Skip to main content
Intended for healthcare professionals
Free access
Research article
First published online October 23, 2018

Incidental News Exposure on Facebook as a Social Experience: The Influence of Recommender and Media Cues on News Selection

Abstract

Incidental exposure to shared news on Facebook is a vital but understudied aspect of how citizens get involved with politics. This experiment investigates the influence of recommender characteristics (tie strength, political knowledge, political similarity) and different media sources (tabloids, legacy, and digital-born outlets) including multiple mediators (e.g., social pressure, outlet credibility) on incidental exposure to political news on Facebook. A 3 × 3 multi-stimulus, between-subject experiment with two additional quasi-factors and 135 different stimuli was conducted using a representative sample (N = 507). Results showed that strong ties and recommenders with high knowledge increase news exposure, but the impact of knowledge is limited to recommenders with similar political opinions. Similar effects occur for different media types, which also have an independent impact on news exposure. Structural equation modeling reveals that media source effects are mediated through media perceptions, whereas recommender effects work via the desire for social monitoring and perceived issue importance.
In today’s high-choice media environment, it has become easier for citizens to only select news that is in line with their political leanings (Stroud, 2010) or to avoid political news altogether (Prior, 2007). In such a setting, incidental news exposure (INE) may become increasingly important to ensure that citizens encounter counterattitudinal information or at least remain connected to the political public sphere. Social network sites (SNS) have been accused of further limiting the chances of INE through algorithmic curation (Thorson & Wells, 2015), but have also been hailed as a promising new source of INE (Lee & Kim, 2017): Even Facebook users who have not intentionally subscribed to a news outlet or whose previous Facebook behavior has not marked them as a suitable target for sponsored posts by news organizations may encounter news articles in their newsfeed via social curation (Thorson & Wells, 2015), that is, because someone in their personal network has shared an article. Although these personal news recommendations on Facebook are particularly important for citizens who do not follow news and politics on their own, we still know very little about the factors that turn incidental contact with news on Facebook into incidental news exposure.
Based on a multiple stimuli experimental study (N = 507), the article addresses this research gap by investigating which personal news recommendations the recipients are more likely to follow. Drawing on the concept of informational utility (Knobloch-Westerwick & Kleinman, 2012), we assume that incidental news contact is more likely to become incidental news exposure if recipients attribute a higher degree of utility to the recommended content. As Facebook provides a social environment for news use, the recipient of personal news recommendation is likely to draw cues for informational utility from the recommender. Following the lead of Turcotte, York, Irving, Scholl, and Pingree (2015) who investigated the impact of personal news recommendations by Facebook friends perceived as opinion leaders on intentional news exposure, we assume that recipients use informational utility cues based on how politically knowledgeable or politically similar the recommender is (politico-social cues). However, we also look in more detail at informational cues based on the closeness of the relationship to the recommender (social cue) by contrasting close and weak ties. In addition, we investigate whether the media outlet as the source of the recommended article also provides relevant cues for its potential informational utility (media cues). And, finally, we uncover the underlying mechanisms that encourage people to read shared news articles on Facebook (e.g., social pressure, social monitoring, issue importance, or outlet curiosity).

Literature Review

Given the importance of an informed citizenry for democracy, the decline in the overall reach of the news media and its detrimental effects on political knowledge and participation (Prior, 2007) have become a growing cause for concern. The now dominant online media have been both feared for reducing intentional news exposure by allowing users to focus more easily on their own, often apolitical, interests (Scheufele & Nisbet, 2002) and praised for increasing the chances of incidental news exposure due to the sheer vastness of political information available online (Tewksbury, Weaver, & Maddex, 2001). A number of studies have focused on the impact of INE via SNS on political knowledge and political participation: Kim, Chen, and Gil de Zúñiga (2013) pointed out that INE was more beneficial for those who are already interested in politics, further widening knowledge and participation gaps. By contrast, Valeriani and Vaccari (2015) reported that INE helps reduce participation gaps. These contradictory results indicate, first, a lack of differentiation between incidental news contact (INC) and INE, and, second, a need to better understand how (social media) users select news following INC, that is, which factors then encourage INE through which underlying mechanisms, particularly on SNS (Karnowski, Kümpel, Leonhard, & Leiner, 2017).

Recommender Cues as a Heuristic for News Exposure on Facebook

Beyond sponsored posts by news companies, INC and INE on Facebook are mostly social experiences: They can occur when someone from a user’s social network recommends an article by sharing it. A promising way to determine whether such an article is then selected is the informational utility approach. Atkin (1973) proposed that information is perceived as more useful—and, thus, more likely to be read—the better it meets the individual’s needs for surveillance, performance, guidance, and reinforcement information that support his or her cognitive, affective, behavioral, or defense adaptation to uncertainty. However, news products’ informational utility can only be assessed after consumption. Hence, heuristics triggered by informational cues play a decisive role in deciding whether to select a specific news item or not (Marewski, Galesic, & Gigerenzer, 2009). While the informational utility concept has successfully been applied to explain, for example, selective exposure to online sources (Knobloch-Westerwick, Carpentier, Blumhoff, & Nickel, 2005), this article explores whether the additional information on recommenders embedded in shared news on Facebook also provides users with cues relating to the informational utility of the recommended articles, and how these relate to an individual’s information needs as proposed by Atkin (1973).
The impact of these informational cues related to the recommender should be comparatively strong as news exposure on Facebook is a far more social experience than in traditional media: Whereas concrete newspaper articles or newscast items might become the topic of interpersonal conversations from time to time, on Facebook, a direct response or interaction following a news recommendation is far more likely. In addition, others can monitor whether the user has followed their recommendation whenever he or she reacts to the article (e.g., by commenting on it).
Still, Facebook friendship networks consist of a wide range of different types of relationships—close friends, co-workers, distant relations, and so on—in other words, both strong ties and weak ties (Granovetter, 1973). For the question of INE via news recommendations, weak ties on Facebook are important because they potentially offer more (politically) diverse and new information compared with strong ties (Bakshy, Rosenn, Marlow, & Adamic, 2012). However, recent large-scale data analysis suggests that strong ties exert more influence than weak ties over the behavior of others in an individual’s personal network (Bond et al., 2012). This would suggest that Facebook users are also more likely to read articles recommended by close friends.
There are two psychological mechanisms that may explain this influence of tie strength on news recommendations. According to Nadkarni and Hofmann (2012), Facebook use is driven by two main motives: the need to belong and the fear of ostracism. Social networks are used for “social grooming” (Tufekci, 2008), that is, for fostering relationships to satisfy one’s need to belong. On the one hand, Facebook can, thus, be a positive tool for social monitoring, its use even increasing the “inquisitiveness” about one’s social network (Karakayali & Kilic, 2013): Facebook users want to learn more about their own social network, their actions, interests, and opinions, as this information is useful for maintaining or even intensifying the relationships. Just as the posted profile information, status updates, and holiday pictures, news recommendations by friends can actively be used for this form of digital social monitoring. By reading the recommended article, the user can expect to learn more about the political opinions and interests of their friends, which may be useful for future interactions by reducing communicatory uncertainty and, thus, fulfill the need for performance information (Atkin, 1973). News recommendations by strong ties should, thus, have a greater informational utility as they allow users to increase their knowledge of the people they often interact with or value highly.
However, there is also a downside to this usefulness of Facebook for social monitoring: It occurs in both directions. Facebook users know that their own actions are also monitored by their social network (Marder, Joinson, & Shankar, 2012). Looking at Facebook chats, Mai, Freudenthaler, Schneider, and Vorderer (2015) found that fear of isolation and ostracism increased the perceived social pressure to respond to messages. Similarly, users may feel social pressure to comply with news recommendations. Even though there is no direct social control—the recommending friend can only see whether friends have followed their recommendation if they react to it—being on Facebook creates a feeling of co-presence. This feeling of being observed (Marder et al., 2012) on Facebook may motivate users to follow the recommendations of a close friend and read the article simply to maintain the relationship, not because they are interested in his or her opinions and interests in the specific situation (i.e., social monitoring). Thus, we propose that both social monitoring and social pressure mediate the effect of tie strength.
Hypothesis 1 (H1): Recommendations by strong ties lead to higher news exposure than recommendations by weak ties and articles without recommendation.
Hypothesis 2 (H2): The effect of tie strength on news exposure is mediated via (a) an increased motivation for social monitoring and (b) greater perceived social pressure.
The informational cues provided by the news recommendation of a friend, however, are not limited to these social cues related to how useful the article might be to manage personal relationships. The recommender may also trigger social cues related to how useful the recommended article might be for political questions (i.e., politico-social cues). According to the informational utility approach, people perceive news to be more useful if it provides them with information to satisfy guidance needs, that is, to learn about and understand developments that may affect them and develop an opinion on them (Atkin, 1973). The usefulness of the information for guidance is perceived as higher if it comes from (or is recommended by) someone with expertise in the relevant field (Metzger & Flanagin, 2013). Especially on Facebook, where users feel (more) overloaded with news and information than, for example, while watching television (Hargittai, Neuman, & Curry, 2012), politico-social cues gain importance. Turcotte et al. (2015) demonstrated that opinion leaders’ news recommendations on SNS increased the trust in the recommended news outlet and led to more intentional news exposure (news seeking) in the recommended news outlet than recommendations by persons not perceived as opinion leaders. An article shared by a friend who is perceived to be knowledgeable about the subject should, thus, have greater informational utility for the recipient because it signals that the article is perceived as relevant and recommendable by someone who has a comparatively high level of knowledge on the topic allowing the recipient to apply the expert heuristic.
Hypothesis 3 (H3): Recommendations by Facebook friends perceived to be highly knowledgeable about the topic of the news article lead to higher news exposure than those by less knowledgeable friends and articles without a recommendation.
According to partisan selective exposure theory, people employ a confirmation bias when selecting news and are more willing to expose themselves to attitude-consistent than to attitude-discrepant information as this fulfills their need for reinforcement (Knobloch-Westerwick, Johnson, & Westerwick, 2015; Metzger & Flanagin, 2013). Hence, knowing whether the friend shares the same political leanings could serve as a further heuristic to help decide whether the article contains useful information and should be read. The results of large-scale data analyses support this proposed mechanism on SNS—at least for political issues. Bakshy, Messing, and Adamic (2015) analyzed the subsequent usage of seven million shared news articles on Facebook and found that only 20% to 30% of all clicked-on news articles were from politically dissimilar ties; the majority of articles that were ultimately read were recommended by like-minded ties.
Hypothesis 4 (H4): Recommendations by Facebook friends perceived as having a similar opinion on the topic of the news article lead to higher news exposure than those by friends with a dissimilar opinion and articles without a recommendation.
Within the rationale of confirmation bias, it seems likely that the expected effect of the perceived knowledge of the recommender (see H3) is not independent of the perceived opinion similarity. Users might expect news recommendations by friends with high topical knowledge to be even more useful when the recommended information is likely to be not only of superior quality but also to confirm existing beliefs. In contrast, news recommended by friends with high knowledge but a dissimilar opinion might still be perceived as useful for guidance needs and for specific goals such as preparing for a debate on the topic (Hart et al., 2009). At the same time, however, those recommendations could pose a serious threat to opinion reinforcement as they can be expected to contain strong counterarguments to one’s own opinion and should, thus, be selected less (Hart et al., 2009). We investigate this possible interaction by asking whether the effect of the perceived knowledge of the recommender persists regardless of the expected utility to reinforce existing opinions.
Research Question 1 (RQ1): Does a friend’s perceived political knowledge interact with perceived opinion similarity in predicting news exposure?
To better understand why political knowledge and similarity of the recommending friend might affect the recipient’s news exposure, we will also take a look at the role of perceived issue importance as both an independent outcome of news recommendations and as a mediator for news exposure. Traditionally, perceived issue importance was seen—on an aggregate level—as being driven by the media agenda in agenda-setting processes. When taking a more fine-grained look, however, it emerges that the perception of the relevance of a specific issue is not universal, it differs between social groups, or more precisely, issue publics, which are each more likely to select articles on “their” issues (Bolsen & Leeper, 2013). Furthermore, people are aware of these differences in issue importance between different groups and will adapt their assessments of issue importance accordingly: They are aware that some issues are more important to themselves personally than to their social group or to society at large and vice versa (Glynn, Ostman, & McDonald, 1995).
Moreover, recent research has pointed out that it does not require exposure to complete news items for changes in perceptions of issue relevance to occur. According to Stoycheff, Pingree, Peifer, and Sui (2018), agenda setting can also occur as agenda cueing when cues such as the number of posts about a topic are used to evaluate the relative importance of this topic. Still, given the social nature of INE on social networks, citizens’ perception of issue importance should be affected by the way in which it is brought to their attention, that is, who “shared” and, thus, recommended the article. By sharing an article, the recommending friend clearly indicates that he or she believes this an important issue. If this friend is knowledgeable on the topic or has similar opinions on it, recipients should be more likely to trust his or her issue assessment and, thus, adjust their own perception of issue relevance, which in turn should increase news exposure.
Although these news recommendations should affect all perceptions of issue importance, we assume that their impact should be felt more when assessing issue importance for oneself and for one’s social group as people are focused on their own self-representation and the contact with their immediate social group on Facebook (Alhabash & Ma, 2017).
Hypothesis 5 (H5): Recommendations by friends that are perceived as having high knowledge or a similar opinion on the topic, and the interaction of both increase perceived issue importance for (a) society, (b) one’s own social environment, and (c) oneself.
Perceived issue importance or topic relevance has also long since been identified as a main driver of news selection (Rubin, 1994) and might, thus, mediate the recommender effects on news exposure: As the recommenders’ high topical knowledge or opinion similarity increase the perceived issue importance, this could encourage news exposure. It remains to be seen, however, whether it is the perceived general relevance or the perceived personal relevance of an issue that guides news selection (Bolsen & Leeper, 2013). Thus, we ask more broadly which kind of issue importance mediates the effects of the politico-social characteristics of the recommender.
Research Question 2 (RQ2): Does the perceived importance of an issue for (a) society, or (b) one’s own social environment, or (c) oneself mediate the effects of the friend’s perceived political knowledge, of the friend’s perceived opinion similarity, and of their interaction on news exposure?

Media Cues as a Heuristic for News Exposure

Recipients have developed news preferences outside of Facebook, for example, based on their political views (Iyengar & Hahn, 2009), their preference for entertainment rather than news (Aalberg, Blekesaune, & Elvestad, 2013), or for negative content and tabloid packaging (Kleemans, Hendriks Vettehen, Beentjes, & Eisinga, 2011). As media outlets share the links to their news articles on Facebook, the users’ news preferences should guide their news selection on the SNS, too. Therefore, we expect that recipients’ news exposure differs for media source cues from legacy, tabloid, and digital-born media according to their preferences.
Three possible underlying psychological mechanisms can explain how media source cues structure Facebook users’ news exposure: They signal the credibility and societal relevance of political information, but also spark curiosity about an unknown outlet. First, if a reader perceives an outlet to be very credible, they are more likely to read articles from this source (Choi, Watt, & Lynch, 2006). Legacy news media (e.g., newspapers of record, such as the New York Times) are perceived to be highly credible sources of political information. By contrast, citizens consider the tabloid media (e.g., The Sun) to have significantly lower credibility (Urban & Schweiger, 2014). Experimental data confirm that users rely on media source cues to estimate the credibility of the information offered. The higher perceived credibility of legacy media increases the likelihood that SNS users will select articles from quality media rather than tabloids (Winter & Krämer, 2014).
Second, users may choose to read a news story because of the societal relevance of the news outlet. In most countries, one or more news outlets are perceived as central sources whose news coverage is followed by a large share of the population, by political elites, or by other news media (Nielsen, 2012). Although articles from digital-born news providers look very similar to the products of mainstream media when shared on Facebook, the recipients lack previous experience with them, so their brand logo cannot serve as a media source cue on the potential credibility or societal relevance of the outlet.
Third, a shared article from an unknown source might spark curiosity. Stumbling over an unknown outlet raises arousal and, thus, curiosity as a coping mechanism. As they are easily able to find out more about the outlet—it is literally only one click away—their interest in reading the article increases (Silvia, 2008). Articles from an unknown source might, thus, be read out of curiosity about the newly discovered outlet.
Because legacy media are perceived to be more credible and relevant, we expect legacy media to have a positive mediation effect via perceived credibility and societal relevance on reading intention compared with digital-born outlets and tabloids. We also expect outlet curiosity to positively mediate user’s intention to read shared articles from digital-born outlets compared with legacy outlets and tabloids.
Hypothesis 6 (H6): News exposure to recommended articles differs by the type of media outlet.
Hypothesis 7 (H7): The effect of media type on news exposure is mediated through (a) perceived credibility of the outlet, (b) perceived societal relevance of the outlet, and (c) curiosity about the outlet.

Method

Design and Sample

We conducted a randomized 3 × 3 between-subject, multi-stimulus online experiment with two additional quasi-factors in Germany in March 2017. In all conditions, participants saw a mock-up Facebook stream containing a teaser for a political news article. First, we varied the type of centrist media as the source of the article (legacy newspaper vs. unknown digital-born outlet vs. tabloid newspaper). Second, tie strength manipulated whether the article was shared and recommended by a close friend of the participant (a strong tie), by a distant friend (a weak tie), or whether the article appeared without any recommendation in the Facebook stream. The recommender’s political knowledge and opinion similarity were both measured as non-manipulated quasi-factors, that is, as perceived by the participant receiving the recommendation.
Using an online access panel provided by the survey company Respondi, we recruited an interlocked quota sample representative in terms of age and gender of German Facebook users between 18 and 69 (following the official Facebook definition, we defined users as those using their Facebook profile at least once a month). We tried to reach a statistical power (1-β) of .95, which would require 560 finished interviews for medium effect sizes (d = .5) given our complex design. However, necessary data cleansing due to failed awareness checks left us with N = 507 finished interviews (completion rate: 31.05%), which equates to a power of .92 assuming no missing values on covariates or quasi-factors. The average age was M = 38.45 (SD = 12.49). Similar to the German Facebook population, there were slightly more men (n = 268, 52.9%) than women in the sample, and about half of the participants reported holding at least a high school degree (n = 281, 55.9%). Participants received vouchers for the completion of the survey.

Procedure and Manipulation

After receiving their consent, age, gender, education, residency, and frequency of Facebook use were measured. Then, participants answered questions on their interest in different topics. Next, we manipulated the tie strength, depending on the randomly assigned experimental group: We either prompted participants to think of a person to whom they feel close and with whom they have relatively frequent contact via Facebook or of a person from the participant’s distant network (weak tie condition):
Please think of a person who is close to you (you only know distantly), with whom you feel personally strongly connected (feel loosely connected) and with whom you have regular (rare) contact via Facebook, e.g., through seeing his or her Facebook posts or through private messages. This could be a friend, a relative or your partner (former school friend, distant acquaintance or a former workmate), for example.
To amplify the manipulation, participants wrote down the name of the person and rated how close they feel to the person and how often they had contact with them via Facebook. We presented the name of this person in each question and told respondents that they should keep them in mind before the stimulus was shown next.
The stimuli for all conditions consisted of three parts (see Online Appendix B). The first part told participants to imagine that the following two posts originated from the chosen person, displaying the name again. Participants in the “no recommendation” condition saw the same reminder, but were told that only the first post originated from the imagined person. The second part was an apolitical post by the imagined recommender about vacation pictures. This part was constant in all conditions to increase the realism of the Facebook stream. The third part consisted of a posted news article showing the headline, a picture, and a short teaser. For tie strength, the weak and strong tie condition saw the news article as a shared post with a recommending comment as it typically appears on Facebook. The profile picture and the name of the recommender were pixelated so that participants could imagine the profile picture and the name of the chosen person. For participants in the no recommendation conditions, the news article appeared without a recommendation as an original post by the media outlet. For the media source cue, we varied the logo, name, and URL of the media outlet from which the news article was initially posted using their real Facebook pages.
To enhance our external validity, we used three multi-stimulus factors. First, we used two different but comparable media outlets per media type: Süddeutsche Zeitung (645,099 fans as of March 15, 2017) and Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung (458,649 fans) as legacy outlets; and Telepolis (20,574 fans) and Krautreporter (86,879 fans) as unknown digital-born media. The only exception is the tabloid condition, because Bild Zeitung (2,280,155 fans) is the only national tabloid. Second, the news article dealt with one of five comparable topics (regulation of monopolies, elections in Serbia, transparency of lawsuits, water protection, and broadband expansion), all of which were political topics, hard news, moderately relevant, and without issue ownership of a specific party. Third, we used two comparable sentences for the recommendation of the news article by the Facebook friend: “Finally a report that summarizes the whole issue. It’s worth it!” and “Finally an article that puts the topic straight. Readable!” Due to these multi-stimulus factors, our experiment used 135 different stimuli for the nine experimental conditions.
We measured our mediators and dependent variable after the participants had seen the stimulus. Next, we asked for quasi-factors and manipulation checks. Lastly, the participants answered some basic questions on their general media use before debriefing.

Measures

Dependent variable

The intention to read the news article was measured through 4 items using 6-point scales from 1 to 6 (e.g., “I would like to read what the news article contains”). The items showed high reliability (Cronbach’s α = .97) and were summarized to an index (M = 3.87, SD = 1.45).

Politico-social quasi-factors

Regarding the topic of the news article, we measured perceived opinion similarity between the participant and the recommender using a single item on a 7-point scale from 1 to 7 (“Please remember the topic of the news article. Would you say that you and [name] have rather the same or rather an opposite opinion on that issue?”, M = 5.11, SD = 1.32). With a second single item using a 7-point scale, the participant assessed the perceived knowledge of the recommender on this topic (“What do you think, how much knowledge has [name] about the topic of the news article?”, M = 3.80, SD = 1.64).

Politico-social cue mediators

The perceived importance of the topic of the news article was measured on three dimensions using 3 items on 6-point scales for each dimension: perceived importance of the topic for the society (e.g., “The topic is important for the whole society,” α = .94, M = 4.14, SD = 1.13), for own social environment (e.g., “The topic is relevant in my social environment,” α = .97, M = 3.27, SD = 1.35), and for oneself (e.g., “The topic is significant to me,” α = .97, M = 3.43, SD = 1.44).

Social cue mediators

Perceived social pressure to read the article was measured with 3 items (e.g., “I would have read the article because my social environment would expect it of me”) on 6-point scales and summarized to an index (α = .90, M = 1.71, SD = 1.03). Motivation for social monitoring was assessed with 3 items (e.g., “I would have read the article because I could better assess what opinion [name] has on this topic”), again using 6-point scales and computing an index (α = .97, M = 3.22, SD = 1.54).

Media cue mediators

The participants were asked to choose the one media outlet they had seen in the stimulus from a list of 12 names. The perceived credibility of the outlet was measured with 5 items adapted from Tsfati (2010), each of which used 6-point scales (e.g., “The media outlet is credible,” α = .98, M = 3.08, SD = 1.53). Societal relevance of the outlet was asked with 4 items, again on 6-point scales (e.g., “Issues concerning the whole society are the focus of the media outlet,” α = .93, M = 3.81, SD = 1.30). In addition, we gauged participants’ outlet curiosity using 1 item (“I would have read the article because I did not know the outlet and was curious what kind of outlet it is,” M = 2.25, SD = 1.42). Only participants who correctly remembered the media outlet were asked their perceptions of the outlet to which they were exposed (n = 254, 50.1%). However, our analyses still include those participants who could not remember the outlet because we assume that media cues could be effective even without a correct recall of the outlet. Participants with missing values on these mediators will only be excluded pairwise when testing the media cue mediators.

Control variables

We measured Facebook usage with 2 items allowing us to calculate the Facebook use per week in hours (M = 6.25, SD = 8.77). To measure the previous topic interest, we used a battery of 10 items on 7-point scales. Five of them asked for the participants’ interest in each of the five topics we used for the news article, and five were on other topics for distraction (M = 4.27, SD = 1.67). Lastly, we asked whether the participants are following any news site on Facebook: 59.8% (n = 303) reported that they did not follow any news site. We performed a simultaneous confirmatory factor analysis with all our measures showing that all items loaded on the expected factors (Root Mean Square Error Approximation [RMSEA] = .030, Comparative Fit Index [CFI] = .987, Standardized Root Mean Square Residual [SRMR] = .034).

Manipulation Check and Tests of Internal Validity

We asked the perceived social utility of information of the chosen recommender at the end of the questionnaire with 4 items on 6-point scales (e.g., “I like to be up to date regarding [name],” α = .97, M = 3.83, SD = 1.68). An independent t test confirmed that information from strong contacts (M = 5.14, SD = .84) had a significantly higher social utility than information from weak contacts, M = 2.43, SD = 1.15, t(370) = 26.03, p < .001, d = 2.69. We also asked about participants’ familiarity with different media outlets revealing that we succeeded in selecting media outlets with differing degrees of familiarity for the manipulation: 90% knew the screened legacy outlet, and 95.3% knew the tabloid, but only 10.2% were familiar with the digital-born outlet, χ2 (df = 2, n = 507) = 337.60, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .82.
Furthermore, we tested the internal validity of the experiment. First, we confirmed that the manipulation of the tie strength had no unintended spillover effects on users’ intention to read or on measured mediators of the control group. Second, we checked whether our multi-stimulus factors worked successfully. There were no differences in the dependent variable or any of the mediators between the two recommendation sentences nor between the summarized media outlets per media type. In addition, the multi-stimulus factor topic showed no main effect on the dependent variable nor on the social, and media cue, mediators nor interaction effects with our independent variables on these outcomes. That is, the results do not vary systematically by the five topics of the news articles. However, regarding our three politico-social mediators, the water protection issue was seen as significantly more important than the other topics. Therefore, we tested for metric invariance between this topic and all other topics displaying satisfying invariance (n = 507, RMSEA = .061, CFI = .957, SRMR = .059), and an alignment procedure showed no differences in the factor loadings. Thus, we summarized the topics as intended for the following data analyses but will control for the higher issue importance of the water protection topic in the mediation analysis.

Results

To test our proposed main and interaction effects (H1, H3, H4, RQ1, H6), we computed a factorial between-subject ANCOVA with the independent variables tie strength, knowledge of the recommender, opinion similarity, media type, and all possible interactions between them; the covariates previous topic interest, Facebook use per week, and the general following of any news site on Facebook; and reading intention as the dependent variable (see Online Appendix A for descriptive data and Table 1 for model coefficients). The recommender’s level of knowledge and opinion similarity were integrated by using median splits (ndissimilar (1-5) = 151, 53.9%; nsimilar (6-7) = 129, 46.1%; nlow knowledge (1-3) = 118, 42.1%; nhigh knowledge (4-7) = 162, 57.9%). Due to missing values on covariates and quasi-factors, our sample size decreased to n = 382 for this analysis. That is, the statistical power is reduced to .79 to find effects with a medium size (d = .5) but is still high enough to find strong effects (d = .8) with 1-β = .99. The model explained 27% of the variance (R2 = .27, R2adjusted = .21) and was significant (p < .001).
Table 1. ANCOVA Test of Between-Subjects Effects for Reading Intention by Media Type, Tie Strength, Opinion Similarity, Knowledge Recommender, Their Interactions, and Covariates (n = 382).
Factor df F p η p 2
Adjusted model 29 4.45 <.001*** .27
CV Previous topic interest 1 30.26 <.001*** .08
CV Facebook use per week 1 4.05 .045* .01
CV Following any news site (yes = 1) 1 9.12 .003** .03
Media type 2 4.20 .016* .02
Tie strength 1 7.08 .008** .02
Opinion similarity (high = 1) 1 0.41 .525 .00
Knowledge recommender (high = 1) 1 9.41 .002** .03
Media type × Tie strength 2 0.14 .870 .00
Media type × Opinion similarity 2 0.34 .717 .00
Media type × Knowledge recommender 2 0.71 .490 .00
Opinion similarity × Knowledge recommender 2 12.30 .001** .03
Note. R2 = .27, R2adjusted = .21, model is fully saturated but only significant (p < .05) factors or factors of interest for the hypotheses are displayed. CV = covariate.
*
p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
For tie strength (H1), we revealed a significant main effect (F(1, 352) = 4.20, p = .016, ηp2 = .02). Simple effects displayed that articles recommended by close friends led to a significantly (p = .024) higher reading intention (M = 4.21, SE = .11) than those recommended by distant contacts (M = 3.77, SE = .13) or not recommended articles (M = 3.79, SE = .13, p = .035; all simple effects analyses included the three covariates and used the Šidák correction, all reported p values are two-tailed). There were no differences between articles that were not recommended or recommended by distant contacts (p = .99). While we did not find a main effect of opinion similarity (H4, F(1, 352) = .41, p = .53), we detected a significant main effect for the recommender’s level of knowledge (H3, F(1, 352) = 9.41, p = .002, ηp2 = .03). Simple effects showed that recommendations from friends perceived to have high levels of knowledge about the topic led to a significantly (p = .007) higher intention to read an article (M = 4.24, SE = .11) than recommendations from friends perceived to have low levels of knowledge (M = 3.73, SE = .13) and articles that were not recommended (M = 3.79, SE = .13, p = .020); there was no difference between low levels of knowledge and no recommendation (p = .987).
For the media type (H6), we found a significant main effect (F(2, 352) = 4.20, p = .016, ηp2 = .02). Simple effects showed that the intention to read articles from legacy newspapers (M = 4.22, SE = .13) was significantly (p = .030) higher than for tabloids (M = 3.76, SE = .13), but there were no differences between legacy and digital-born outlets (M = 3.92, SE = .13, p = .28) or between tabloids and digital-born outlets (p = .76).
We found a significant interaction between opinion similarity and recommender’s level of knowledge (RQ1, F(2, 352) = 12.30, p = .001, ηp2 = .03). Simple effects showed that the aforementioned main effect of knowledge is mainly shaped by the condition of high opinion similarity (Msimilar high knowledge = 4.58, SEsimilar high knowledge = .15, Msimilar low knowledge = 3.50, SEsimilar low knowledge = .20, p < .001) whereas knowledge does not have an effect when opinions are dissimilar (Mdissimilar high knowledge = 3.90, SEdissimilar high knowledge = .15, Mdissimilar low knowledge = 3.97, SEdissimilar low knowledge = .15, p = .747). To secure our finding, we performed an additional moderation analysis with PROCESS using the metric, mean-centered values for recommender’s level of knowledge and opinion similarity, excluding the conditions with no recommendation from the analysis (new n = 289) and controlling for the influence of tie strength. This analysis confirmed the significance of both the recommender’s level of knowledge (b = .20, p = .006) and the interaction between knowledge and opinion similarity (b = .08, p = .039). As Figure 1 illustrates, the effect of knowledge depends on at least a moderate opinion similarity. The Johnson-Neyman significance value is –.77 for the mean-centered opinion similarity, equating to a value of 4.39 on the non-centered 7-point scale.
Figure 1. Effect of the knowledge of the recommender by opinion similarity controlled for tie strength (n = 289).
Note. The variables knowledge of the recommender and opinion similarity were mean centered before calculating its interaction.
In addition, there were no interactions between media type and tie strength (F(2, 352) = .14, p = .87), media type and opinion similarity (F(2, 352) = .34, p = .717), or media type and recommender’s level of knowledge (F(2, 352) = .71, p = .49). That is, the recommender cues work the same for the three different media types.
To test for the proposed mediation processes (H2, RQ2, H7) and the experimental effect on issue importance (H5) beyond the already described main and interaction effects, we performed a structural equation model in R’s latent variable analysis (lavaan) package with the robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator using tie strength, opinion similarity, recommender’s level of knowledge, its interaction, and media source as exogenous variables; the social, politico-social, and media cue mediators as latent endogenous variables; and reading intention as the final endogenous variable. Again, we excluded the conditions without recommendation, controlled for the same three covariates, and used the metric mean-centered values for recommender’s level of knowledge, opinion similarity, and its interaction. We split the categorical independent variable media type into dummies using legacy outlets as the respective baseline. In addition, we controlled for the topic of water protection to account for its greater perceived issue importance (CFI = .977, RMSEA = .034, SRMR = .032).
The results (see Figure 2 for the structural model and Table 2 for statistical tests on indirect effects) show that tie strength influenced the two social mediators. Strong ties increased social pressure (b = .48, p < .001) and social monitoring (b = .90, p < .001), but only social monitoring is associated with reading intention (b = .40, p < .001). The indirect effect of tie strength via social monitoring (H2a) is significant (unstandardized coefficient = .36, p < .001).
Figure 2. Estimated structural model (n = 332).
Note. Displayed are only significant (p < .05) unstandardized coefficients. Black paths were hypothesized, gray paths are additional signficant coefficients. Measurement model, covariates, and correlations between mediators are included but not shown for clarity reasons. CFI = .977, RMSEA = .034, SRMR = .032. CFI = comparitive fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Table 2. Indirect and Total Effects of Media Type, Tie Strength, Opinion Similarity, Knowledge Recommender, and the Interaction Between Opinion Similarity and Knowledge Recommender on Reading Intention (n = 332).
  Unstandardized coefficient [95% CI] p
Tie strength (0 = weak, 1 = strong)
 Indirect: tie strength via social monitoring (H2a) .36 [.21, .51] <.001***
 Total: tie strength on reading intention (H1) .44 [.16, .73] .002**
Knowledge recommender
 Indirect: knowledge via social monitoring (non-hypothesized) .08 [.03, .12] <.001***
 Indirect: knowledge via issue importance self (RQ2c) .03 [.00, .07] .056
 Total: knowledge on reading intention (H3) .15 [.05, .24] .003**
OS × KR
 Indirect: OS × KR via issue importance self (RQ2c) .03 [.01, .06] .041*
 Total: OS × KR on reading intention (RQ1) .07 [.01, .13] .037*
Legacy media (0) vs. tabloid (1)
 Indirect: legacy vs. tabloid via societal relevance outlet (H7b) −.48 [−.86, −.09] .015*
 Total: legacy vs. tabloid on reading intention (H6) −.48 [−.81, −.15] .005**
Legacy media (0) vs. digital-born (1)
 Indirect: legacy vs. digital born via societal relevance (H7b) −.25 [−.51, .00] .055
 Total: legacy vs. digital-born on reading intention (H6) −.37 [−.67, −.07] .015*
Note. Only significant (p < .05) or marginally significant (p < .06) coefficients are displayed. Non-significant coefficents and included covariates are not shown for clarity reasons. CI = confidence interval; H = hypothesis; RQ = research question; OS = opinion similarity; KR = knowledge recommender.
p < .06. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
The recommender’s level of knowledge affected all three politico-social mediators (H5a issue importance society: b = .08, p = .035, H5b issue importance social environment: b = .19, p < .001, H5c issue importance self: b = .14, p = .001). Only issue importance for oneself in turn correlates with reading intention (b = .24, p = .018). However, the indirect effect of knowledge via issue importance for oneself was only marginally significant (unstandardized coefficient = .03, p = .056, RQ2c). Instead, the recommender’s knowledge showed an indirect effect on reading intention via social monitoring (unstandardized coefficient = .08, p < .001). Opinion similarity showed no effects, but the interaction between knowledge and similarity increased the perceived issue importance of the social environment (H5b, b = .08, p = .006) and oneself (H5c, b = .13, p < .001). However, only the indirect effect on reading intention via issue importance for oneself was significant (unstandardized coefficient = .03, p = .041, RQ2c).
Media type had no influence on the social or politico-social mediators; it only exerted effects on the media mediators. Tabloid articles led to a significantly lower perception of outlet credibility (b = −2.29, p < .001) and societal relevance of the outlet (b = −1.23, p < .001) than those from legacy media. Among these mediators, only the indirect effect on reading intention via the societal relevance of the outlet was significant (unstandardized coefficient = –.48, p = .015, H7b). Digital-born media were perceived as less credible (b = –.86, p = .003) and less societally relevant (b = –.65, p = .003) than legacy outlets, but only the indirect effect via societal relevance was at least marginally significant (unstandardized coefficient = –.25, p = .055, H7b).

Discussion

Facebook (and other SNS) have been seen as promising sources of INE to assure an informed citizenry (Lee & Kim, 2017). However, the inherently social character of INE on Facebook may lead to changes in the heuristics used by citizens to decide which of the news articles that they incidentally stumble upon will actually be read.
According to our experiment, there are three paths that encourage news exposure based on INC among Facebook users. First, social cues of the recommender increase reading intention via social monitoring: If the recipient feels close to the recommending friend, he or she will read the recommended article to increase their knowledge of the recommender. Although respondents feel stronger social pressure to read articles recommended by close ties, this does not produce greater intentions to read. More importantly, recommendations from weak ties do not motivate recipients to read the recommended article compared with articles that are not recommended. In other words, though weak ties are expected to be more diverse and should, thus, have a higher potential to recommend counterattitudinal information, Facebook users are less likely to follow their recommendations, regardless of their political knowledge or the similarity of their opinions, or how respectable the recommended media source is. This phenomenon is further enhanced by the Facebook algorithm that assures that recommendations from weak ties as well as recommendations that do not match a user’s interests are less likely to appear in their news stream. Hence, INE on Facebook can only have a positive impact on exposure to political news, and in particular on exposure to politically diverse news, if users have close ties to people who are interested in politics and have different political outlooks.
The second path reflects the importance of opinion leaders for INE in a social setting such as Facebook (see also Turcotte et al., 2015): Following recommendations by friends with high levels of topical knowledge and similar political opinions, participants believed the issue of the recommended article was more relevant to them personally and were, hence, more likely to read it. This, again, may facilitate exposure to like-minded news articles, but not cross-cutting exposure. Interestingly, political similarity alone does not predict reading intentions: According to our data, people are not motivated by a simple confirmation bias. They will not read just any article because they expect it to confirm their views; they will read articles by recommenders with similar opinions and high political knowledge, which are more likely to contain useful arguments supporting their views. They are motivated by the expected utility for opinion confirmation. Our results may, thus, represent a next step towards developing a theoretical understanding of how opinion leadership works in SNS.
The third path relates to traditional media source cues: Articles by legacy and (to a lesser degree) digital-born news outlets are perceived to have more societal relevance than tabloid articles and are, thus, more likely to be read when recommended. Contrary to expectations, perceptions of credibility differ by outlet type but have no impact on reading intention. This finding contradicts a number of previous studies on the importance of credibility for news selection (Choi et al., 2006), but might be explained by the particular usage situation on a social network. As news selection on Facebook is a social experience, respondents apparently place more importance on whether an article is likely to contain relevant than credible information. This would indicate that when using Facebook, users are more concerned with what people are talking about than whether it is true or not. This notion is corroborated by our finding that reading intentions, as well as perceptions of credibility and societal relevance, are higher for unknown digital-born outlets than for tabloids. Although legacy outlets are seen as more credible and societally relevant, Facebook users appear to give unknown sources considerable “benefit of the doubt” (at least compared with tabloids). This has important implications for the likelihood of mis- and disinformation by unknown sources being spread on Facebook. In addition, the non-significant interaction between tie strength and media cues indicates that recommender cues work the same for different types of media: Recommendations also have a substantive impact on exposure to tabloids, even though they are seen as significantly less credible than legacy media. In fact, a recommended tabloid article is just as likely to be read (M = 4.03, SE = .18) as an article without a recommendation from legacy media (M = 4.02, SE = .23).
Apart from the effects on reading intention, our study allowed a more detailed look at how agenda-setting effects may occur via agenda cueing on SNS. According to our study, SNS posts may not only affect perceptions of issue importance in general as found by Stoycheff et al. (2018), their impact differs depending on the recommender’s socio-political characteristics and the scope of issue relevance: For evaluating societal issue importance, users followed the recommendations of highly knowledgeable, but not necessarily politically similar friends. In other words, they trusted perceived experts. However, indirect effects on reading intentions were limited to recommendations by highly knowledgeable, politically similar friends, which increased the perceived issue relevance for oneself. Thus, future studies on agenda setting (or agenda cueing) in SNS should include different social- and socio-political characteristics to further distinguish the conditions for changes in perceived issue importance of different scopes.
These interpretations have limitations. First, our manipulation of tie strength did not use the real profiles of participants’ Facebook friends but worked via imagination. However, the significant manipulation check and the main effect of tie strength showed that this manipulation worked. Still, one could argue that the entire experimental situation was rather artificial as the post itself did not appear in participants’ real Facebook stream but as a simple screenshot of a stream, and participants were asked to imagine it was their own news feed. Thus, our experimental setting reduces the ecological validity of our results compared with the study by Turcotte et al. (2015) in which participants were deceived into thinking the friend actually posted the news article on Facebook. Second, our dependent variable only measured the intention to read the article, rather than real usage. Third, the five topics we used for the news articles were non-polarized. More polarized topics may lead to stronger effects of opinion similarity regardless of the perceived knowledge of the recommender. Fourth, our mediation analyses should be interpreted carefully as the mediator outcomes are only partly explained by our experimental manipulations but also by personal predispositions. That is, the explained variance in news exposure by our mediation model originates only partly from our experimental factors. Lastly, we conducted our experiment in Europe, in a media system in which the audience is well aware of the differentiation between media types with respect to credibility and societal relevance. Thus, replications in different media systems are needed to confirm whether the media source still plays a role independent of personal recommendations.
Despite these limitations, this study showed that personal news recommendations motivate individuals to get involved with political news—if news is recommended by close friends or by contacts with high levels of knowledge and similar opinions on the topic. INE via Facebook may, thus, help keep people connected to the political public sphere, particularly those who are not connected via traditional media. However, our results also suggest that though news contact on Facebook may be “incidental” in the sense of “unintentional,” it does not occur “by chance,” that is, not all Facebook users will benefit in an equal manner. First, the likelihood of unintentionally encountering news (particularly diverse news) will depend for each user on their choice of Facebook friends (who may vary in their news-sharing habits) as well as their past reactions to shared news articles: The less they click on recommended articles, the less they will see news recommendations from friends in the future due to the news-feed algorithm. Second, people do not treat all “incidental” news recommendations the same. Their news exposure following incidental news contact depends on social and political preferences, they prefer to maintain existing strong relations with others and to engage with news they expect to confirm their existing beliefs (by preferring recommendations from like-minded, knowledgeable friends).
In sum, INE via Facebook has the potential to maintain a normatively desirable connection to the public sphere, but this is limited by the configuration of Facebook’s algorithm and the motivations of its users. As our study shows, people tend to follow INE recommendations by more homophile strong ties and knowledgeable, politically similar contacts, that is, INE on Facebook can easily be transformed into a form of self-reinforcing selective exposure. Thus, future research should continue to explore the potential of INE via Facebook and other social media platforms, but should pay close attention to its preconditions and circumstances because not all kinds of INE will contribute equally towards ensuring a well-informed public.

Acknowledgments

We thank the team around Werner Wirth (University of Zurich) for valuable comments on the study design, Peter Schmidt (University of Giessen) for his advice on invariance testing and structure equation modeling, and the anonymous reviewers for their very helpful comments on the paper.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

ORCID iD

References

Aalberg T., Blekesaune A., Elvestad E. (2013). Media choice and informed democracy. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 18, 281-303.
Alhabash S., Ma M. (2017). A tale of four platforms: Motivations and uses of Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, and Snapchat among college students? Social Media & Society, 3, 1-13.
Atkin C. (1973). Instrumental utilities and information seeking. In Clarke P. (Ed.), New models for mass communication research (pp. 205-242). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Bakshy E., Messing S., Adamic L. (2015). Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook. Science, 348, 1130-1132.
Bakshy E., Rosenn I., Marlow C., Adamic L. (2012). The role of social networks in information diffusion. In Mille A., Gandon F., Misselis J., Rabinovich M., Staab S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on World Wide Web (pp. 519-528). New York, NY: ACM.
Bolsen T., Leeper T. J. (2013). Self-interest and attention to news among issue publics. Political Communication, 30, 329-348.
Bond R. M., Fariss C. J., Jones J. J., Kramer A. D. I., Marlow C., Settle J. E., Fowler J. H. (2012). A 61-million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization. Nature, 489, 295-298.
Choi J. H., Watt J. H., Lynch M. (2006). Perceptions of news credibility about the war in Iraq: Why war opponents perceived the Internet as the most credible medium. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 209-229.
Glynn C. J., Ostman R. E., McDonald D. G. (1995). Opinions, perception, and social reality. In Glasser T. L., Salmon C. T. (Eds.), Public opinion and the communication of consent (pp. 249-277). New York, NY: Guilford.
Granovetter M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology, 78, 1360-1380.
Hargittai E., Neuman W. R., Curry O. (2012). Taming the information tide: Perceptions of information overload in the American home. The Information Society, 28, 161-173.
Hart W., Albarracín D., Eagly A. H., Brechan I., Lindberg M. J., Merrill L. (2009). Feeling validated versus being correct: A meta-analysis of selective exposure to information. Psychological Bulletin, 135, S555-S588.
Iyengar S., Hahn K. S. (2009). Red media, blue media: Evidence of ideological selectivity in media use. Journal of Communication, 59, 19-39.
Karakayali N., Kilic A. (2013). More network conscious than ever? Challenges, strategies, and analytic labor of users in the Facebook environment. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 18, 175-193.
Karnowski V., Kümpel A. S., Leonhard L., Leiner D. J. (2017). From incidental news exposure to news engagement. How perceptions of the news post and news usage patterns influence engagement with news articles encountered on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 76, 42-50.
Kim Y., Chen H.-T., Gil de Zúñiga H. (2013). Stumbling upon news on the Internet: Effects of incidental news exposure and relative entertainment use on political engagement. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 2607-2614.
Kleemans M., Hendriks Vettehen P. G. J., Beentjes J. W. J., Eisinga R. (2011). The influence of age and gender on preferences for negative content and tabloid packaging in television news stories. Communication Research, 39, 679-697.
Knobloch-Westerwick S., Carpentier F. D., Blumhoff A., Nickel N. (2005). Selective exposure effects for positive and negative news: Testing the robustness of the informational utility model. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 82, 181-195.
Knobloch-Westerwick S., Johnson B. K., Westerwick A. (2015). Confirmation bias in online searches: Impacts of selective exposure before an election on political attitude strength and shifts. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20, 171-187.
Knobloch-Westerwick S., Kleinman S. B. (2012). Preelection selective exposure: Confirmation bias versus informational utility. Communication Research, 39, 170-193.
Lee J. K., Kim E. (2017). Incidental exposure to news: Predictors in the social media setting and effects on information gain online. Computers in Human Behavior, 75, 1008-1015.
Mai L. M., Freudenthaler R., Schneider F. M., Vorderer P. (2015). “I know you’ve seen it!” Individual and social factors for users’ chatting behavior on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 296-302.
Marder B., Joinson A., Shankar A. (2012). Every post you make, every pic you take, I’ll be watching you: Behind social spheres on Facebook. In Sprague R. H. (Ed.), Proceedings of the 45th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (pp. 859-868).
Marewski J. N., Galesic M., Gigerenzer G. (2009). Fast and frugal media choices. In Hartmann T. (Ed.), Media choice: A theoretical and empirical overview (pp. 107-128). New York, NY: Routledge.
Metzger M. J., Flanagin A. J. (2013). Credibility and trust of information in online environments: The use of cognitive heuristics. Journal of Pragmatics, 59, 210-220.
Nadkarni A., Hofmann S. G. (2012). Why do people use Facebook? Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 243-249.
Nielsen R. K. (2012). Ten years that shook the media world: Big questions and big trends in international media developments. Retrieved from https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2017-09/Nielsen%20-%20Ten%20Years%20that%20Shook%20the%20Media_0.pdf
Prior M. (2007). Post-broadcast democracy: How media choice increases inequality in political involvement and polarizes elections. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Rubin A. M. (1994). Media uses and effects: A uses-and-gratifications perspective. In Bryant J., Zillmann D. (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (pp. 417-436). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Scheufele D. A., Nisbet M. C. (2002). Being a citizen online: New opportunities and dead ends. Harvard International Journal of Press/Politics, 7(3), 55-75.
Silvia P. J. (2008). Interest—The curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 57-60.
Stoycheff E., Pingree R. J., Peifer J. T., Sui M. (2018). Agenda cueing effects of news and social media. Media Psychology, 21, 182-201.
Stroud N. J. (2010). Polarization and partisan selective exposure. Journal of Communication, 60, 556-576.
Tewksbury D., Weaver A. J., Maddex B. D. (2001). Accidentally informed: Incidental news exposure on the world wide web. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 78, 533-554.
Thorson K., Wells C. (2015). Curated flows: A framework for mapping media exposure in the digital age. Communication Theory, 26, 309-328.
Tsfati Y. (2010). Online news exposure and trust in the mainstream media: Exploring possible associations. American Behavioral Scientist, 54, 22-42.
Tufekci Z. (2008). Grooming, gossip, Facebook and MySpace. What can we learn about these sites from those who won’t assimilate? Information, Communication & Society, 11, 544-564.
Turcotte J., York C., Irving J., Scholl R. M., Pingree R. J. (2015). News recommendations from social media opinion leaders: Effects on media trust and information seeking. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 20, 520-535.
Urban J., Schweiger W. (2014). News quality from the recipients’ perspective: Investigating recipients’ ability to judge the normative quality of news. Journalism Studies, 15, 821-840.
Valeriani A., Vaccari C. (2015). Accidental exposure to politics on social media as online participation equalizer in Germany, Italy, and the United Kingdom. New Media & Society, 18, 1857-1874.
Winter S., Krämer N. C. (2014). A question of credibility: Effects of source cues and recommendations on information selection on news sites and blogs. Communications, 39, 435-456.

Biographies

Johannes Kaiser is a research and teaching assistant at the Department of Communication and Media Research at the University of Zurich. His research focuses on political psychology and the effects of political communication.
Tobias R. Keller is a research and teaching assistant at the Department of Communication and Media Research at the University of Zurich, Switzerland. His research focuses on political messages on social media.
Katharina Kleinen-von Königslöw is professor for digital communication and sustainability at the University of Hamburg. Her research focuses on transnationalization, fragmentation, and entertainization of political communication in the digital age.

Supplementary Material

Please find the following supplemental material available below.

For Open Access articles published under a Creative Commons License, all supplemental material carries the same license as the article it is associated with.

For non-Open Access articles published, all supplemental material carries a non-exclusive license, and permission requests for re-use of supplemental material or any part of supplemental material shall be sent directly to the copyright owner as specified in the copyright notice associated with the article.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
EMAIL ARTICLE LINK
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: October 23, 2018
Issue published: February 2021

Keywords

  1. incidental news exposure
  2. news sharing
  3. social ties
  4. Facebook
  5. experiment

Rights and permissions

© The Author(s) 2018.
Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

Johannes Kaiser
University of Zürich, Switzerland
Tobias R. Keller
Katharina Kleinen-von Königslöw
University of Hamburg, Germany

Notes

Johannes Kaiser, IKMZ—Department of Communication and Media Research, University of Zürich, Andreasstrasse 15, CH-8050 Zürich, Switzerland. Email: [email protected]

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Communication Research.

VIEW ALL JOURNAL METRICS

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 4915

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 48 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 0

  1. The effects of incidental news exposure on social media on Chinese you...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  2. Counteracting the Impact of Online Fake News on Brands
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  3. Accidently acquired? Examining the conditional indirect effects of soc...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  4. Testing the Social Media Produsage Hypothesis
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  5. Social Media Incidental Appearance Exposure and Young People's Body Im...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  6. Avoiding the news to participate in society? The longitudinal relation...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  7. Media Diets of Vegetarians. How News Consumption, Social Media Use and...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  8. An interaction model among enterprise and government actions and publi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  9. Destination Information Search in Social Media and Travel Intention of...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  10. The Corrections Dilemma: Media Retractions Increase Belief Accuracy Bu...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  11. Mobile Selective Exposure: Confirmation Bias and Impact of Social Cues...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  12. Uninformed and Misinformed: Advancing a Theoretical Model for Social M...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  13. The correlation between the Big Five personality traits and political ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  14. Incidental News Exposure on Facebook and Its Relation to Trust in News
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  15. Misinformation in Open and Closed Online Platforms: Impacts and Counte...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  16. News Articles on Social Media: Showing Balanced Content Adds More Cred...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  17. Reading a Snippet on a News Aggregator vs. Clicking through the Full S...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  18. Normalization with Israel: An Analysis of Social Networks Discourse Wi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  19. Seeing political information online incidentally. Effects of first- an...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  20. Deciding what’s (sharable) news: Social movement organizations as cura...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  21. Neither Absent nor Ambient: Incidental News Exposure From the Perspect...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  22. Social Media Information Environments and Their Implications for the U...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  23. Partisan Blocking: Biased Responses to Shared Misinformation Contribut...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  24. Increasing vaccination intention in pandemic times: a social marketing...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  25. Disinformation Sharing Thrives with Fear of Missing Out among Low Cogn...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  26. Digital News Distribution and Intermediaries
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  27. Correcting science misinformation in an authoritarian country: An expe...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  28. Same, same, but different! Qualitative evidence on how algorithmic sel...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  29. Out-of-Place Content: How Repetitive, Offensive, and Opinion-Challengi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  30. What Does Channel Say? Understanding How Social Media Social Capital F...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  31. Disentangling the Influence of Recommender Attributes and News-Story A...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  32. Block, Hide or Follow—Personal News Curation Practices on Social Media
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  33. ‘I saw it on Facebook’: an experimental analysis of political learning...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  34. Peripheral elaboration model: The impact of incidental news exposure o...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  35. From incidental exposure to intentional avoidance: Psychological react...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  36. Network Approaches to Misinformation Evaluation and Correction
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  37. Learning from Incidental Exposure to Political Information in Online E...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  38. Attracting the news: Algorithms, platforms, and reframing incidental e...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  39. The Matthew Effect in social media news use: Assessing inequalities in...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  40. Processing news on social media. The political incidental news exposur...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  41. The ecology of incidental exposure to news in digital media environmen...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  42. Conceptualizing different forms of news processing following incidenta...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  43. Peers Versus Pros: Confirmation Bias in Selective Exposure to User-Gen...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  44. What Affects First- and Second-Level Selective Exposure to Journalisti...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  45. Intentional vs. unintentional influences of social media friends
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  46. Following Politicians on Social Media: Effects for Political Informati...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  47. Does incidental exposure on social media equalize or reinforce partici...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  48. The Social Context of Media Trust: A Network Influence Model
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  49. Getting Tagged, Getting Involved with News? A Mixed-Methods Investigat...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get access

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.