Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

American and German attitudes towards cow-calf separation on dairy farms

  • Gesa Busch ,

    gesabusch@web.de

    Current Address: Agriculture and Food economics, Faculty of Science and Technology, Free University of Bolzano, South Tyrol, Italy

    Affiliations Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada, Marketing for Food and Agricultural Products, Department of Agricultural Economics and Rural Development, Faculty of Agricultural Sciences, Georg-August-University Göttingen, Niedersachsen, Germany

  • Daniel M. Weary,

    Affiliation Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

  • Achim Spiller,

    Affiliation Marketing for Food and Agricultural Products, Department of Agricultural Economics and Rural Development, Faculty of Agricultural Sciences, Georg-August-University Göttingen, Niedersachsen, Germany

  • Marina A. G. von Keyserlingk

    Affiliation Animal Welfare Program, Faculty of Land and Food Systems, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

Abstract

Public concerns regarding the quality of life of farm animals are often focused on specific practices such as separating the cow and calf immediately after birth. The available scientific literature provides some evidence in support of this practice (including reduced acute responses to separation when it does occur), as well as evidence of disadvantages (such as increased risk of uterine disease in cows). The aim of this study is to systematically examine public views around this practice. Specifically, this study analyzes the views of American and German citizens to separation of cow and calf at birth using a quantitative segmentation approach. Although the majority of participants opposed early separation, a small proportion of our sample supported the practice. According to participants’ preference for early and later separation and their evaluation of different arguments for both practices, three clusters were identified. US participants were more likely to support early separation compared to German participants. The arguments presented for and against both practices caused different reactions in the three clusters, but did not appear to sway the opinions of most participants. The results show considerable opposition to the practice of early separation in large parts of the sample and suggest that the dairy industry should consider approaches to address this concern.

Introduction

In recent years there has been increasing public concern regarding the quality of life of farm animals [1,2,3]. The majority of work to date addressing citizens’ evaluation of contentious farming practices has taken place in Europe but this type of work is now gaining traction in North America [4,5]. Primary public concerns are in relation to restriction of movement (e.g. sows in gestation stalls [6]; laying hens in cages [7]), painful procedures (e.g. dehorning of dairy calves [8] and castration of piglets [9]) or lack of natural behavior (e.g. dairy cows access to pasture [10]). Some people working within agriculture have argued that these criticisms are unwarranted given that the public lacks knowledge of farming practices; from this perspective, criticisms will wane if the public is educated about agricultural production methods and the context of their use [11]. Alternatively, critiques of animal agriculture may reflect a lack of shared values surrounding agricultural production [12], making educational efforts unlikely to succeed in heading off criticisms about husbandry practices [13].

Political reactions to these criticisms have varied with some countries introducing legislation specific to farm animal welfare (e.g. implementation of national laws prohibiting tail docking and teeth clipping in piglets in Sweden and Norway; [14]), while other countries have relied on industry led initiatives (e.g. Canada; [15]).

Compared to the pork and poultry industries, dairy farming has received less attention, perhaps because dairy has a more positive public image than other livestock industries [16,17,18,19]. Some have argued that this positive perception of the dairy industry is a direct consequence of dairy marketing strategies and the higher visibility of cows in the countryside through open barns and pasture access [20]. However, some practices in dairy farming are also being increasingly discussed in the public.

Separating the calf from the cow shortly after birth is a routine practice on dairy farms around the world. In brief, calves are separated from their dams within a few hours following birth and then housed in a separate location and fed artificially (with milk or milk replacer). Advocates of this practice argue that it prevents cow and calf from forming a strong bond, and thus reduces the stress associated with separation when it does occur. The results from a series of studies on the short-term acute affects support this claim (e.g. [21,22,23]). Other arguments in support of early separation include better supervision of calf colostrum and milk intake [24]. One economic argument frequently presented by farmers is that the practice facilitates harvesting the maximum amount of milk available for sale [25]. Moreover, currently used housing and husbandry systems are not designed for keeping cow-calf pairs, making a change in practice difficult. Others have also argued that immediate separation reduces the risk of disease transfer to the calf, including paratuberculosis [26].

Arguments for later separation emphasize allowing more natural living of cow and calf [25], including emotional benefits for both [27]. Furthermore, some research has shown biological benefits for calves, including better weight gains [28] and fewer bouts of diarrhea when separated later [21], as well as improved udder [29] and uterine health [24] for cows.

The topic of early cow-calf separation can evoke strong negative responses from the public. Walker et al. [30] reported that 90% of their sample believed that the separation of animal parent and offspring results in the animal grieving. From the public’s perspective, when they are made aware of this practice, early cow-calf separation appears to have little support from those not working in the dairy industry [13,31].

Citizen perceptions of animal welfare are more diverse than farmers [32], so it is likely that there is no unified ‘public opinion’ on animal welfare topics. Segmentation, a tool used in consumer research to detect target groups for special commercial products, may be useful to determine diverse opinions and for garnering insights on particularly contentious animal welfare concerns. For example, Vanhonacker and Verbeke [2] argue in favor of this approach in animal welfare research as drawing conclusions from sample means is likely to obscure important voices, including those very concerned and those that are unconcerned with the issue. Other work has also shown differences in segments according to consumer and citizen attitudes towards animal products or animal production systems in different countries (e.g. Brazil [33]; USA [5,34]; Belgium, Poland, Denmark and Germany [4]; Netherlands [35]; Belgium [36]). Te Velde et al. [32] also provided evidence in support of segmentation and argued that research into the values and norms of different groups within society is needed to build a robust societal contract between farmers and consumers.

We are unaware of any work that has addressed the topic of cow-calf separation using a quantitative approach. The available literature on contentious practices within the dairy industry has focused on the acceptance and evaluation of the whole dairy sector (e.g. [16,17,37]) or on qualitative approaches to particular practices (e.g. [38,39]). The current study addresses this gap by analyzing the distribution of citizen attitudes towards a contested production practice in dairy farming using a quantitative method. We focus on the common but contentious practice of cow-calf separation in the hours immediately following birth.

A qualitative study by Ventura et al. [31] used a convenience sample of North Americans to examine the views of people from inside and outside the dairy industry on the issue of separation of the dairy cow from her calf immediately following birth. These authors identified six primary themes in the qualitative responses provided by 163 participants in the study: cow and calf emotions, calf health, cow health and production, a natural life, dissatisfactions with industry motivations and the changeability of dairy farming systems. These six themes (and the related arguments) were used to build the survey for this study. Specifically, we addressed the following objectives: 1) When do citizens think that cows and calves should be separated in dairy farming, 2) Can citizens be segmented according to their attitudes towards the different arguments of cow-calf separation and if so, what variables can be used to profile these groups and, 3) Does the provision of information in the form of arguments about the issue lead to a shift in participants’ opinion? The results are hoped to help those working within the dairy industry and policy makers to develop socially sustainable dairy farming practices.

Furthermore, values are influenced by cultural norms within societies [16] thus differences between countries may exist if values differ [15]. Vanhonacker and Verbeke [2] suggested that “nationality is not a key-characteristic for pro welfare behavior”, but little work to date has examined responses to welfare issues in residents of different countries, and none has done so in relation to early cow-calf separation. Thus our objective 4) was to draw upon an American and German sample to assess whether participants vary from these two countries.

Materials and methods

We conducted an online survey with US and German citizens. The Behavioral Research Ethics Board at the University of British Columbia, Canada approved the study. Participants were explicitly informed that all data is treated confidentially and that only anonymous questionnaires were used. Participants had to consent to take part in the study and could withdraw consent at any point by simply closing their Internet browser.

Survey design

A quantitative questionnaire was designed initially in the English language. For the German sample, the questionnaire was translated into German. To ensure the quality and validity of the translation it was back-translated into English by a German speaking researcher familiar with dairy farming but not involved in the study, and then adjusted as needed.

The main part of the survey contained 22 arguments that were based on the six themes (and 12 subthemes) identified by Ventura et al. [31] (see Table 1). These arguments provide information about the topic of separation in order to test for effects on opinion about the issue. For the current study we developed two arguments for each of the subthemes based on the original quotes from Ventura et al. [31]. For the subthemes “Dishonesty/Wrong focus” and “Profit motives” (part of the theme “Dissatisfaction with industry motives”) only one item was used to avoid redundancy given that similar arguments were presented. To reduce acquiescence bias in surveys [40,41] one of the arguments for each subtheme was framed positively while the other was framed negatively. Examples for the development of questions are given in Table 1. Agreement to the statements was tested using 7-point Likert-scales ranging from -3 =“I strongly disagree” to +3 =“I strongly agree”.

thumbnail
Table 1. Examples of illustrative quotes reported by Ventura et al. [31] and the related, balanced quantitative arguments developed for the current study for each of six main themes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t001

Before participants were presented the different arguments they received the following information as background on the topic:

“On dairy farms cows are kept for the purpose of producing milk for human consumption. In order to produce milk, a cow needs to give birth to a calf approximately once a year. Once born, there are two main ways calves can be handled:

  1. The calf is separated from the cow within the first few hours of birth and moved to a separate barn so that there is no contact between cow and calf (“early separation”).
    OR
  2. The calf stays with the cow for some days or weeks and is then moved to a separate barn and there is no contact between cow and calf from then on (“later separation”).

The two options regarding the timing of when the calf was removed from its mother, early or later separation, were randomly presented to control for order effects. Immediately following the provision of the background information participants were asked to give their opinion on when calves should be separated from the cows using a 7-point scale ranging from -3 = “They definitely should be separated later” to 0 = “I am not sure” to +3 = “They definitely should be separated early”. The question was then repeated after participants were presented with the 22 arguments outlined in Table 1 to test for shifts in opinion caused by the information provided through the arguments.

Additional survey questions asked included peoples’ value orientation on a Short Schwartz Value Scale (SSVS) [42], belief in animal mind following Hills [43], pet ownership/pet relations using the pet relation scale by Kafer et al. [44], familiarity with agriculture, dietary convictions and demographics such as gender, age, having children and breastfeeding own children to further describe the different citizen segments.

To control if participants read the questions we included two types of quality check. First, a quality check question was randomly positioned within the arguments that simply asked participants to tick “I agree” to control if they really read the questions. Second, participants that answered the survey in less than 1/3 of the average response time were excluded, as we assumed that answering the questionnaire in this little time precluded a thorough reading of all questions.

Participant recruitment and data collection

Participants in both countries were recruited online in June and July 2015 through an external provider. For the US sample, Amazon’s Mechanical Turk platform was used to invite participants to take part in the study. In Germany, participants were recruited via Clickworker, a platform similar to Mechanical Turk, but focused exclusively on German residents. Crowdsourcing platforms such as Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and Clickworker provide access to a large and diverse subject pool at comparably low costs compared to the more traditional face to face or telephone interviews which helps to fasten empirical experiments and theory validation in research [45]. Although participants recruited through crowdsourcing platforms differ from the general public due to possible self-selection biases, they have been shown to be more diverse than student samples and importantly produce reliable outcomes comparable to offline samples [45,46]. Before taking part in the survey, participants were briefly informed about the topic of the survey and were required to give consent to take part. All participants were free to exit the survey at any time. The reward given to those respondents that completed the survey was US$1 or 1€ in the US and German sample, respectively.

A total of 517 participants from the US and 601 from Germany took part in the study. After removing 147 participants that failed the quality check question, completed the survey in less than 1/3 of the average response time or did not answer all questions in the main part of the study, the responses from 967 participants (476 US and 491 German participants) remained for further analyses.

Data analyses

The data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics 23. We initially undertook an exploratory factor analysis (see [47]) in which we included all of the arguments presented by the participants on cow-calf separation. This process allowed us to identify relations between single items and to increase reliability of the measurement by creating scales that could be used for further analyses. Variables showing only low (<0.4) factor loadings or high (≥0.4) factor loadings on more than one factor were excluded from analysis. Three reliable factors were identified, which included 16 of the 22 arguments (Kayser Meyer Olkin criterion = 0.88; Bartlett’s test of sphericity: p = 0.000; total variance explained = 54.74%). The first factor (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.77) primarily included arguments that focused on the emotional life of the cow and calf. In addition to those, an argument referring to changing housing systems to benefit the animals was also included due to high factor loading. Factor 2 (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.74) reflected arguments that were in favor of early separation; whereby, health, emotion and management arguments were mixed together. Factor 3 (Cronbachs Alpha = 0.68) related to arguments in favor of later separation that were focused on health, production and management.

Using these three factors as well as the question concerning the right time of separation (after the provision of arguments), we undertook a cluster analyses to identify different segments within the sample populations that were reasonably homogenous within each segment but as heterogeneous as possible between the segments [48]. This procedure was divided into three parts. First, outliers in the sample were identified using Single Linkage clustering method. Nine participants were identified as outliers and excluded from further analyses. Second, the optimal number of clusters and the cluster means were determined using the Ward Method [49]. Thirdly, a partitioning K-Means cluster analysis (suitable for larger sample sizes (> 500 cases [50])) was done to form the clusters. This method forms random clusters based on starting points identified with the Ward Method at the outset and reassigns participants with the aim of minimizing within cluster variation to create homogeneous groups. This process resulted in the identification of a three-cluster solution for the present study. Using discriminate analysis 97.8% of cases could be classified correctly, indicating a good cluster solution.

To further characterize the clusters and test for differences between clusters, one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with post-hoc tests, independent T-tests, paired-samples T-tests with correlations and cross tabulation with chi-square and z-tests were used. To create reliable scales for cluster comparison, two further factor analyses were conducted: firstly, participant’s belief in animal mind with two reliable factors: a) Belief in animal mind negative and b) Belief in animal mind positive; Kayser Meyer Olkin criterion = 0.73; Bartlett’s test of sphericity: p = 0.000; total variance explained = 54.1%). Secondly, the pet relation scale was calculated (one reliable factor; Kayser Meyer Olkin = 0.89; Bartlett’s test of sphericity: p = 0.000; total variance explained = 61.0%). To assess the more general values of respondents, two constructs from the SSVS (‘conservation’ and ‘self-transcendence’) were calculated as proposed by Lindeman and Verkasalo [42]. In this case, conservation measures “whether people resist change and emphasize self-restriction and order or whether they are ready for new experiences and emphasize independent action and thought” and self-transcendence measures peoples’ motivation “to transcend selfish concerns and promote the welfare of others or whether they are more motivated to enhance their own personal interests even at the expense of others.” [42].

Results

Sample description

Both the US and German samples were convenience samples. No quotas for demographic requirements were set during the survey. The distribution of gender, age and region compared to census data for both populations is shown in Table 2. With regard to age, both samples were young relative to the census data. The regional distribution in the US sample was similar to the population data but for Germany participants from the western part were overrepresented.

thumbnail
Table 2. Distribution of demographics in the US (n = 476) and German (n = 491) online sample compared to census data from both countries.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t002

To test participants’ familiarity with dairy farming they were asked to state their frequency of visits to commercial dairy farms; 55% and 56% in the US and German sample stated that they have never been on a commercial dairy farm, 21.6% and 21.8% reported having visited a dairy farm once, 21.8% and 20.8% a few times and only 1.5% and 1.4% reported having regularly visited a commercial dairy farm, respectively.

Favoring early or later separation

After reading the information on cow-calf separation, participants were asked to indicate their opinion on whether separation should occur early or later. The question was repeated a second time after participants had the opportunity to read the 22 arguments presented on this topic (see Table 1). For the US sample more than half of respondents were initially in favor of later separation (55.1%), 17.6% were unsure and 27.3% favored early separation. In contrast, 68.7% of Germans favored later separation, 11.2% were unsure and 20.2% favored early separation before the presentation of arguments (Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Responses by American (US; n = 476) and German (German; n = 491) participants’ to whether cow-calf separation should take place early or later, BEFORE and AFTER the provision of arguments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t003

Regardless of country, we noted an increase in the total number of unsure responses and a decline in the responses at the extreme ends of the scales for later separation, as well as a decline for all categories favoring early separation, after participants were provided the 22 different arguments for and against early and late separation.

Attitudes towards cow-calf separation—Description of the clusters

The cluster analysis, based on the three factors that emerged once the participants responded to the different arguments for early and later separation and on the question of when cows and calves should be separated, identified three unique groups of participants. The three clusters were named “Late”, “Unsure”, and “Early”, referring to the preferred time of separation. Participants from the US and German samples were found in each of the three clusters although proportions varied to some degrees. For example, the proportion of US residents was higher in the “Early” cluster compared to the German but this was reversed for the “Late” cluster; with the “Unsure” cluster having approximately equal proportions of each country.

Cluster “Late”–Favor later separation.

This cluster represented 370 participants (38.7% of the entire sample) making it intermediate in size relative to the other two clusters. Respondents in this cluster were strongly in favor of later separation of cow and calf (Table 4). After the provision of arguments there was an increase in the support for later separation, especially at the middle of the scale supporting later separation, while answers of respondents showing extreme support before remained for the most part unaffected after the provision of the arguments.

thumbnail
Table 4. Percentages and means (± S.D.) falling into the 7-point Likert scale on whether cow-calf separation should take place early or later BEFORE and AFTER the provision of arguments.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t004

We also considered how many participants changed their responses after the provision of different arguments. In the”Late” cluster, 71.1% of the participants did not change their ‘before’ response, 14.6% of participants strengthened their existing opinion to support later separation and 10.5% chose to weaken their support for late separation practice.

Table 5 displays the responses to the variables used to build the clusters. Cluster “Late” participants showed high agreement with arguments that pertained to the animals’ emotions in Factor 1 such as “The cow has an emotional attachment to her calf.” Respondents within this cluster rejected the argument that the attachment between cow and calf differs from that of a human mother and child. Compared to the other clusters, participants in this cluster disagreed with arguments in favor of potential health and emotional advantages associated with early separation (Factor 2). Furthermore, participants believed that it would not be hard to change practices on farms that currently separate early. This cluster of participants agreed with arguments that promoted potential health and production benefits of later separation (Factor 3) and assumed that early separation was done to reduce labor for the farmer and did not consider the welfare of the cow and the calf.

thumbnail
Table 5. Cluster “Late”, “Unsure” and “Early” means (±S.D.) for the cluster-building variables “When do you think calves should be separated from the cow?” and factor 1, 2 and 3 arising from exploratory factor analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t005

Table 6 shows arguments for early and later separation that could not be incorporated into the factors due to either low factor scores (<0.4) or due to high scores on more than one factor (>0.4) (see [47]). Table 7 compares factors for belief in animal mind, the pet relation scale, and the two constructs arising from the SSVS. Cluster “Late” differed from the other clusters in most of the statements. These participants, for example, related early separation with distress for the cow and calf and believed that the cow and calf enjoyed mutual companionship. They also disagreed with the argument that by nursing a calf, the cow will not release her milk to the farmer and were skeptical regarding the calves’ ability to develop normal social behavior when separated early from their mothers. Cluster “Late” also had the highest belief in the cow’s mind, i.e. they agreed that cows are conscious and aware of what is happening to them and that they are capable of experiencing feelings and emotions. In contrast, this cluster was more indecisive in their responses to whether cows were more likely to respond to instinctive urges, have limited ability to see cause and effect of an action and experience emotions less intensely than humans.

thumbnail
Table 6. Cluster “Late”, “Unsure” and “Early” means (±S.D.) for cluster-describing variables about weaning that were not related to the factors.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t006

thumbnail
Table 7. Cluster “Late“, “Unsure”and “Early” means (±S.D.) for cluster describing variables in relation to pets and Schwartz values.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t007

In cluster “Late”, the proportion of participants having children was higher compared to the other two clusters (“Late” = 51.6%, “Unsure” = 32.6%, “Early” = 27.5%, p≤0.05), but no differences were observed in the proportion reporting breastfeeding (“Late” = 77.0%, “Unsure” = 73.3%, “Early” = 76.6%, p˃0.05). With regard to pet ownership, cluster “Late” showed the highest proportion, differing from clusters”Unsure” and “Early” (”Late” = 59.7%, “Unsure” = 49.4%, “Early” = 46.2%, p≤0.05). Pet owner participants in cluster “Late” showed the highest score on the pet relation scale indicating a close relationship to their pet; their pet gave them a reason to get up in the morning, was the best friend they have and was constantly at their side. In terms of general values, cluster “Late” differed in the construct of self-transcendence from cluster “Early”, showing the highest score of all groups (see Table 6). Clusters did not differ in frequency of having visited a commercial dairy farm (p˃0.05).

In Table 8, demographic characteristics of the clusters are shown. Cluster “Late” also had a higher proportion of German participants and the highest share of female participants. Cluster “Late” contained fewer participants under the age of 29 years compared to the other two clusters and also had a higher share of older participants. The proportion of vegetarians was higher in this cluster and the number of participants within this cluster that have a family member working in agriculture was lower. The share of participants having regular contact with agriculture through others such as friends or neighbors was higher. No differences between the clusters were found with regard to whether participants lived in rural or urban areas.

thumbnail
Table 8. Percentage of participants falling into clusters “Late”, “Unsure” and “Early”, in relation to participant demographics.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174013.t008

Cluster “Unsure”–Unsure of whether to favor later separation.

The “Unsure” cluster was the largest of the three clusters and included just under half of all of the participants (43.6%). This cluster also favored later separation but showed less certainty. After seeing the arguments, 42.1% of participants maintained their initial opinion on the separation question. Of the 57.9% who changed their opinion, 16.8% strengthening their support in favor of later separation and 25.9% either changed their initial support in favor of early or late separation to an unsure response. The means in this cluster did not change from before to after having seen the arguments.

The participants in the “Unsure” cluster showed lower agreement to arguments highlighting the emotions of cow and calf (Factor 1) compared to the”Late” cluster, but still agreed that the cow has an emotional attachment to her calf (see Table 5). This cluster rejected, although to a lower extent than cluster”Late”, that cows and calves experience few emotions. However, they were also unsure about whether the attachment between cow and calf is different from that between a human mother and child and they agreed that housing systems on dairy farms can be changed to maximize benefits for the animals, but to a lesser extent compared to cluster “Late”.

The “Unsure” cluster differed from the two other clusters regarding their responses to the arguments presented in favor of potential health and emotional advantages associated with early separation (Factor 2). Cluster “Unsure” respondents were uncertain whether to agree or disagree with the health and emotional arguments presented in this factor. Only in the arguments about the cow being able to injure the calf or the calves suckling damaging the cows’ teats did these participants show disagreement. With regard to health, production and management arguments favoring later separation (Factor 3), “Unsure” participants showed some support for each of these arguments (differing from the “Late” cluster in all cases). The “Unsure” cluster was intermediate in terms of support of the argument that a calf has better access to milk if it can nurse freely from the cow compared to the “Late” and “Early” cluster.

Participants within the “Unsure” cluster agreed that early separation causes distress for the animals and that the cow protects the calf from threats and injuries. This cluster also agreed with the arguments that were in accordance with the nature of the cow and calf to enjoy companionship for a while. They were, however, unsure if the cow releases her milk to the farmer if kept with her calf and tended to be unsure if calves can develop normal social behavior. They also tended to agree that farmers treat their cows with respect because they are their livelihood. Cluster “Unsure” could also be characterized as having some belief in animal mind, differing in this respect from the “Late” and to some extent from the “Early” cluster. Participants in this cluster agreed that cows are conscious and aware of what is happening to them and that they are able to experience emotions. They were however unsure if cows are able to solve problems and make decisions. They agreed that cows respond more mechanically to instinctive urges and that they have limited ability to see cause and effect. They also tended to agree that cows experience emotions less intensely than humans. Participants in this cluster scored lower on the pet relation scale than the “Late” cluster, and were similar to the “Early” cluster in this respect. Regarding the SSVS, the “Unsure” cluster differed only from the “Early” cluster in the construct of self-transcendence.

In the “Unsure” cluster, the US and German participants were equally distributed. There were slightly more men than women in this cluster. A higher proportion of participants were less than 29 years of age in this cluster compared to the”Late” cluster, and there were fewer vegetarians. There were also fewer participants in regular contact with agriculture through others such as friends or neighbors compared to the “Late” cluster. The percentage of people having children or pets was lower in this cluster compared to the “Late” cluster but did not differ from the “Early” cluster.

Cluster “Early”–Favor early separation.

This cluster was the smallest with participants’ representing approximately 18% of the entire sample. Participants in this cluster favored early separation of the cow and calf in dairy farming. Within this cluster there were fewer unsure answers and support for early separation increased after reading the different arguments (see Table 4). In this cluster, 55% of participants did not change their response to the separation question after the arguments were presented, whereas 17.5% weakened and 9.4% strengthened their existing opinion to support early separation. Within this cluster 9.9% of participants favored later separation before the arguments and changed their opinion to favor late separation afterwards. Regarding the arguments in Factor 1, this cluster only differed from the”Late” cluster. With regard to Factor 2, this cluster showed the highest agreement compared to the other two clusters. Participants showed higher agreements with arguments favoring early separation, particularly with respect to the argument that later separation is hard on the mother. The participants in this cluster appeared unsure regarding arguments about calf injuries and teat damage presented in favor of later separation, focusing rather on the farmer being able to ensure adequate colostrum intake with early separation. Participants in this cluster also tended to agree that it is hard for farms to change practices associated with early separation.

Cluster “Early” had the lowest means in Factor 3 compared to the other two clusters. They were also unsure if a cow produces more milk when together with her calf and if frequent suckling helps prevent cow diseases. They were also unsure if early separation improves calf health and whether it reduces labor. They did agree, albeit to a lesser extent, that calves have better access to milk if kept together with the cow.

Cluster “Early” participants were unsure if early separation causes distress for the animals but they did agree (albeit to a lesser extent than the other two clusters) that the cow helps to protect the calf from threats and injury. Participants within this cluster tended to agree that the cow would not release her milk to the farmer if kept together with her calf. This cluster agreed that calves can develop normal social behavior even when separated and that farmers treat their cows with respect. Although this cluster showed the lowest belief in animal mind, they did think that cows were able to experience emotions but did so less intensely than humans. This cluster showed the same pet relation score as cluster “Unsure”. In terms of Schwartz-Values, cluster “Early” showed the lowest score on self-transcendence.

Cluster “Early” contained a higher share of US participants. The majority of participants represented in this cluster were men and half of the participants were under the age of 29. The proportion of vegetarians in this cluster was the lowest of all three clusters. Similar to the “Unsure” cluster approximately 23% of the participants in this cluster had family members working in agriculture. The number of participants having children was the lowest in this cluster, and differed from the”Late” cluster. The proportion of participants reporting that they were current pet owners was also less compared to the”Late” cluster.

Discussion

This study used a quantitative approach to provide insights into citizen attitudes towards separation of cow and calf. The online survey was cost and time efficient compared to other data collection methods. Additionally, it helped avoid social desirability biases [56]. Possible disadvantages of this methodology are that the number of indecisive answers is higher in online surveys compared to face-to-face interviews [56]. To mitigate the effects of these challenges, we used 7-point scales instead of 5-point scales, as the potential for using the mid-point category decreases when there are more scale steps [57]. Demographics such as sex and region were close to census data but both the U.S. and German samples were younger compared to the original populations. Also, given that we used a convenience sample, our results should not be considered representative on a national scale. However, our work brings to light the contentiousness of cow-calf separation and can be used as basis for future quantitative studies that make use of representative samples.

Demographic differences between the clusters

We found few differences in demographics between clusters. Ellis et al. [17] found differences in dairy cattle welfare perceptions between age groups, but we observed no clear pattern in the current study, although cluster “Late” showed the lowest share of young participants (between 18 and 29 years). The higher percentage of females in the “Late” cluster is in line with other studies reporting that females desire higher care for animals [5] and show higher welfare sensibility [58]. Although some work has reported that having children was associated with lower animal welfare concerns [3,36], we found no such association in our study. That more participants in cluster “Late” were parents and were female, in comparison with either cluster “Unsure” or “Early”, may be because the discussion reminded parents, particularly mothers, of their relationship with their children. We found no differences in place of residence (e.g. urban verses rural); in contrast, other studies have sometimes found that rural participants were more satisfied with farming practices [16] and trust farmers more [1]. Participants having a family member working in agriculture were more likely to support early separation in our study, perhaps because such individuals have a more positive image of farmers than those further removed from agriculture [16,59]. The increased proportion of US residents in the “Early” cluster and the increased proportion of German participants in the “Late” cluster may be a consequence of some differences in values which may be influenced by cultural norms within societies [16]. Our noted differences of participant proportions within the “Early” and “Late” clusters from the two countries suggest that the distribution of values in both societies may differ [15]. Unfortunately, our sample is not representative on either of the national scales but we encourage more work of this nature.

Favoring early versus later separation

Irrespective of the arguments presented for and against separation, the common practice of separating cow and calf was not widely supported by either US or German participants. Boogaard et al. [16] reported that participants did not want calves to grow up without a dam. Ventura et al. [31] found more support for later separation in the general public, but that farmers and veterinarians working with cattle farmers supported early separation.

The current study was designed to present balanced arguments in favor of both early and later separation. Thus, by design, we did not expect that providing this information would shift the views of our participants. Some authors have argued that education of the public about farming practices will head off criticisms [12], but our results show that different clusters attend to different types of information and arguments. In all clusters, a relatively large share of participants kept to their initial answer. This effect, also known as confirmation bias, begins once a person has taken a position towards a topic that normally also includes justifying this position and, therefore, will preferentially favor arguments in support of their initial position [60,61]. Another explanation could be peoples’ moral intuition. Moral intuition describes a judgment without having gone through a conscious decision process but rather relies on a persons’ feeling of appropriateness [62]. For the “Late” cluster we saw a strengthening of support for later separation overall while nearly three quarters of participants kept their initial answer. For the “Unsure” cluster the sample means remained the same before and after the provision of arguments, but about one quarter of participants adopted an unsure response after having read the arguments. The notable increase of unsure responses in this cluster suggests that participants were attending to all of the arguments, and were thus confused by the intentionally balanced arguments we presented. Participants in the “Early” cluster were most engaged by the arguments in favor of early separation, and their responses stayed the same or strengthened in this regard. In summary, participants’ reaction to the provision of arguments differed between the clusters despite the provision of the same balanced arguments for and against the practice. This result suggests that efforts to educate the non-farming public will not consistently lead to the desired effects especially when the conflict is not mainly a consequence of ignorance but reflects differences in values [12]. This result is in line with a study of Ventura et al. [13], who surveyed participants before and after a farm visit. The authors found that citizens’ knowledge improved after the visit, but there was no increase in support for the farming practices (in fact, support declined).

The current study was not designed to test if opinions would shift if arguments were presented only in favor of the practice. We did not weigh the arguments on the basis of the evidence in their favor, but rather presented the most common arguments for and against cow-calf separation. Some participants may have questioned the validity of the arguments, perhaps contributing to the observed confirmation bias. Studies systematically analyzing the effects of non-balanced, evidence-based information provision would be of value, but we caution readers that in the case of cow-calf separation the evidence in favor of the various options is often weak and contradictory making an unbiased assessment of the ‘facts’ difficult.

Our results provide additional evidence in support of the assumption that people with similar animal welfare interests have similar characteristics regardless of country of origin [2]. Specifically, we found homogeneous segments in our cluster solution that included participants from both countries. Our results support the use of segmentation to better characterize people’s views on contentious topics [2,32].

Evaluation of different arguments for later and early separation

Participants in the “Late” cluster placed great emphasis on the natural rearing process and on the emotional consequences of separation. In contrast, participants in the “Early” cluster, emphasized the disadvantages associated with later separation (e.g. distress associated with severing this bond). This latter concern is supported by the scientific literature [24] but other research has shown advantages of later separation. For example, Wagner et al. [63] found that mother-reared calves exposed to an isolation test showed lower stress levels than artificially-fed calves and were able to cope more actively with challenging situations. Another study by Wagner et al. [64], showed that calves separated later from their mothers were more interactive. Other work has found that calves reared as cow-calf pairs show better learning abilities and less food neophobia compared to individually housed calves [65].

Participants in the “Unsure” cluster seemed to be uncertain in how to evaluate the arguments. Thus, for this largest segment in our study, the information provided was either not sufficient or was (by design) too conflicting. In addition, it is possible that the two options we provided in the survey (early and later separation) did not reflect their range of preferred opinions (e.g. some may have wanted cow and calf never be separated).

Belief in animal mind and pet relation

Our results regarding participants’ belief in animal mind are in line with those of Knight and Barnett [66] who reported that participants who perceived that the mental and emotional capabilities of animals were high, were more likely to oppose animal use. Similarly, Bastian et al. [67] found that attributing mind to an animal reduces the willingness to eat meat.

The results of our study also support the hypothesis that experiences with pets increase belief in animals’ capacities. Ellis et al. [17] found that respondents often referred to their pet when making inferences about animal emotions, and other studies found that pet owners are more concerned about animal welfare [3] and more likely to oppose animal research than non-pet owners [66]. People without a pet [30], or with less familiarity with a pet [68], perceive animals as less capable of experiencing emotions. Boogaard et al. [59] reported that pet owners perceive the quality of life of farm animals to be less positive and they also have a less positive image of farmers. Collectively, this evidence suggests that individuals with connections to companion animals are more likely to infer emotions in animals and disagree with early separation of cow and calf.

Value orientation

Te Velde et al. [32] emphasized the importance of values in animal welfare discussions, as part of peoples’ frame of reference used to construct perceptions on various topics. Boogaard et al. [16] concluded that the acceptance of modern dairy farming is related to people’s fundamental value orientation and that socially minded respondents were more opposed to modern dairy farming practices. The scientific advisory board for agricultural policies in Germany in their report entitled “Ways towards socially accepted livestock farming”, suggested that agricultural policies must meet these values and in doing so improve the welfare for animals [69]. In the case of cow-calf separation, people favoring late separation show higher self-transcendence and therefore favoring this practice can be connected to higher empathy in people.

Conclusions and practical implications

The majority of participants ascribed mental and emotional capabilities to cows and calves, indicating that they perceived these animals to have the capacity to experience pain and distress. Although the common practice of separating cow and calf in dairy farming after birth was not supported by the majority of participants from either the US and Germany we did note a slightly higher proportion of Germans in the cluster that favored keeping cows and calves together with the reserve being true for the cluster that favored separating cow and calf at birth. Most interestingly, the presentation of balanced arguments did not lead to overall shifts in opinions, but we did find that different clusters of participants attended to different arguments likely reflecting value differences. These results contribute to a growing body of literature indicating that educational efforts by the agricultural industries to bring public views in line with industry practices will not be successful.

Acknowledgments

We thank the University of British Columbia’s (UBC) Animal Welfare Program members Kathrin Schirmann for the translation of the German questionnaire back into the English language and Beth Ventura for helping with the questionnaire used for this study. Gesa Busch and Achim Spiller are grateful to the “Ministry of Science and Arts” in Lower Saxony, Germany, for funding the PhD Program “Animal Welfare in Intensive Livestock Production Systems” which led to this work being completed. The work at UBC was made possible by donations made to the Animal Welfare Program.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: GB DMW AS MAGvK.
  2. Data curation: GB.
  3. Formal analysis: GB.
  4. Funding acquisition: AS DMW MAGvK.
  5. Investigation: GB.
  6. Methodology: GB DMW AS MAGvK.
  7. Project administration: GB.
  8. Resources: DMW AS MAGvK.
  9. Supervision: MAGvK.
  10. Visualization: GB DMW AS MAGvK.
  11. Writing – original draft: GB DMW MAGvK.
  12. Writing – review & editing: GB DMW AS MAGvK.

References

  1. 1. Robbins JA, Franks B, Weary DM, von Keyserlingk MAG. Awareness of ag-gag laws erodes trust in farmers and increases support for animal welfare regulations. Food Policy. 2016; 61: 121–125.
  2. 2. Vanhonacker F, Verbeke W. Public and consumer policies for higher welfare food products: Challenges and opportunities. J Agric Environ Ethics. 2014; 27: 153–171.
  3. 3. Kendall HA, Lobao LM, Sharp JS. Public Concern with Animal Well-Being: Place, Social Structural, Location, and Individual Experience. Rural Sociol. 2006; 71: 399–428.
  4. 4. Krystallis A, Dutra de Barcellos M, Kügler JO, Verbeke W, Grunert KG. Attitudes of European citizens towards pig production systems. Livest Sci. 2009; 126: 46–56.
  5. 5. Prickett RW, Norwood FB, Lusk JL. Consumer preferences for farm animal welfare: results from a telephone survey of US households. Anim Welf. 2010; 19: 335–347.
  6. 6. Tonsor GT, Olynk N, Wolf C. Consumer preferences for animal welfare attributes: the case of gestation crates. J Agr Appl Econ. 2009; 41: 713–730.
  7. 7. Savory CJ. Laying hen welfare standards: a classic case of ‘power to the people’. Anim Welf. 2004; 13: 153–158.
  8. 8. Robbins JA, Weary DM, Schuppli CA, von Keyserlingk MAG. Stakeholder views on treating pain due to dehorning dairy calves. Anim Welf. 2015; 24: 399–406.
  9. 9. Vanhonacker F, Verbeke W, Tuyttens FAM. Belgian consumers’ attitudes towards surgical castration and immunocastration of piglets. Anim Welf. 2009; 18: 371–380.
  10. 10. Schuppli CA, von Keyserlingk MAG, Weary D. Access to pasture for dairy cows: response from an on-line engagement. J Anim Sci. 2014; 92: 5185–5192. pmid:25261215
  11. 11. Driessen C. Farmers engaged in deliberative practices; An ethnographic exploration of the mosaic of concersn in livestock agriculture. J Agric Environ Ethics. 2012; 25: 163–179.
  12. 12. Hansen J, Holm L, Frewer L, Robinson P, Sandøe P. Beyond the knowledge deficit: recent research into lay and experts attitudes to food risks. Appetite. 2003; 41: 111–121. pmid:14550309
  13. 13. Ventura BA, von Keyserlingk MAG, Wittman H, Weary DM. What difference does a visit make? Changes in animal welfare perceptions after interested citizens tour a dairy farm. PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0154733. pmid:27243965
  14. 14. Veissier I, Butterworth A, Bock B, Roe E. European approaches to ensure good animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008; 113: 279–297.
  15. 15. von Keyserlingk MAG, Hötzel MJ. The ticking clock: addressing farm animal welfare in emerging countries. J Agric Environ Ethics. 2015; 28: 179–195.
  16. 16. Boogaard BK, Bock BB, Osting SJ, Wiskerke JSC, van der Zijpp AJ. Social acceptance of dairy farming: the ambivalence between the two faces of modernity. J Agric Environ Ethics. 2011; 24: 259–282.
  17. 17. Ellis KA, Billington K, McNeil B, McKeegan DEF. Public opinion on UK milk marketing and dairy cow welfare. Anim Welf. 2009; 18: 267–282.
  18. 18. Schröder MJA, McEachern MG. Consumer value conflicts surrounding ethical food purchase decisions: a focus on animal welfare. Int J Consum Stud. 2004; 28: 168–177.
  19. 19. Verbeke WAJ, Viaene J. Ethical challenges for livestock production: meeting consumer concerns about meat safety and animal welfare. J Agric Environ Ethics. 2000; 12: 141–151.
  20. 20. Weinrich R, Kühl S, Zühlsdorf A, Spiller A. Consumer attitudes in Germany towards different dairy housing systems and their implications for the marketing of pasture raised milk. Int Food Agribus Man. 2014; 17: 205–222.
  21. 21. Weary D, Chua B. Effects of early separation on the dairy cow and calf 1. Separation at 6 h, 1 day and 4 days after birth. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2000; 69: 177–188. pmid:10906402
  22. 22. Flower FC, Weary DM. Effects of early separation on the dairy cow and calf: 2. Separation at 1 day and 2 weeks after birth. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2001; 70: 275–284. pmid:11179551
  23. 23. Lidfors LM. Behavioural effects of separating the dairy calf immediately or 4 days post-partum. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1996; 49: 269–283.
  24. 24. Flower FC, Weary DM. The effects of early separation on the dairy cow and calf. Anim Welf. 2003; 12: 339–348.
  25. 25. Wagenaar JPTM, Langhout J. Practical implications of increasing 'natural living' through suckling systems in organic dairy calf rearing. NJAS. 2007; 54: 375–386.
  26. 26. Marcé C, Ezanno P, Seegers H, Pfeiffer DU, Fourichon C. Within-herd contact structure and transmission of Mycobacterium avium subspecies paratuberculosis in a persistently infected dairy cattle herd. Prev Vet Med. 2011; 100: 116–125. pmid:21549436
  27. 27. Daros RR, Costa JHC, von Keyserlingk MAG, Hötzel MJ, Weary D. Separation from the dam causes negative judgement bias in dairy calves. PLoS One. 2014; 9: e98429. pmid:24848635
  28. 28. Krohn CC, Foldager J, Mogensen L. Long-term effect of colostrum feeding methods on behaviour in female dairy calves. Acta Agric Scandinavica. 1999; 49: 57–64.
  29. 29. Krohn CC. Effects of different suckling systems on milk production, udder health, reproduction, calf growth and some behavioural aspects in high producing dairy cows—a review. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2001; 72: 271–280. pmid:11311421
  30. 30. Walker JK, McGrath N, Handel IG, Waran NK, Phillips CJC. Does owning a companion animal influence the belief that animals experience emotions such as grief? Anim Welf. 2014; 23: 71–79.
  31. 31. Ventura B, von Keyserlingk MAG, Schuppli CA, Weary DM. Views on contentious practices in dairy farming: The case of early cow-calf separation. J Dairy Sci. 2013; 96: 6105–6116. pmid:23791487
  32. 32. Te Velde H, Aarts N, van Woerkum C. Dealing with ambivalence: farmers’ and consumers’ perceptions of animal welfare in livestock breeding. J Agric Environ Ethics. 2002; 15: 203–219.
  33. 33. Dutra de Barcellos M, Krystallis A, Stela de Melo Saab M, Kügler JO, Grunert KG. Investigating the gap between citizens’ sustainability attitudes and food purchasing behaviour: empirical evidence from Brazilian pork consumers. Int J Consum Stud. 2011; 35: 391–402.
  34. 34. Nillson T, Foster K, Lusk J.L. Marketing opportunities for certified pork chops. Can J Agr Econ. 2006; 54: 567–583.
  35. 35. Meuwissen MPM, Van Der Lans IA, Huirne RBM. Consumer preferences for pork supply chain attributes. NJAS. 2007; 54: 293–312.
  36. 36. Vanhonacker F, Verbeke W, van Poucke E, Tuyttens FAM. Segmentation based on consumers’ perceived importance and attitude toward farm animal welfare. Int J Sociol Agric Food. 2007; 15: 91–107.
  37. 37. Boogaard BK, Oosting SJ, Bock BB. Defining sustainability as a socio-cultural concept: Citizen panels visiting dairy farms in the Netherlands. Livest Sci. 2008: 117: 24–33.
  38. 38. Weary D, Schuppli CA, von Keyserlingk MAG. Tail docking dairy cattle: Responses from an online engagement. J Anim Sci. 2011; 89: 3831–3837. pmid:21666003
  39. 39. Weary D, Ventura B, von Keyserlingk MAG. Societal views and animal welfare science: understanding why the modified cage may fail and other stories. Animal. 2016; 10: 309–317. pmid:26206166
  40. 40. Cloud J, Vaughan GM. Using balanced scales to control acquiescence. Sociometry. 1970; 33: 193–202.
  41. 41. Ray JJ. Is the acquiescent response style problem not so mythical after all? Some results from a successful balanced F scale. J Pers Assess. 1979; 43: 638–643. pmid:16366993
  42. 42. Lindeman M, Verkasalo M. Measuring values with the Short Schwartz’s Value Survey. J Pers Assess. 2005; 85: 170–178. pmid:16171417
  43. 43. Hills AM. Empathy and belief in the mental experience of animals. Anthrozoos. 1995; 8: 132–142.
  44. 44. Kafer R, Lago D, Wamboldt P, Harrington F. The pet relationship scale: replication of psychometric properties in random samples and association with attitudes toward wild animals. Anthrozoos. 1992; 12: 93–105.
  45. 45. Mason W, Suri S. Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behav Res. 2012; 44: 1–23.
  46. 46. Shank DB. Using crowdsourcing websites for sociological research: the case of Amazon Mechanical Turk. Am Soc. 2016; 47: 47.
  47. 47. Field A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. 3rd ed. London: SAGE publication; 2009.
  48. 48. Ketchen DJ, Shook CL. The application of cluster analysis in strategic management research: an analysis and critique. Strategic Manage J. 1996; 17: 441–458.
  49. 49. Ward JH. Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. J Am Stati Assoc. 1963; 58: 236–244.
  50. 50. Mooi E, Sarstedt A. A concise guide to market research. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer; 2011.
  51. 51. US Census Bureau. Annual estimates of the resident population by single year of age and sex for the United States, 2015. Database: US Census [Internet]. Accessed: http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?src=bkmk.
  52. 52. US Census Bureau. Annual estimates of the resident population for the United States, Regions, States, and Puerto Rico, 2014. Database: US Census [Internet]. Accessed: http://www.census.gov/popest/data/national/totals/2014/index.html.
  53. 53. Statistisches Bundesamt. Bevölkerung: Deutschland, Stichtag, Geschlecht; 2015. Database: Destatis [Internet]. Accessed: https://www-genesis.destatis.de/.
  54. 54. Statistisches Bundesamt. Bevölkerung: Deutschland, Stichtag, Altersjahre, 2015. Database: Destatis [Internet]. Accessed: https://www-genesis.destatis.de/.
  55. 55. Statistisches Bundesamt. Gebietsfläche: Bundesländer, Stichtag; 2015. Database: Destatis [Internet]. Accessed: https://www-genesis.destatis.de/.
  56. 56. Duffy B, Smith K, Terhanian G, Bremer J. Comparing data from online and face-to-face surveys. Int J Market Res. 2005; 47: 615–639.
  57. 57. Matell MS, Jacoby J. Is there an optimal number of alternatives for Likert scale items? Effects of testing time and scale properties. J Appl Psychol. 1972; 56: 506–509.
  58. 58. María GA. Public perception of farm animal welfare in Spain. Livest Sci. 2006; 103: 250–256.
  59. 59. Boogaard BK, Oosting SJ, Bock BB. Elements of societal perception of farm animal welfare: A quantitative study in The Netherlands. Livest Sci. 2006; 104: 13–22.
  60. 60. Nickerson RS. Confirmation Bias: A Ubiquitous Phenomenon in Many Guises. Rev Gen Psychol. 1998; 2: 175–220.
  61. 61. Jonas E, Schulz-Hardt S, Frey D, Thelen N. Confirmation bias in sequential information search after preliminary decisions: An expansion of dissonance theoretical research on selective exposure to information. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001; 80: 557–571. pmid:11316221
  62. 62. Woodward J, Allman J. Moral intuition: Its neural substrates and normative significance. J Physiol. 2007; 179–202.
  63. 63. Wagner K, Barth K, Hillmann E, Palme R, Futschik A, Waiblinger S. Mother rearing of dairy calves: reactions to isolation and to confrontation with an unfamiliar conspecific in a new environment. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2013; 147: 43–54.
  64. 64. Wagner K, Seitner D, Barth K, Palme R, Futschik A, Waiblinger S. Effects of mother versus artificial rearing during the first 12 weeks of life on challenge responses of dairy cows. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015; 164: 1–11.
  65. 65. Meagher RK, Daros RR, Costa JH, von Keyserlingk MAG, Hötzel MJ, Weary DM. Effects of degree and timing of social housing on reversal learning and response to novel objects in dairy calves. PLoS One. 2015; 10: e0132828. pmid:26274583
  66. 66. Knight S, Barnett L. Justifying attitudes towards animal use: a qualitative study of people’s views and beliefs. Anthrozoos. 2008; 21: 31–42.
  67. 67. Bastian B, Loughnan S, Haslam M., Radke HRM. Don’t mind meat? The denial of mind to animals used for human consumption. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2011; 38: 247–256. pmid:21980158
  68. 68. Morris P, Knight S, Lesley S. Belief in animal mind: does familiarity with animals influence beliefs about animal emotions? Soc Anim. 2012; 20: 211–224.
  69. 69. WBA (Wissenschaftlicher Beirat für Agrarpolitik beim Bundesministerium für Ernährung und Landwirtschaft). Wege zu einer gesellschaftlich akzeptierten Nutztierhaltung. Berichte über Landwirtschaft. 2015; Sonderheft 221. http://www.bmel.de/SharedDocs/Downloads/Ministerium/Beiraete/Agrarpolitik/GutachtenNutztierhaltung-Kurzfassung.pdf?__blob=publicationFile