social norms (normative conformity; Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). The decrease in conformity across age might be because older participants have more confidence in their own perception of a situation, perhaps because they have experienced the situation more often than children or adolescents have. Thus, our findings suggest that social influence on risk perception depends on the age of the participant, and we speculate that children and adolescents are more susceptible to informational influence from other people than adults are.
Our second hypothesis addressed the source of the social influence. Our findings suggest that social influence depends on age and is also critically dependent on the source of influence: Most age groups were more influenced by the adult social-influence group than by the teenager social-influence group, but the teenager social-influence group had a stronger impact on young adolescents. Thus, children, young adults, and adults changed their ratings to a greater degree toward the ratings from the adult social-influence group than toward the ratings from the teenager social-influence group. One explanation for this finding is that people in most age groups consider adults to be more competent and experienced role models than teenagers are. Previous studies have shown that expertise and status are strong predictors of social influence (Driskell & Mullen, 1990; Engelmann et al., 2012; Jetten et al., 2006). In contrast, young adolescents were more influenced by the teenager social-influence group than by the adult social-influence group. There was no significant difference between the adult and teenager social-influence groups regarding influence of midadolescents’ ratings.
An early study investigating decision making in social situations, in which parents and peers offered conflicting advice, found that although conformity to the advice of parents decreases with age, conformity to peers increases during adolescence (ages 12–17; Utech & Hoving, 1969). On the other hand, parental opinions continue to be important during adolescence: Adolescents rely on parental advice particularly when making decisions relevant for their futures (Commendador, 2010; Galotti & Mark, 1994). Our finding that the influence of the teenager social-influence group was stronger than the influence of the adult social-influence group in young adolescents might be due to additional normative influence in young adolescence. We speculate that adolescents seek to conform to the people of the same age, not because they trust the ratings of teenagers more than they trust the ratings of adults, but because they want to be accepted by their peer group (in this case, the teenager social-influence group). However, this is speculative, and future studies should attempt to further distinguish between normative and informational influence in adolescence. Young adolescents do not perceive situations as less risky than do other age groups but do tend to change their risk perception in the direction of the opinions of peers of a similar age; together, these two findings suggest that the target of public-health interventions should be adolescent social norms rather than (or in addition to) the potential health risks associated with certain situations and choices.

Conclusion

In this study, we investigated social influence on risk perception across development from late childhood to adulthood. In general, all age groups changed their ratings to be in line with other people’s ratings, but there was a steady decline in this social conformity from late childhood through adulthood. This first finding might be explained by informational conformity: All groups trust in the ratings of the social-influence groups, but this trust is highest in children and decreases with age. Our second finding provided insights into the impact of different social-influence groups on risk perception in different age groups. Our results support the notion that early adolescence is a pivotal phase in which individuals begin to challenge the authority and experience of adults and place higher value on the opinions of other teenagers than on opinions of adults. The explanation for this trend might be heightened normative conformity in young adolescents, who place particular importance on being accepted by their peer group.

Acknowledgments

We thank E. Garrett, A. Lazari, L. Tori Makhluf, and A. Vassiliou for their help with data collection and the Science Museum, London, for providing the opportunity for data collection.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Funding

This research was supported by the Royal Society, The Leverhulme Trust, and a Royal Society University Research Fellowship (to S.-J. Blakemore).

References

Anderson P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82.
Berndt T. J. (1979). Developmental changes in conformity to peers and parents. Developmental Psychology, 15, 608–616.
Berns G. S., Capra C. M., Moore S., Noussair C. (2010). Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music. NeuroImage, 49, 2687–2696.
Blakemore S.-J., Mills K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187–207.
Brown B. B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In Feldman S. S., Elliott G. R. (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 171–196). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cialdini R. B., Trost M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms, conformity and compliance. In Gilbert D. T., Fiske S. T., Lindzey G. (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 151–192). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Cohn L. D., Macfarlane S., Yanez C., Imai W. K. (1995). Risk-perception: Differences between adolescents and adults. Health Psychology, 14, 217–222.
Commendador K. A. (2010). Parental influences on adolescent decision making and contraceptive use. Pediatric Nursing, 36, 147–156, 170.
Costanzo P. R., Shaw M. E. (1966). Conformity as a function of age level. Child Development, 37, 967–975.
Deutsch M., Gerard H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636.
Dishion T. J., Tipsord J. M. (2011). Peer contagion in child and adolescent social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 189–214.
Driskell J. E., Mullen B. (1990). Status, expectations, and behavior: A meta-analytic review and test of the theory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 541–553.
Eaton D. K., Kann L., Kinchen S., Shanklin S., Flint K. H., Hawkins J., . . . Wechsler H. (2012). Youth risk behavior surveillance—United States, 2011. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 61(4), 1–162.
Elkind D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38, 1025–1034.
Engelmann J. B., Moore S., Capra C. M., Berns G. S. (2012). Differential neurobiological effects of expert advice on risky choice in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 557–567.
Galotti K. M., Mark M. C. (1994). How do high school students structure an important life decision? A short-term longitudinal study of the college decision-making process. Research in Higher Education, 35, 589–607.
Gardner M., Steinberg L. (2005). Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 41, 625–635.
Halpern-Felsher B. L., Millstein S. G., Ellen J. M., Adler N. E., Tschann J. M., Biehl M. (2001). The role of behavioral experience in judging risks. Health Psychology, 20, 120–126.
Hilmert C. J., Kulik J. A., Christenfeld N. J. S. (2006). Positive and negative opinion modeling: The influence of another’s similarity and dissimilarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 440–452.
Hoving K. L., Hamm N., Galvin P. (1969). Social influence as a function of stimulus ambiguity at three age levels. Developmental Psychology, 1, 631–636.
Iscoe I., Williams M., Harvey J. (1963). Modification of children’s judgments by a simulated group technique: A normative developmental study. Child Development, 34, 963–978.
Jetten J., Hornsey M. J., Adarves-Yorno I. (2006). When group members admit to being conformist: The role of relative intragroup status in conformity self-reports. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 162–173.
Klucharev V., Hytönen K., Rijpkema M., Smidts A., Fernández G. (2009). Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformity. Neuron, 61, 140–151.
Lapsley D. K., Milstead M., Quintana S. M., Flannery D., Buss R. R. (1986). Adolescent egocentrism and formal operations: Tests of a theoretical assumption. Developmental Psychology, 22, 800–807.
Mills B., Reyna V. F., Estrada S. (2008). Explaining contradictory relations between risk perception and risk taking. Psychological Science, 19, 429–433.
Millstein S. G., Halpern-Felsher B. L. (2002). Judgments about risk and perceived invulnerability in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 12, 399–422.
Peake S. J., Dishion T. J., Stormshak E. A., Moore W. E., Pfeifer J. H. (2013). Risk-taking and social exclusion in adolescence: Neural mechanisms underlying peer influences on decision-making. NeuroImage, 82, 23–34.
Reyna V. F., Farley F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: Implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, 1–44.
Sebastian C. L., Tan G. C. Y., Roiser J. P., Viding E., Dumontheil I., Blakemore S.-J. (2011). Developmental influences on the neural bases of responses to social rejection: Implications of social neuroscience for education. NeuroImage, 57, 686–694.
Shulman E. P., Cauffman E. (2014). Deciding in the dark: Age differences in intuitive risk judgment. Developmental Psychology, 50, 167–177.
Simons-Morton B., Lerner N., Singer J. (2005). The observed effects of teenage passengers on the risky driving behavior of teenage drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37, 973–982.
Somerville L. H. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 121–127.
Spurrier M., Blaszczynski A. (2014). Risk perception in gambling: A systematic review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30, 253–276.
Stallen M., Smidts A., Sanfey A. G. (2013). Peer influence: Neural mechanisms underlying in-group conformity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 50. Retrieved from http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00050/full
Steinberg L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78–106.
Utech D. A., Hoving K. L. (1969). Parents and peers as competing influences in the decisions of children of differing ages. The Journal of Social Psychology, 78, 267–274.
Van Leijenhorst L., Moor B. G., Op de Macks Z. A., Rombouts S. A. R. B., Westenberg P. M., Crone E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. NeuroImage, 51, 345–355.
Walker M. B., Andrade M. G. (1996). Conformity in the Asch task as a function of age. The Journal of Social Psychology, 136, 367–372.
Zaki J., Schirmer J., Mitchell J. P. (2011). Social influence modulates the neural computation of value. Psychological Science, 22, 894–900.

Supplementary Material

In this Data Supplement:

Summary

Additional supporting information can be found at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data

Resources

File (suppl-material.pdf)

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
EMAIL ARTICLE LINK
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: March 25, 2015
Issue published: May 2015

Keywords

  1. development
  2. adolescence
  3. social influence
  4. peer influence
  5. conformity
  6. risk perception

Rights and permissions

© The Author(s) 2015.
Creative Commons License (CC BY 3.0)
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access page (http://www.uk.sagepub.com/aboutus/openaccess.htm).
PubMed: 25810453

Authors

Affiliations

Lisa J. Knoll
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience
Lucía Magis-Weinberg
Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London
Maarten Speekenbrink
Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London
Sarah-Jayne Blakemore
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience

Notes

Lisa J. Knoll, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, Alexandra House, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, United Kingdom E-mail: [email protected]
Author Contributions
All authors contributed to the study design. L. J. Knoll and L. Magis-Weinberg programmed the experiment. L. J. Knoll and L. Magis-Weinberg collected the data. L. J. Knoll, L. Magis-Weinberg, and M. Speekenbrink analyzed the data. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the results. All authors contributed to the writing of the manuscript and approved the final version for submission.

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Psychological Science.

VIEW ALL JOURNAL METRICS

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 50471

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 217 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 223

  1. e-commerce website customer engagement: Delineating the role of UTAUT,...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  2. Reducing the gender gap on adolescents’ interest in study fields: The ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  3. Food and non-alcoholic beverage marketing via Fortnite streamers on Tw...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  4. Body image among adolescents: what is its relationship with rejection ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google ScholarPub Med
  5. Longitudinal links of authoritative teaching and bullying victimizatio...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  6. Predicting comorbid mental health difficulties in people with autoimmu...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  7. Mental Health Concerns and Help-Seeking Behaviors Among Adolescents in...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  8. The impact of suppressing puberty on neuropsychological function: A re...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  9. Colorful candy, teen vibes and cool memes: prevalence and content of I...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  10. Non-suicidal Self-injurious Thoughts and Behaviors Among Adolescent In...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  11. Prioritise Propensity: A multimethod analysis of peer influence and sc...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  12. The intervention design to promote active travel mode among children a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  13. Developmental psychopathology
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  14. Children and adolescents’ experiences of cyberaggression and cyberbull...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  15. Peer influence and its impact on behavior among South Indian adolescen...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  16. Is adolescence believed to be a period of greater risk taking than adu...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  17. Testing the social motivation theory of autism: the role of co‐occurri...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  18. Adolescents’ Drinking Behavior Off- and Online: Disentangling Exposure...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  19. Virtual agents and risk-taking behavior in adolescence: the twofold na...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  20. A systematic review and meta‐analysis of the effect of digital game‐ba...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  21. Academic performance under COVID-19: The role of online learning readi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  22. The influence of peer non-suicidal self-harm on young adults’ urges to...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  23. ‘With a little social support … ’: assessing the moderating effect of ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  24. Experiencing Pain Invalidation is Associated with Under-Reporting of P...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  25. Teenagers' but not young adults' beliefs about intrinsic interpersonal...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  26. Longitudinal Associations Between Peer Victimization and Positive and ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  27. The significance of feeling safe for resilience of adolescents in sub-...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  28. Like the cool kids? The role of popular classmates in the development ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  29. Exploring the Influence of Public Perception of Mass Media Usage and A...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  30. Adolescent risk-taking and decision making: a qualitative investigatio...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  31. A Conceptual Procedure-Based Study on Influencer Marketing Trend Forec...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  32. Escape Addict: A digital escape room for the prevention of addictions ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  33. Sensitivity and specificity in affective and social learning in adoles...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  34. Transition Regret and Detransition: Meanings and Uncertainties
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  35. Information sources about face-to-face meetings with people from the I...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  36. Recall of food marketing on videogame livestreaming platforms: Associa...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  37. Female rats prefer to forage food from males, an effect that is not in...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  38. Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACEs) and the Link to Antisocial, Delin...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  39. How Does Online e-cigarette Advertisement Promote Youth’s e-cigarettes...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  40. Consent in minors: the differential treatment of acceptance and refusa...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  41. Hasj er ikke greit
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  42. Exploring effects of multi-level factors on transitions of risk-taking...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  43. Local and global reward learning in the lateral frontal cortex show di...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  44. Young People’s Perception of the Danger of Risky Online Activities: Be...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  45. Thumbs up or thumbs down: neural processing of social feedback and lin...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  46. E-Cigarette Retailers’ Use of Instagram in New Zealand: A Content Anal...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  47. What factors contribute to the acceptance of artificial intelligence? ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  48. Brain Systems Underlying Fundamental Motivations of Human Social Confo...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  49. The role of brain structure in the association between pubertal timing...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  50. Social features in internet-based programs to prevent alcohol consumpt...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  51. Is it ethical to advertise unhealthy foods to children?
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  52. Prevalence and Factors Associated with Risky Sexual Behaviors Among Se...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  53. University Students’ Risk Perception, Protective Measures, and General...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  54. Understanding the role of parents and peers on adolescent risk behavio...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  55. Emotional Intelligence and Resilience Outcomes in Adolescent Period, i...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  56. Susceptibility to prosocial and antisocial influence in adolescence fo...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  57. Adolescents’ exposure to and evaluation of food promotions on social m...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  58. The cancellation heuristic in intertemporal choice shifts people’s tim...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  59. Adolescents Are More Likely to Help Others on Days They Take Risks and...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  60. Advertising and Young People’s Critical Reasoning Abilities: Systemati...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  61. Does mental health mediate the effect of deviant peer affiliation on s...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  62. How do children adapt their fairness norm? Evidence from computational...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  63. ECD-IMC: an integrated marketing communications model for selected Ph...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  64. Family Roles in the Risk of Serious Mental Illness Among Youth with Su...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  65. Promoting positive emotions and instilling concern for the needs of ot...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  66. Tracking Mental Wellbeing of Dutch Adolescents During the First Year o...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  67. Addressing safety issues along the way to school: Qualitative findings...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  68. Alcohol and tobacco use in Portuguese adolescents: The relationship wi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  69. Social contextual risk taking in adolescence
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  70. Youth perceptions of prevention norms and peer violence perpetration a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  71. Social influence in adolescence as a double-edged sword
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  72. The Role of Emotion and Learning in Decision‐making Situations During ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  73. The Contribution of Peer Values to Children’s Values and Behavior
    Go to citation Crossref Google ScholarPub Med
  74. What does it mean to be susceptible to influence? A brief primer on pe...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  75. Characterization of the Core Determinants of Social Influence From a C...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  76. Do peer and parental norms influence media content-induced cyber aggre...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  77. Social avoidance motivation tendency linked to face processing ability...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  78. The role of perceived threats on mental health, social, and neurocogni...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  79. Peer feedback decreases impulsive choice in adolescents with and witho...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  80. Social information use in adolescents with conduct problems and varyin...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  81. Why didn't you tag me?!: Social exclusion from Instagram posts hurts, ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  82. Hierarchy in the cockpit: How captains influence the decision-making o...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  83. Sanctions, Perceptions, and Crime
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  84. Trends in Influencer Marketing: A Review and Bibliometric Analysis
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  85. The Impact of Migration Status on Adolescents’ Mental Health during CO...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  86. Influences of Social Learning in Individual Perception and Decision Ma...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  87. More Stereotyping but More Sympathy: Youth Political Socialization in ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  88. Adolescents’ experiences of the information they received about the co...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  89. Maturing Out of Victimization: Extending the Theory of Psychosocial Ma...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  90. Social deficits in autism spectrum disorder: A perspective from the so...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  91. Preference uncertainty accounts for developmental effects on susceptib...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  92. Majority and popularity effects on norm formation in adolescence
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  93. Toward understanding the functions of peer influence: A summary and sy...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  94. Adolescent decision making: A decade in review
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  95. Feasibility of a serious game coupled with a contact-based session led...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  96. Gender-Specific Changes in Life Satisfaction After the COVID-19–Relate...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  97. Adolescent Health Series: Adolescent neurocognitive development in Wes...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  98. Early Adolescents’ Food Selection After Evaluating the Healthiness of ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  99. Lies have long legs cheating, peer scrutiny and loyalty in teams
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  100. Peer Presence Effect on Numerosity and Phonological Comparisons in 4th...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  101. An Investigation of Factors Influencing the Risk Perception and Revisi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  102. Adolescent risk-taking in the context of exploration and social influe...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  103. The importance of belonging and the avoidance of social risk taking in...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  104. Social norms in adolescent risk engagement and recommendation
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  105. The Co-development of Friends’ Delinquency with Adolescents’ Delinquen...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  106. The influence of peers on adolescents&rsquo; risk-taking behavior and ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  107. Adolescent psychopathological profiles and the outcome of the COVID-19...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  108. Applying Behavioral Economics to Improve Adolescent and Young Adult He...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  109. Individual Risk Perceptions and Behavior*
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  110. Investigating the association between family dysfunction and peer anti...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  111. Would you like to shop via mobile app technology? The technology accep...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  112. Sensation Seeking, Peer Influence, and Risk-Taking Behavior in Adolesc...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  113. The Interplay Between Adolescent Friendship Quality and Resilient Func...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  114. Neurobiological Sensitivity to Social Rewards and Punishments Moderate...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  115. Where You Lead, I Will Follow: Exploring Sibling Similarity in Brain a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  116. Neural Correlates of Conflicting Social Influence on Adolescent Risk T...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  117. Centennials, FOMO, and Loneliness: An Investigation of the Impact of S...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  118. Social experience calibrates neural sensitivity to social feedback dur...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  119. Lifestyle and psychosocial correlates of oral hygiene practice among I...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  120. Kids helping kids: The lived experience of adolescents who support fri...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  121. Whose Norms, Whose Prejudice? The Dynamics of Perceived Group Norms an...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  122. Severe Social Anxiety Among Adolescents During COVID-19 Lockdown
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  123. Risk Perception and Risk Behavior in the Context of Transportation
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  124. Barriers and facilitators on the pathway to mental health care among 1...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  125. Exploring the role of egocentrism and fear of missing out on online ri...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  126. Treatment Considerations for Adolescents Who Exhibit Cognitive/Communi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  127. Do They Really Mean It? Children’s Inference of Speaker Intentions and...
    Go to citation Crossref Google ScholarPub Med
  128. Prosocial Influence and Opportunistic Conformity in Adolescents and Yo...
    Go to citation Crossref Google ScholarPub Med
  129. No evidence for a relationship between social closeness and similarity...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  130. Psychosocial school environment and health risk behaviours of adolesce...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  131. Neural sensitivity to conflicting attitudes supports greater conformit...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  132. Neural and behavioral signatures of social evaluation and adaptation i...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  133. Susceptibility to prosocial and antisocial influence in adolescence
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  134. Understanding brain development: Investing in young adolescents’ cogni...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  135. The effects of social deprivation on adolescent development and mental...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  136. Neural processes during adolescent risky decision making are associate...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  137. The Effect of YouTube eWOM on Consumer Buying Interest
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  138. Adolescent brain and the natural allure of digital media

    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  139. Amplified Concern for Social Risk in Adolescence: Development and Vali...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  140. Neural correlates of social influence on risk perception during develo...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  141. Social Cognitions and Mental Health as Predictors of Adolescents’ Mind...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  142. Behavioral and Neural Pathways Supporting the Development of Prosocial...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  143. Web 3.0’da Çocukların Sanal Mahremiyetine Adli Bir Bakış
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  144. Discrimination Increases the Association Between Parental and Adolesce...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  145. The impact of becoming a parent about the perception of Internet risk ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  146. Facilitators and Barriers for a Good Night’s Sleep Among Adolescents
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  147. Self and Others in Adolescence
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  148. Decision-Making
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  149. Foundations of addictive problems in adolescents: Neurobiological fact...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  150. Willing to do what others won't: Narcissism, risk-taking, and social a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  151. Health in the school: perceptions of being adolescent
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  152. Neuroenhancement using transcranial electrical brain stimulation in ad...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  153. Social exclusion affects working memory performance in young adolescen...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  154. Identifying food marketing to teenagers: a scoping review
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  155. The CogBIAS longitudinal study of adolescence: cohort profile and stab...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  156. When do peers influence adolescent males' risk taking? Examining decis...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  157. Health in the school: perceptions of being adolescent
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  158. Age and Gender Effects in Sensitivity to Social Rewards in Adolescents...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  159. Making Headway for Discussions About Concussions: Experiences of Forme...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  160. The Protective Effects of Supportive Parenting on the Relationship Bet...
    Go to citation Crossref Google ScholarPub Med
  161. Incorporating the social context into neurocognitive models of adolesc...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  162. Direct and reflected self-concept show increasing similarity across ad...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  163. Turning a Blind Eye? Punishment of Friends and Unfamiliar Peers After ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  164. Analysis of the Relationship between Emotional Intelligence, Resilienc...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  165. Improving shared health decision making for children and adolescents w...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  166. Hunting for What Works: Adolescents in Addiction Treatment
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  167. Does Interest in Smoking Affect Youth Selection of Pro-smoking Videos?...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  168. Peer-Influence on Risk-Taking in Male Adolescents with Mild to Borderl...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  169. How Does Individualism-Collectivism Relate to Bullying Victimisation?
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  170. Developmental perspectives on risky and impulsive choice
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  171. Developmental trajectory of social influence integration into perceptu...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  172. The Relationship between Moral Climate of Sports and the Moral Behavio...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  173. Applying Behavioral Theory to Increase Mindfulness Practice Among Adol...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  174. The Diversity Paradox: Opportunities and Challenges of “Contact in Con...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  175. Developmental patterns of change in the influence of safe and risky pe...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  176. Communicating with children and adolescents about the risk of natural ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  177. Trends in repeated pregnancy among adolescents in the Philippines from...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  178. 框架效应的应用研究及其应用技巧
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  179. Media use and brain development during adolescence
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  180. What lies beneath peer acceptance in adolescence? Exploring the role o...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  181. Age differences in the prosocial influence effect
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  182. Differential effects of parent and peer presence on neural correlates ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  183. Depressive and socially anxious symptoms, psychosocial maturity, and r...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  184. Family conflict shapes how adolescents take risks when their family is...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  185. Avoiding Social Risk in Adolescence
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  186. Not just social sensitivity: Adolescent neural suppression of social f...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  187. Combined effects of peer presence, social cues, and rewards on cogniti...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  188. Studying individual differences in human adolescent brain development
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  189. Social Influence on Positive Youth Development: A Developmental Neuros...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  190. How to measure information similarity in online social networks: A cas...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  191. The CogBIAS longitudinal study protocol: cognitive and genetic factors...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  192. Examining the relationships between body image, eating attitudes, BMI,...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  193. Risk in Science Instruction
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  194. Differences in the early stages of social information processing for a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  195. Age-related differences in social influence on risk perception depend ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  196. Heightened activity in social reward networks is associated with adole...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  197. Flexing dual-systems models: How variable cognitive control in childre...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  198. Neural Correlates of Social Influence on Risk Taking and Substance Use...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  199. Social seeking declines in young adolescents
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  200. Peers Influence Prosocial Behavior in Adolescent Males with Autism Spe...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  201. Neural Correlates of Social Influence Among Cannabis Users
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  202. But is helping you worth the risk? Defining Prosocial Risk Taking in a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  203. The peer's point of view: Observing a peer performing an action enhanc...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  204. Hanging Out With the Right Crowd: Peer Influence on Risk‐Taking Behavi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  205. Brief Report: A Comparison of the Preference for Viewing Social and No...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  206. The effect of social rank feedback on risk taking and associated rewar...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  207. What motivates adolescents? Neural responses to rewards and their infl...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  208. The development of social cognition in adolescence: An integrated pers...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  209. Letting the good times roll: adolescence as a period of reduced inhibi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  210. Is there heightened sensitivity to social reward in adolescence?
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  211. Neural mechanisms of sensitivity to peer information in young adult ca...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  212. Analyse social influence on student motivation based on social activit...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  213. Neural correlates of prosocial peer influence on public goods game don...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  214. Cultural Socialization Across Contexts: Family–Peer Congruence and Ado...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  215. Dealing With Uncertainty: Testing Risk- and Ambiguity-Attitude Across ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  216. Do risks matter? Variable and person-centered approaches to adolescent...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  217. Neural mechanisms of social influence in adolescence
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  218. Impact of ASUMA Intervention on HIV Risk Behaviors among Puerto Rican ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  219. Adolescent neurodevelopment of cognitive control and risk-taking in ne...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  220. Traveling with children: beyond car seat safety
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  221. Traveling with children: beyond car seat safety
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  222. Adolescence as a Sensitive Period of Brain Development
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  223. The neuroscience of adolescent decision-making
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get access

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:

APS members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.

APS members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.