skip to main content
10.1145/3565995.3566035acmotherconferencesArticle/Chapter ViewAbstractPublication PagesaciConference Proceedingsconference-collections
research-article

Watching Animal-Computer Interaction: Effects on Perceptions of Animal Intellect

Published:29 March 2023Publication History

ABSTRACT

Watching animals use digital technology is known to affect our attitudes towards them, but there has been little empirical study of this topic. There is a need for greater understanding of how technology can shape people's perceptions of other species, since human attitudes are a significant factor in animal welfare. We studied the effects of a digital installation, created as enrichment for zoo-housed orangutans. It was hypothesised that seeing the installation in use would strengthen zoo visitors’ perceptions of orangutans’ intellect and strengthen support for their conservation. Effects were investigated through visitor interviews (n=39) and surveys (n=101), comparing responses of people who saw the installation with those who did not. Seeing primates use the digital installation was found to be associated with stronger attribution of cognitive abilities. Watching animals comprehend game rules, and seeing their human-like patterns of interaction seemed to contribute to this effect. However, no overall impact was found on attitudes to orangutan conservation. This research provides insights into the potential effects of animal-computer interaction on the attitudes of human observers, and suggests avenues for technology design to strengthen people's understanding of animal minds.

References

  1. Marianna Adams, John H. Falk, and Lynn D. Dierking. 2003. Things Change. In Researching Visual Arts Education in Museums and Galleries. Springer, Dordrecht, 15–32. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-0043-7_2Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  2. Leslie M. Adelman, John H. Falk, and Sylvia James. 2000. Impact of National Aquarium in Baltimore on Visitors’ Conservation Attitudes, Behavior, and Knowledge. Curator: The Museum Journal 43, 1: 33–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2151-6952.2000.tb01158.xGoogle ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  3. Brock Bastian, Steve Loughnan, Nick Haslam, and Helena R. M. Radke. 2012. Don't Mind Meat? The Denial of Mind to Animals Used for Human Consumption. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 38, 2: 247–256. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211424291Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  4. Linda M. Blud. 1990. Social interaction and learning among family groups visiting a museum. Museum Management and Curatorship 9, 1: 43–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/09647779009515193Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  5. Juliane Bräuer, Daniel Hanus, Simone Pika, Russell Gray, and Natalie Uomini. 2020. Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition.” Journal of Intelligence 8, 3: 28. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8030028Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  6. Max E. Butterfield, Sarah E. Hill, and Charles G. Lord. 2012. Mangy mutt or furry friend? Anthropomorphism promotes animal welfare. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 48, 4: 957–960. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.010Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  7. Simon Coghlan, Sarah Webber, and Marcus Carter. 2021. Improving ethical attitudes to animals with digital technologies: the case of apes and zoos. Ethics and Information Technology 23, 4: 825–839. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10676-021-09618-7Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  8. Cécile Cornou. 2009. Automation Systems for Farm Animals: Potential Impacts on the Human-Animal Relationship and on Animal Welfare. Anthrozoös 22, 3: 213–220. https://doi.org/10.2752/175303709X457568Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  9. Gareth Davey. 2006. An hourly variation in zoo visitor interest: Measurement and significance for animal welfare research. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 9, 3: 249–256. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0903_7Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  10. Amber J. de Vere, Stan A. Kuczaj. 2016. Where are we in the study of animal emotions?. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 7, 5:364-362. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1399Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  11. Frans De Waal. 2016. Are We Smart Enough to Know how Smart Animals Are? WW Norton & Company, New York, NY, USA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  12. Margo DeMello. 2012. Animals and Society. Columbia University Press, New York, NY, USA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  13. Timothy J. Eddy, Gordon G. Gallup, and Daniel J. Povinelli. 1993. Attribution of Cognitive States to Animals: Anthropomorphism in Comparative Perspective. Journal of Social Issues 49, 1: 87–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1993.tb00910.xGoogle ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  14. Crystal L. Egelkamp and Stephen R. Ross. 2019. A review of zoo-based cognitive research using touchscreen interfaces. Zoo Biology 38, 2. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21458Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  15. Nicholas Epley, Adam Waytz, and John T. Cacioppo. 2007. On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychological review 114, 4: 864. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.864Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  16. John H. Falk, Joseph Heimlich, and Kerry Bronnenkant. 2008. Using Identity-Related Visit Motivations as a Tool for Understanding Adult Zoo and Aquarium Visitors’ Meaning-Making. Curator: The Museum Journal 51, 1: 55–79. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2151-6952.2008.tb00294.xGoogle ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  17. John H. Falk, Theano Moussouri, and Douglas Coulson. 1998. The Effect of Visitors’ Agendas on Museum Learning. Curator: The Museum Journal 41, 2: 107–120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2151-6952.1998.tb00822.xGoogle ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  18. John H Falk, Eric M Reinhard, Cynthia L Vernon, Kerry Bronnenkant, and Nora L Deans. 2007. Why Zoos & Aquariums Matter: Assessing the Impact of a Visit to a Zoo or Aquarium. Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Retrieved from https://www.aza.org/uploadedFiles/Education/why_zoos_matter.pdfGoogle ScholarGoogle Scholar
  19. David Fraser. 2008. Understanding Animal Welfare: The Science in its Cultural Context. John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, USA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  20. John Fraser, Maria Maust-Mohl, Rachel Morrison, Diana Reiss, Sarah Knight, Nezam Ardalan, and Martin Weiss. 2013. A Proposed Transdisciplinary Framework to Align Research on Animal Minds. New Knowledge Organisation Ltd, New York.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  21. Fiona French, Clara Mancini and Helen Sharp. 2017. High tech cognitive and acoustic enrichment for captive elephants. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 300, 2018:173–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.09.009Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  22. Megan S. Geerdts, Gretchen A. Van de Walle, and Vanessa LoBue. 2015. Parent–Child Conversations About Animals in Informal Learning Environments. Visitor Studies 18, 1: 39–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/10645578.2015.1016366Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  23. Heather M. Gray, Kurt Gray, and Daniel M. Wegner. 2007. Dimensions of Mind Perception. Science 315, 5812: 619–619. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1134475Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  24. Greg Guest, Kathleen M. MacQueen, and Emily E. Namey. 2011. Applied thematic analysis. SAGE Publications, Thousand Oaks, CA, USA.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  25. Susan J. Hazel, Lisel O'Dwyer, and Terry Ryan. 2015. “Chickens Are a Lot Smarter than I Originally Thought”: Changes in Student Attitudes to Chickens Following a Chicken Training Class. Animals 5, 3: 821–837. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani5030386Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  26. William S. Helton and Nicole D. Helton. 2005. Changing Animal and Environmental Attitudes with Evidence of Animal Minds. Applied Environmental Education & Communication 4, 4: 317–323. https://doi.org/10.1080/15330150500302114Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  27. Harold A. Herzog Jr., Nancy S. Betchart, and Robert B. Pittman. 1991. Gender, Sex Role Orientation, and Attitudes toward Animals. Anthrozoös 4, 3: 184–191. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279391787057170Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  28. Matthew J. Higgs, Sasha Bipin, and Helen J. Cassaday. 2020. Man's best friends: attitudes towards the use of different kinds of animal depend on belief in different species’ mental capacities and purpose of use. Royal Society Open Science 7, 2: 191162. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.191162Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  29. Adelma M. Hills. 1995. Empathy and Belief in the Mental Experience of Animals. Anthrozoös 8, 3: 132–142. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279395787156347Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  30. Nathalie Hostiou, Jocelyn Fagon, Sophie Chauvat, Amélie Turlot, Florence Kling, Xavier Boivin, and Clément Allain. 2017. Impact of precision livestock farming on work and human- animal interactions on dairy farms. A review. Bioscience, Biotechnology and Biochemistry 21: 1–8.Google ScholarGoogle Scholar
  31. Linda Kalof, Joe Zammit-Lucia, Jessica Bell, and Gina Granter. 2016. Fostering kinship with animals: animal portraiture in humane education. Environmental Education Research 22, 2: 203–228. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2014.999226Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  32. Sarah Knight, Aldert Vrij, Julie Cherryman, and Karl Nunkoosing. 2004. Attitudes towards animal use and belief in animal mind. Anthrozoös 17, 1: 43–62. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279304786991945Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  33. Saara Kupsala, Markus Vinnari, Pekka Jokinen, and Pekka Räsänen. 2016. Public Perceptions of Mental Capacities of Nonhuman Animals. Society & Animals 24, 5: 445–466. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341423Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  34. Shaun Lawson, Ben Kirman, Conor Linehan, Tom Feltwell, and Lisa Hopkins. 2015. Problematising Upstream Technology Through Speculative Design: The Case of Quantified Cats and Dogs. In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’15), 2663–2672. https://doi.org/10.1145/2702123.2702260Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  35. Jerry F. Luebke and Jennifer Matiasek. 2013. An exploratory study of zoo visitors’ exhibit experiences and reactions. Zoo Biology 32, 4: 407–416. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21071Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  36. Susan W. Margulis, Catalina Hoyos, and Meegan Anderson. 2003. Effect of felid activity on zoo visitor interest. Zoo Biology 22, 6: 587–599. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.10115Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  37. Maria Maust-Mohl, John Fraser, and Rachel Morrison. 2012. Wild Minds: What People Think about Animal Thinking. Anthrozoös 25, 2: 133–147. https://doi.org/10.2752/175303712X13316289505224Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  38. Paul Morris, Sarah Knight, and Sarah Lesley. 2012. Belief in Animal Mind: Does Familiarity with Animals Influence Beliefs About Animal Emotions? Society and Animals 20, 3: 211–224. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341234Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  39. Jaak Panksepp. 2011. Toward a cross-species neuroscientific understanding of the affective mind: do animals have emotional feelings? American Journal of Primatology 73, 6: 545–561. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20929Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  40. Stephanie D. Preston and Frans B. M. de Waal. 2002. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, 1: 1-20; https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X02000018Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  41. Jeffrey L. Rasmussen, D. W. Rajecki, and Heather D. Craft. 1993. Humans’ perceptions of animal mentality: Ascriptions of thinking. Journal of Comparative Psychology 107, 3: 283–290. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.107.3.283Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  42. M. Root-Bernstein, L. Douglas, A. Smith, and D. Veríssimo. 2013. Anthropomorphized species as tools for conservation: utility beyond prosocial, intelligent and suffering species. Biodiversity and Conservation 22, 8: 1577–1589. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10531-013-0494-4Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  43. James G. W. Schultz and Steve Joordens. 2014. The effect of visitor motivation on the success of environmental education at the Toronto Zoo. Environmental Education Research 20, 6: 753–775. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504622.2013.843646Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  44. Robert W. Shumaker. 2018. The history and status of cognitive research with great apes in the United States. Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology 68, 2: 105–119.Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  45. Jessica Sickler, John Fraser, Thomas Webler, Diana Reiss, Paul Boyle, Heidi Lyn, Katherine Lemcke, and Sarah Gruber. 2006. Social narratives surrounding dolphins: Q method study. Society & Animals 14, 4: 351–382. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853006778882457Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  46. Jeffrey C. Skibins, Emily Dunstan, and Katie Pahlow. 2017. Exploring the Influence of Charismatic Characteristics on Flagship Outcomes in Zoo Visitors. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 22, 2: 157–171. https://doi.org/10.1080/10871209.2016.1276233Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  47. Jeffrey C. Skibins and Robert B. Powell. 2013. Conservation caring: Measuring the influence of zoo visitors’ connection to wildlife on pro-conservation behaviors. Zoo Biology 32, 5: 528–540. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21086Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  48. Julian Smith. 2011. Apps for apes: Orang-utans want iPads for Christmas. New Scientist 212: 69–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0262-4079(11)63173-4Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  49. Caroline E. Spence, Magda Osman, and Alan G. McElligott. 2017. Theory of Animal Mind: Human Nature or Experimental Artefact? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21, 5: 333–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.003Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  50. Kim-Pong Tam. 2015. Are anthropomorphic persuasive appeals effective? The role of the recipient's motivations. British Journal of Social Psychology 54, 1: 187–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12076Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  51. Kim-Pong Tam, Sau-Lai Lee, and Melody Manchi Chao. 2013. Saving Mr. Nature: Anthropomorphism enhances connectedness to and protectiveness toward nature. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49, 3: 514–521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.02.001Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  52. Anette Therkelsen and Maria Lottrup. 2015. Being together at the zoo: zoo experiences among families with children. Leisure Studies 34, 3: 354–371. https://doi.org/10.1080/02614367.2014.923493Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  53. Esmeralda G. Urquiza-Haas and Kurt Kotrschal. 2015. The mind behind anthropomorphic thinking: attribution of mental states to other species. Animal Behaviour 109: 167–176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.011Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  54. Adam Waytz, Nadav Klein, and Nicholas Epley. 2013. Imagining Other Minds: Anthropomorphism Is Hair-Triggered but Not Hare-Brained. The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Imagination. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195395761.013.0018Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref
  55. Sarah Webber, Marcus Carter, Sally Sherwen, Wally Smith, Zaher Joukhader, and Frank Vetere. 2017. Kinecting with Orangutans: Zoo Visitors’ Empathetic Responses to Animals’ Use of Interactive Technology. 6075–6088. https://doi.org/10.1145/3025453.3025729Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  56. Andrew D. Wilson. 2010. Using a Depth Camera As a Touch Sensor. In ACM International Conference on Interactive Tabletops and Surfaces (ITS ’10), 69–72. https://doi.org/10.1145/1936652.1936665Google ScholarGoogle ScholarDigital LibraryDigital Library
  57. Barbara Woods. 2002. Good zoo/bad zoo: Visitor experiences in captive settings. Anthrozoös 15, 4: 343–360. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279302786992478Google ScholarGoogle ScholarCross RefCross Ref

Index Terms

  1. Watching Animal-Computer Interaction: Effects on Perceptions of Animal Intellect

    Recommendations

    Comments

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Sign in

    Full Access

    • Published in

      cover image ACM Other conferences
      ACI '22: Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on Animal-Computer Interaction
      December 2022
      226 pages
      ISBN:9781450398305
      DOI:10.1145/3565995

      Copyright © 2022 ACM

      Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. Copyrights for components of this work owned by others than ACM must be honored. Abstracting with credit is permitted. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. Request permissions from [email protected]

      Publisher

      Association for Computing Machinery

      New York, NY, United States

      Publication History

      • Published: 29 March 2023

      Permissions

      Request permissions about this article.

      Request Permissions

      Check for updates

      Qualifiers

      • research-article
      • Research
      • Refereed limited
    • Article Metrics

      • Downloads (Last 12 months)65
      • Downloads (Last 6 weeks)5

      Other Metrics

    PDF Format

    View or Download as a PDF file.

    PDF

    eReader

    View online with eReader.

    eReader

    HTML Format

    View this article in HTML Format .

    View HTML Format