Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Myth of Man the Hunter: Women’s contribution to the hunt across ethnographic contexts

  • Abigail Anderson,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Seattle Pacific University, Seattle, Washington, United States of America

  • Sophia Chilczuk,

    Roles Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Seattle Pacific University, Seattle, Washington, United States of America

  • Kaylie Nelson,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Seattle Pacific University, Seattle, Washington, United States of America

  • Roxanne Ruther,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Department of Biology, Seattle Pacific University, Seattle, Washington, United States of America

  • Cara Wall-Scheffler

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing

    cwallsch@spu.edu

    Affiliations Department of Biology, Seattle Pacific University, Seattle, Washington, United States of America, Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington, United States of America

Abstract

The sexual division of labor among human foraging populations has typically been recognized as involving males as hunters and females as gatherers. Recent archeological research has questioned this paradigm with evidence that females hunted (and went to war) throughout the Homo sapiens lineage, though many of these authors assert the pattern of women hunting may only have occurred in the past. The current project gleans data from across the ethnographic literature to investigate the prevalence of women hunting in foraging societies in more recent times. Evidence from the past one hundred years supports archaeological finds from the Holocene that women from a broad range of cultures intentionally hunt for subsistence. These results aim to shift the male-hunter female-gatherer paradigm to account for the significant role females have in hunting, thus dramatically shifting stereotypes of labor, as well as mobility.

Introduction

The notion of separate subsistence roles for females and males in forager societies has been a long-standing paradigm for a wide range of social science disciplines, as well as in the public sphere, and include influential texts such as Man the Hunter [1], and Woman the Gatherer [2]. This viewpoint further purports that females engage in the majority of child-rearing activities, which is aligned with the slow-moving pace of gathering. Such assumed sex-specific gender roles in subsistence activities are commonly construed with additional gendered traits such as human men being less emotional and more aggressive, while human women tend to demonstrate more nurturing behavior and a focused interest in children. While we have known these patterns are culturally defined and thus variable for over a century now (e.g. [35]), it is only recently that the data are building to move a more accurate paradigm of subsistence flexibility into discourse [68].

One of the most prominent discoveries recently includes a 9,000 year old burial located in the Andean highland area of Wilamaya Patjxa in Peru [9]. The burial included an adult female alongside a hunting toolkit consisting of stone projectiles as well as animal processing equipment [9]. Researchers typically presume that stone projectiles buried alongside males are hunting tools but are less persuaded when projectiles are associated with females; the specific assemblage clearly evidenced hunting in this case. In their own review of the literature, Haas et al. [9] examined burials in the Americas from the Late Pleistocene to the Early Holocene period, identifying eleven females from ten sites who were associated with big-game hunting tools. By using a probability analysis of all twenty-seven sites which had evidence of big-game hunting, Haas et al. determined that females made up a “nontrivial” amount of big-game hunters across the Americas [9]. In fact, their analysis suggested that females represented up to fifty percent of big game hunters from the Americas prehistorically.

In addition to tools generally associated with big-game hunting being conferred to males, tools associated with warfare are also consistently assumed to occupy burials of males [10]. In 2017, a well-known burial in Sweden revealed an individual alongside weapons and equipment associated with high-ranking Viking warriors [11]. The individual was assumed to be male considering the historical interpretation of the prevalence of male warriors, but genomics confirmed that the individual was a female. In addition, archaeologists discovered a 2,500 year old burial site that contained four females associated with weapons and warrior equipment [12]. The age of the females ranged from 12 or 13 years old to 40 to 50 years old and were believed to be a part of the nomadic group known as Scythians [12]. Scythian women were warriors in their culture as supported by the fact that one-third of the females in this society were buried with weapons [12]. The purpose of these anecdotes is two-fold. Firstly, researcher bias shapes science’s interpretation of data, and it behooves each generation of scientists to ensure that paradigms fit the existing data. Secondly, the number of anecdotes on females taking up weapons and tools interpreted as “violent” is extensive across time as well as space [13, 14], making such examples more of a pattern of female behavior than anecdotal [10].

The discovery and reanalysis of the human burials from a range of geographic and temporal situations has prompted further research into the organizational structure of many Holocene societies. The question of the division of labor being sex (biological denotation, often based on external cues like genitalia) or gender (social denotation, often based on biological cues, but shaped by the intersection of social norms and personal expression) specific among human populations remains insufficiently researched and undetermined [7, 15]. Here we aim to close some of the research gap between female and male subsistence roles by gleaning, with as much resolution as possible, information on subsistence strategies among forager groups around the world. Our hypothesis is that the majority (i.e., more than half) of hunter-gatherer communities do expect females to contribute to hunting strategies. Such findings would continue the challenge to long-held perceptions of sex-specific gender roles within foraging subsistence labor [6, 7, 9, 10, 16].

Methods

The relationship between subsistence activity and gender was compiled by reading ethnographic reports of foraging societies. A list of potential foraging societies along with their location and type of subsistence activity was first constructed using D-PLACE, the Database of Places, Languages, Culture and Environment [17]. This database is based on the ethnographic atlas by Lewis Binford [18] and contains detailed information on over 1,400 human societies. In order to reasonably sample across geographic areas, 391 foraging societies from around the globe were chosen to investigate further. Of the 391 different societies the continent, location, ecosystem, and primary subsistence activity were obtained from D-PLACE and recorded. Each foraging society was then investigated by searching through the original references cited in D-PLACE [17], Binford [18], and by searching digitized databases and archives. Multiple reports featuring the same foraging societies were read to ensure accuracy and reliability. Of the 391 foraging societies, explicit data on hunting was obtained for 63 of the societies (Fig 1; Table 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. World map of the locations of 63 different foraging societies analyzed.

The map is in the public domain and can be attributed to Petr Dlouhy, https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:World_Map_Blank_-_with_blue_sea.svg.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0287101.g001

thumbnail
Table 1. Cultural groups.

Foraging societies grouped by continent with foraging society name and approximate location. An asterisk denotes evidence of women hunting.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0287101.t001

Data used for this study included reports on what, when, and how hunting occurred in the cultural group. Ethnographic reports needed to include explicit information, in the form of tables or sentences that females went on hunting trips, and were involved in tracking, locating animals, and helping with the killing if applicable. Given that there is a difference between the phrase ‘women went hunting’ and ‘women accompanied the hunters’ it should be noted that we were looking for phrases along the lines of ‘women were hunting’ or ‘women killed animals,’ not references to the idea that women might be accompanying men “only” to carry the kills home, though obviously this does happen as well (e.g. [19]). Specific contributions such how much killing took place, and total calories from female-only kills were not written about frequently enough to warrant their assessment here.

If women were hunting, it was further investigated to see if the hunting was done purposely, whether women would go out with the intention to hunt, or whether women were hunting spontaneously (i.e. “opportunistically”); this might occur when women may have been doing a different task but if the opportunity arose, they would kill an animal. This was determined by explicit statements in the published literature or by a judgment based on the descriptions. Women’s involvement in hunting was determined by written documentation explicitly stating that women were hunting in that particular foraging society or were excluded and in some instances even forbidden to hunt. The most important subsistence activity was also compared to the relative frequency of women hunting. Additionally, the type of the game hunted was assembled into three categories of small, medium, and large. The type of game was defined by the relative size of the prey, hunting toolkit that was used, or if size was explicitly stated in the literature. For example, when looking at the Tiwi society of Australia, a study reported that Tiwi women regularly hunted small animals while the hunting of large game was a man’s activity, suggesting that women were involved in hunting small game only [20]. In instances where the type of game was not explicitly stated, it was determined from other clues in the report. For example, accounts of the Matses from the Amazon state that the women would strike their prey with large sticks and machetes, which would account for large game whereas other societies had documentation of small digging sticks or the killing of rodents, suggesting the prevalence of small game hunting [21]. The prevalence of women hunting with children and dogs was also recorded and analyzed based on statements in the literature.

Compiled data were analyzed to determine the frequency of females hunting, the type of hunting accomplished, as well as the relative size of game.

Results

Data were compiled from literature on sixty-three different foraging societies across the globe. These included nineteen different foraging societies from North America, six from South America, twelve from Africa, fifteen from Australia, five from Asia and six from the Oceanic region (Fig 1 & Table 1). Of the 63 different foraging societies, 50 (79%) of the groups had documentation on women hunting. Of the 50 societies that had documentation on women hunting, 41 societies had data on whether women hunting was intentional or opportunistic. Of the latter, 36 (87%) of the foraging societies described women’s hunting as intentional, as opposed to the 5 (12%) societies that described hunting as opportunistic. In societies where hunting is considered the most important subsistence activity, women actively participated in hunting 100% of the time.

The type of game women hunted was variable based on the society. Of the 50 foraging societies that have documentation on women hunting, 45 (90%) societies had data on the size of game that women hunted. Of these, 21 (46%) hunt small game, 7 (15%) hunt medium game, 15 (33%) hunt large game and 2 (4%) of these societies hunt game of all sizes. In societies where women only hunted opportunistically, small game was hunted 100% of the time. In societies where women were hunting intentionally, all sizes of game were hunted, with large game pursued the most. Of the 36 foraging societies that had documentation of women purposefully hunting, 5 (13%) reported women hunting with dogs and 18 (50%) of the societies included data on women (purposefully) hunting with children. Women hunting with dogs and children also occurred in opportunistic situations as well.

Discussion

Here we investigated whether noted trends of non-gendered hunting labor known from the archaeological record continued into more recent, ethnographic periods. The descriptive sample described here is sufficient to warrant the conclusion that women in foraging societies across the world participate in hunting during more recent time periods, a finding that makes sense given women’s general morphology and physiology [16, 80]. The prevalence of data on women hunting directly opposes the common belief that women exclusively gather while men exclusively hunt, and further, that the implicit sexual division of labor of ‘hunter/gatherer’ is misapplied. Given that this bimodal paradigm has influenced the interpretation of archeological evidence, which includes the reluctance to distinguish projectile tools found within female burials as intended for hunting (or fighting) [9, 10, 81], this paper joins others in urging the necessity to reevaluate archeological evidence, to reassess ethnographic evidence, to question the dichotomous use of ‘hunting and gathering,’ and to deconstruct the general “man the hunter” narrative [6, 7, 80].

Based on the data supporting the existence of female hunters, certain skills and practices within foraging societies allow women to be successful hunters. Of the 63 foraging societies with clear descriptions of hunting strategies, 79% of them demonstrated female hunting. The widespread presence of female hunting suggests that females play an instrumental role in hunting, further adding to the data that women contribute disproportionately to the total caloric intake of many foraging groups [15, 28, 82, 83]. Additionally, over 70% of hunting done by females is interpreted as intentional, meaning that females play an active and important role in hunting—and the teaching of hunting—even if they use different tools and employ different acquisition strategies. For example, among the Aka, women’s participation in net-hunting was required, whereas men’s participation was not [28].

These data suggest that females not only prepare to hunt and actively pursue game, but also that they are skilled in the practice. This is supported by both the existence of a specialized toolkit, as well as distinct strategies compared to their male counterparts, potentially relating to different training regimes, as well as different cultural norms surrounding the hunting, processing, and eating of meat (e.g., [24]). For example, the tools used by Agta women from the Philippines are remarkably different compared to Agta men [21, 84]. Whereas Agta men heavily rely on a consistent strategy of bow and arrows [84], women are much more likely to have personal preferences and show variation. Some women prefer hunting only with knives, a few women use bow and arrows, and others use a combination of the two [84]. Among the Aka, women are also flexible—carrying nets, but also spears, machetes, and cross bows. Even when nets are primarily used in hunting, sometimes women will wield the nets and sometimes men will wield the nets [28].

In addition to weapon choices, women further employ a greater flexibility of hunting strategies compared to men. For example, women hunt with a variety of partners, including their husbands, other women, children, dogs, as well as hunting alone [7, 21]. In contrast, men primarily hunt alone, with a single partner (their wife), or with a dog [18, 22]. Among the Agta, women might hunt in teams, and largely hunt during the day, though they might also hunt unaccompanied [84]. Agta men predominately hunt alone or with one other person if they are hunting at night in the forest [84]. Further, dogs are important to Agta women hunters, while the men typically only are accompanied by dogs when also hunting alongside women; the number of available dogs is a crucial factor in determining the frequency of Agta women hunters, in which a minimum of three mature dogs are typical for success [84]. Among the Aka, the size of the hunting net and the range of travel can depend on what else the women are carrying, whether a child is present, and whether they also have a basket [28].

As might be expected based on both tool and technique specializations, females maintain specializations for certain animals. American Cree women hunt pelt-animals alone and in groups [24]. Additionally, Mbuti women from the Congo hunt using nets [7], and Aka women also hunt using nets, more than men hunt using nets. The difference between these two populations is that among the Mbuti, women usually are flushing out the game, whereas among the Aka, women are usually capturing the game [28]. The Aché and Ju/’hoan women participate in hunting by tracking [7]. The Peruvian Matses and Mossapoula Aka women actively hunt with their husbands in order to increase overall hunting yields [7, 75]. Given that social norms determine how tools are made, and by whom [85], these specialized skills warrant much more attention by the literature. This would allow information on exactly who and how the tools are made, as well as to whom and how skills are being disseminated, can be used to uncover the means by which tasks are taken on by all the members of a group [86].

Suggestions that children are put in danger by accompanying hunts [74] can be mediated with current literature on the numerous ways in which infants and children are carried during expeditions by parents and alloparents. The importance of infants remaining with adults (versus being parked) is an important part of our lineage [87, 88], with children accompanying the wide range of expeditions consistently evidenced in the archaeological [89], as well as the ethnographic record [90]. Data explicitly mentioning that infants are carried while hunting exist for the Aka [91] and the Awa [92], as well as for foraging bouts that might result in opportunistic hunting (e.g., among the Batek [93] and Nukak [94]). Among both the Hadza and the Aka, children (potentially as young as age three) accompany adults on over 15% of hunting trips [95]. The idea that women are hindered by childcare and thus cannot hunt is an area where increasing data collection and thoughtful interpretation is lending a much richer lens to our understanding of human mobility strategies.

Women in foraging societies across the world historically participated and continue to participate in hunting regardless of child-bearing status. The collected data on women hunting directly opposes the traditional paradigm that women exclusively gather and men exclusively hunt and further elucidates the diversity and flexibility of human subsistence cultures [96]. Because the hunter/gatherer paradigm has prevented the recognition of contributions by women to hunting, a new framework would enable past and future discoveries to be evaluated in the context of female hunters. Furthermore, the term “forager,” as suggested by Brightman [24], should be used to acknowledge the non-sexual division of labor concerning hunting and gathering, in order to develop an inclusive framework for understanding human culture [9].

Supporting information

S1 Table. Data table.

This is the data table used for this analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0287101.s001

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to the attention to detail of our two reviewers, whose comments have markedly improved the manuscript.

References

  1. 1. Lee RB, DeVore I. Man the Hunter. New York: Routledge; 1968.
  2. 2. Dahlberg F. Woman the Gatherer. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1983.
  3. 3. Dunsworth HM. Expanding the evolutionary explanations for sex differences in the human skeleton. Evol Anthropol. 2020;29(3):108–16. pmid:32359124
  4. 4. Mead M. Sex and temperament in three primitive societies: The malleability of gender roles. In: Carter GL, editor. Empirical Approaches to Sociology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon; 1998. p. 453–6.
  5. 5. Hamlin KA. From Eve to Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2014.
  6. 6. Fuentes A. Searching for the "roots" of masculinity in primates and the human evolutionary past. Curr Anthropol. 2021;62(Supplement 23):S13–S25.
  7. 7. Khorasani DG, Lee S-H. Women in human evolution redux. In: Willermet C, Lee S-H, editors. Evaluating evidence in biological anthropology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2020. p. 11–34.
  8. 8. Horrell S, Humphries J, Weisdorf J. Beyond the male breadwinner: Life-cycle living standards of intact and disrupted English working families, 1260–1850. Economic History Review. 2022;75(2):530–60.
  9. 9. Haas R, Watson J, Buonasera T, Southon J, Chen JC, Noe S, et al. Female hunters of the early Americas. Science Advances. 2020;6(45):eabd0310. pmid:33148651
  10. 10. Doucette D. Decoding the gender bias: Inferences of atlatls in female mortuary contexts. In: Arnold B, Wicker NL, editors. Gender and the Archaeology of Death. AltaMira Press: Walnut Creek; 2001. p. 119–35.
  11. 11. Hedenstierna-Jonson C, Kjellstrom A, Zachrisson T, Krzewinska M, Sobrado V, Price N, et al. A female Viking warrior confirmed by genomics. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2017;164(4):853–60. pmid:28884802
  12. 12. Guliaev VI. Amazons in the Scythia: New finds at the Middle Don, Southern Russia. World Archaeology. 2003;31(1):112–25.
  13. 13. Toler PD. Women Warriors: An Unexpected History. Boston: Beacon Press; 2019.
  14. 14. Potholm CP. Hiding in Plain Sight: Women Warriors throughout Time and Space. Washington, D.C.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers; 2021.
  15. 15. Waguespack NM. The organization of male and female labor in foraging societies: Implications for Early Paleoindian Archaeology. Am Anthropol. 2005;107(4):666–76.
  16. 16. Wall-Scheffler CM. Women carry for less: Body size, pelvis width, loading position and energetics. Evolutionary Human Sciences. 2022;4:e36.
  17. 17. Kirby KR, Gray RD, Greenhill SJ, Jordan FM, Gomes-Ng S, Bibiko B, et al. D-PLACE: A global database of cultural, linguistic, and environmental diversity. PLoS ONE. 2016;11(7):e0158391. pmid:27391016
  18. 18. Binford LR. Constructing Frames of Reference: An Analytical Method for Archaeological Theory Building Using Hunter-Gatherer and Environmental Data Sets. Berkeley: University of California Press; 2001.
  19. 19. Hilton CE, Greaves RD. Age, sex and resource transport in Venezuelan foragers. In: Meldrum DJ, Hilton CE, editors. From Biped to Strider: The Emergence of Modern Human Walking, Running and Resource Transport. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2004. p. 163–81.
  20. 20. Goodall JC. Tiwi Wives: A Study of the Women of Melville Island, North Australia. Seattle: University of Washington Press; 1971.
  21. 21. Goodman MJ, Griffin PB, Estioko-Griffin AA, Grove JS. The compatibility of hunting and mothering among the Agta hunter-gatherers of the Philippines. Sex Roles. 1985;12(11/12):1199–209.
  22. 22. Lee RB. The! Kung San: Men, Women, and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1979.
  23. 23. Singh KS. Gender roles in history: Women as hunters. Gender, Technology, and Development. 2001;5(1):113–24.
  24. 24. Brightman R. The sexual division of foraging labor: Biology, taboo, and gender politics. Comp Stud Soc Hist. 1996;38(4):687–729.
  25. 25. Ngima Mawoung G. Perceptions of hunting, gathering and fishing technqiues of the Bakola of the coastal region, Southern Cameroon. African Study Monographs. 2006;33:49–70.
  26. 26. Reyes-Garcia V, Diaz-Reviriego I, Duda R, Fernandez-Llamazares A, Gallois S. “Hunting otherwise”: Women’s hunting in two contemporary forager-horticulturalist societies. Hum Natr. 2020;31(203–221). pmid:32915412
  27. 27. Kitanishi K. Seasonal changes in the subsistence activities and food intake of the Aka hunter-gatherers in northeastern Congo. African Study Monographs. 1995;16(2):73–118.
  28. 28. Noss AJ, Hewlett BS. The contexts of female hunting in Central Africa. Am Anthropol. 2001;103(4):1024–40.
  29. 29. Terashima H. Hunting life of the Bambote: An anthropological study of hunter-gathers in a wooded savanna. Senri Ethnological Studies. 1980;6(223–268).
  30. 30. Bailey RC, Aunger R. Net hunters vs archers: Variation in women’s subsistence strategies in the Ituri Forest. Human Evolution. 1989;17:273–97.
  31. 31. Fedders A, Salvadori C. Peoples and Cultures of Kenya. Nairobi: Transafrica; 1988.
  32. 32. Nyamanga P. The Kikuyu Community of Kenya.
  33. 33. Marlowe FW. The Hadza: Hunter-Gatherers of Tanzania. Oakland: University of California Press; 2010.
  34. 34. Lupa KD, Schmitt D. Upper Paleolithic net hunting, small prey exploitation, and women’s work effort: A view from the ethnographic and ethnoarchaeological record of the Congo Basin. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory. 2002;9(2):147–79.
  35. 35. Endicott K. The economy of the Batek of Malaysia: Annual and historical perspectives. Research in Economic Anthropology. 1984;6:29–52.
  36. 36. Van Der Sluys C. Use and trade of rattan by the Jahai in Hulu Perak, Malaysia. Akademika. 1996;48:63–83.
  37. 37. Panter-Brick C. Motherhood and subsistence work: The Tamang of rural Nepal. Hum Ecol. 1989;17(2):205–28. pmid:12283256
  38. 38. Lukas H. Can “They” Save “Us”, the Foragers? Indonesian and Thai Hunter-Gatherer Cultures under Threat from Outside. Bangkok: Office of the National Education Commission; 2004.
  39. 39. Mountford CP, Harvey A. Women of the Adnjamatana Tribe of the Northern Flinders Ranges, South Australia. Oceania. 1941;12(2):155–62.
  40. 40. Devitt J. Contemporary aboriginal women and subsistence in remote, arid Australia: University of Queensland; 1988.
  41. 41. O’Dea K. Traditional diet and food preferences of Australian Aboriginal hunter-gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1991;334(1270):233–41. pmid:1685581
  42. 42. Bird RB, Bird D. Why women hunt: Risk and contemporary foraging in a western desert aboriginal community. Curr Anthropol. 2008;49(4):655–93. pmid:19230267
  43. 43. Gunwinggu. DICE: Database for Indigenous Cultural Evolution. In: Missouri Uo, editor. December 5, 2021 ed2015.
  44. 44. Memmott P, Robins R, Stock E. What exactly is a fish trap? Methodological issues for the study of Aboriginal intertidal rock wall fish traps, Wellesley Island region, Gulf of Carpentaria, Australia. In: Conolly J, Campbell M, editors. Comoparative Island Archaeologies. Oxford: BAR Publishing; 2008. p. 47–67.
  45. 45. Willing T. Tukujana Nganyiurrukura Ngurra: All of Us Looking after Country Together. Broome: Karajarri Traditional Lands Association; 2014.
  46. 46. Boore RP. Savagery and Civilization: The Convergence of Europeans and Papuans Up to the Proclamation of the British New Guinea Protectorate in 1884. Wales: University of Wales Press; 2004.
  47. 47. Southon M, Elders KT. The Sea of Waubin: The Kaurareg and their marine environment. In: Peterson N, Rigsby B, editors. Customary Marine Tenure in Australia. Sydney: Sydney University Press; 2014. p. 351–67.
  48. 48. Lardil. DICE: Database for Indigenous Cultural Evolution2015.
  49. 49. Day WB. Aboriginal people of Darwin: The Longgrass people. Online Presentation: www.drbilldayanthropologist.com/resources/Longgrass%20people%20of%20Darwin%202012.pdf; 2012.
  50. 50. Tindale NB. Aboriginal Tribes of Australia. Los Angeles: University of California Press; 1974.
  51. 51. Hart CWM, Pilling AR, Goodall JC. The Tiwi of North Australia. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston; 1988.
  52. 52. Tonkinson R. The Mardudjara Aborigines: Living in the Dreamtime. Sydney: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston; 1978.
  53. 53. Clendon M. Worrorra. Adelaide: University of Adelaide Press; 2014.
  54. 54. Steward J. Basin-Plateau Aboriginal Sociopolitical Groups. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press; 1997.
  55. 55. Guemple L. Men and women, husbands and wives: The role of gender in tranditional Inuit society. Etudes Inuit Studies. 1986;10(1–2):9–24.
  56. 56. Boas F. The Central Eskimo Sixth Annual Report of the Bureau of American Ethnology, 1884–1885. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution; 1888.
  57. 57. Flannery R. Ellen Smallboy: Glimpses of a Cree Woman’s Life: McGill-Queen’s Press-MQUP; 1995.
  58. 58. Bodenhorn B. “I’m not the great hunter, my wife is”: Inupiat and anthropological models of gender. Etudes Inuit Studies. 1990;15(1/2):55–74.
  59. 59. Brown JK. Economic organization and the position of women among the Iroquois. Ethnohistory. 1970;17(3/4):151–67.
  60. 60. Issenman K. Sinews of Survival: The Living Legacy of Inuit Clothing. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press; 1997.
  61. 61. Faye PL. Notes on the Southern Maidu, American Archaeology and Ethnology Vol 30: 53: University of California 1923.
  62. 62. Flannery R. The position of women among the Mescalero Apache. Primitive Man. 1932;5:26–32.
  63. 63. Brightman R. Grateful Prey: Rock Cree Human-Animal Relationships. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1993.
  64. 64. Lubinski PM. The communal pronghorn hunt: A review of the ethnographic and archaeological evidence. Journal of California and Great Basin Anthropology. 1999;21(2):158–81.
  65. 65. Morris B. Hunting and the gnostic vision. The Society of Malawi Journal. 1995;48(2):26–46.
  66. 66. Landes R. The Obijwa Woman. New York: Norton Publishing; 1938.
  67. 67. Buffalohead PK. Farmers, warriors, traders: A fresh look at Ojibway women. Minnesota History. 1983;48(6):236–44.
  68. 68. Collins J. Understanding Tolowa Histories: Western Hegemonies and Native American Responses. United Kingdom: Taylor & Francis Publishing; 2014.
  69. 69. Williams JS. The Tongva of California. Buffalo, New York: Rosen Publishing; 2003.
  70. 70. Hewlett BS. Diverse contexts of human infancy. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall; 1996.
  71. 71. Ryes-Garcia V, Pyhala A. Hunter-Gatherers in a Changing World. London: Springer Nature Publishing; 2017.
  72. 72. Hyndman DC. Hunting and the classification of game animals among the Wopkaimin. Oceania. 1984;54(4):289–309.
  73. 73. Hurtado AM, Hill K. Seasonality in a foraging society: Variation in diet, work effort, fertility, and sexual division of labor among the Hiwi of Venezuela. J Anthropol Res. 1990;46(3):293–346.
  74. 74. Gurven M, Hill K. Why do men hunt? A reevaluation of “man the hunter” and the sexual division of labor. Curr Anthropol. 2009;50:51–74. pmid:19579355
  75. 75. Romanoff S. Women as hunters among the Matses of the Peruvian Amazon. Hum Ecol. 1983;11(3):339–43.
  76. 76. Hurtado AM, Hawkes K, Hill K, Kaplan H. Female subsistence strategies among Ache hunter-gatherers of Eastern Paraguay. Hum Ecol. 1985;13(1):1–28.
  77. 77. Hilton CE, Greaves RD. Seasonality and sex differences in travel distance and resource transport in Venezuelan foragers. Curr Anthropol. 2008;49(1):144–53.
  78. 78. Medinaceli A, Quinlan RJ. Firearms effects on Tsimane’ hunting and traditional knowledge in Bolivian Amazonia. Ethnobiology Letters. 2018;9(2):230–42.
  79. 79. Martens LK. Wild Harvest: Plants in the Hominin and Pre-Agrarian Human Worlds. United Kingdom: Oxbow Books; 2016.
  80. 80. Ocobock C, Lacy S. Woman the hunter: The physiological evidence. Am Anthropol. in press.
  81. 81. Conkey MW, Spector JD. Archaeology and the study of gender. Advances in Archaeological Method and Theory. 1984;7:1–38.
  82. 82. New J, Krasnow MM, Truxaw D, Gaulin SJC. Spatial adaptations for plant foraging: Women excel and calories count. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007;274:2679–84. pmid:17711835
  83. 83. Berbesque JC, Marlowe FW. Sex differences in food preferences of Hadza hunter-gatherers. Evolutionary Psychology. 2009;7(4):601–16.
  84. 84. Estioko-Griffin AA. Women as hunters: The case of an eastern Cagayan Agta group. In: Griffin PB, Estioko-Griffin AA, editors. The Agta of Northeastern Luzon: Recent Studies. Cebu City: San Carlos Publications; 1985. p. 18–32.
  85. 85. Sillitoe P. Made in Niugini: Technology in the Highlands of Papua New Guinea. London: British Museum Publications; 1988.
  86. 86. Zihlman A. The real females of human evolution. Evol Anthropol. 2012;21:270–6. pmid:23280923
  87. 87. Wall-Scheffler CM, Geiger K, Steudel-Numbers K. Infant carrying: The role of increased locomotory costs in early tool development. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2007;133(2):841–6. pmid:17427923
  88. 88. Ross C. Park or ride? Evolution of infant carrying in primates. Int J Primatol. 2001;22(5):749–71.
  89. 89. Milks A, Lew-Levy S, Lavi N, Friesem DE, Reckin R. Hunter-gatherer children in the past: An archaeological review. J Anthropol Archaeol. 2021;64:101369.
  90. 90. Pretelli I, Ringen E, Lew-Levy S. Foraging complexity and the evolution of childhood. Science Advances. 2022;8:eabn9889. pmid:36223468
  91. 91. Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL. Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366:1168–78. pmid:21357239
  92. 92. Politis GG, Costa Angrizani R, Hernando A, Gonzalez-Ruibal A, Beserra Coelho EM. Quarenta e oito saidas: Etno-arqueologia das saidas diarias de forrageio dos Awa. In: Hernando A, Beserra Coelho EM, editors. Estudos Sobre os Awa. Sao Luis-Maranhao: Edufma; 2013. p. 155–85.
  93. 93. Lye TP. Knowledge, forest and hunter-gatherer movement: The Batek of Pahang, Malaysia: University of Hawai’i; 1997.
  94. 94. Politis GG. Nukak: Ethnoarchaeology of an Amazonian People. New York: Routledge; 2007.
  95. 95. Lew-Levy S, Crittenden A, Boyette AH, Mabulla IA, Hewlett BS, Lamb ME. Inter- and intra- cultural variation in learning-through-participation among Hadza and BaYaka forager children and adolescents from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo. Journal of Psychology in Africa. 2019;29(4):309–18.
  96. 96. Warren G. Is there such a thing as hunter-gatherer archaeology? Heritage. 2021;4(2):794–810.