Skip to main content
Intended for healthcare professionals
Restricted access
Research article
First published online September 4, 2019

Stereotypes, emotions, and behaviors associated with animals: A causal test of the stereotype content model and BIAS map

Abstract

Using the stereotype content model (SCM; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002) and the behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes (BIAS) map (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2007), two experiments tested the effect of animal stereotypes on emotions and behavioral tendencies toward animals. As a novel approach, Study 1 (N = 165) manipulated warmth and competence traits of a fictitious animal species (“wallons”) and tested their effect on emotions and behaviors toward those animals. Stereotypical warm-competent and cold-incompetent “wallons” elicited fondness/delight and contempt/disgust, respectively. Cold-competent “wallons” primarily elicited threat but not awe. Warm-incompetent “wallons” were elusive targets, not eliciting specific emotions. The warmth dimension determined active behaviors, promoting facilitation (support/help) and reducing harm (kill/trap). The competence dimension determined passive behaviors, eliciting facilitation (conserve/monitor) and reducing harm (ignore/let them die off). Study 2 (N = 112) tested the relation between animal stereotypes for 25 species and realistic scenarios concerning behavioral tendencies toward animals. Similar to Study 1, stereotypically warm (vs. cold) animals matched with active scenarios, eliciting more facilitation (i.e., national health campaign) but less harm (i.e., fighting animals). Stereotypically competent (vs. incompetent) animals matched with passive scenarios, eliciting more facilitation (i.e., restricted areas) but less harm (i.e., accidental mortality). Accordingly, stereotypes limited the suitability of scenarios toward animals. Although findings are consistent with the SCM/BIAS map framework, several unpredicted results emerged. The mixed support is discussed in detail, along with the implications of an intergroup approach to animals.

Get full access to this article

View all access and purchase options for this article.

References

Amiot C. E., Bastian B. (2015). Toward a psychology of human–animal relations. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 6–47.
Bastian B., Loughnan S., Haslam N., Radke H. R. M. (2012). Don’t mind meat? The denial of mind to animals used for human consumption. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 247–256.
Bruskotter J. T., Fulton D. C. (2012). Will hunters steward wolves? A comment on Treves and Martin. Society & Natural Resources, 25, 97–102.
Caprariello P. A., Cuddy A. J. C., Fiske S. T. (2009). Social structure shapes cultural stereotypes and emotions: A causal test of the stereotype content model. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 12, 147–155.
Cottrell C. A., Neuberg S. L. (2003). Different emotional reactions to different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice.” Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 88, 770–789.
Cuddy A. J. C., Fiske S. T., Glick P. (2007). The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereorypes. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 92, 631–648.
Eddy T. J., Gallup G. G., Povinelli D. J. (1993). Attribution of cognitive states to animals: Anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 49, 87–101.
Falomir-Pichastor J. M., Munoz-Rojas D., Invernizzi F., Mugny G. (2004). Perceived in-group threat as a factor moderating the influence of in-group norms on discrimination against foreigners. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 135–153.
Fehr B., Russell J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464–486.
Fiske S. T., Cuddy A. J. C., Glick P., Xu J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 82, 878–902.
Gray H. M., Gray K., Wegner D. M. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315.
Haslam N., Kashima Y., Loughnan S., Shi J., Suitner C. (2008). Subhuman, inhuman, and superhuman: Contrasting humans with nonhumans in three cultures. Social Cognition, 26, 248–258.
Haslam N., Loughnan S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 399–423.
Henley N. M. (1969). A psychological study of the semantics of animal terms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 176–184.
Hodson G., MacInnis C. C., Costello K. (2014). (Over)valuing humanness as an aggravator of intergroup prejudices and discrimination. In Bain P. G., Vaes J., Leyens J.-P. (Eds.), Humanness and dehumanization (pp. 86–110). London, UK: Psychology Press.
Hunter L., Brehm J. (2003). Qualitative insight into public knowledge of and concern with biodiversity. Human Ecology, 31, 309–320.
Johansson M., Karlsson J., Pedersen E., Flykt A. (2012). Factors governing human fear of brown bear and wolf. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 17, 58–74.
Kellert S. R., Berry J. K. (1980). Knowledge, affection and basic attitudes toward animals in American society: Phase III. Washington, DC: Fish and Wildlife Service, Department of Interior.
Kellert S. R., Black M., Rush C. R., Bath A. J. (1996). Human culture and large carnivore conservation in North America. Conservation Biology, 10, 977–990.
Keltner D., Haidt J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 297–314.
Kervyn N., Fiske S. T., Malone C. (2012). Brands as intentional agents framework: How perceived intentions and ability can map brand perception. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22, 166–176.
Kervyn N., Fiske S. T., Yzervyt Y. (2015). Foretelling the primary dimension of social cognition: Symbolic and realistic threats together predict warmth in the stereotype content model. Social Psychology, 46, 36–45.
Knight A. J. (2008). “Bats, snakes and spiders, oh my!” How aesthetic and negativistic attitudes, and other concepts predict support for species protection. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28, 94–103.
Leyens J. P., Cortés B., Demoulin S., Dovidio J. F., Fiske S. T., Gaunt R., . . . Vaes J. (2003). Emotional prejudice, essentialism, and nationalism: The 2002 Tajfel Lecture. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 703–717.
López A., Atran S., Coley J. D., Medin D. L., Smith E. E. (1997). The tree of life: Universal and cultural features of folk biological taxonomies and inductions. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 251–295.
Loughnan S., Haslam N., Bastian B. (2010). The role of meat consumption in the denial of moral status and mind to meat animals. Appetite, 55, 156–159.
Oppenheimer D. M., Meyvis T., Davidenko N. (2009). Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 867–872.
Piazza J., Landy J. F., Goodwin G. P. (2014). Cruel nature: Harmfulness as an important, overlooked dimension in judgments of moral standing. Cognition, 131, 108–124.
Plous S. (2003). Is there such a thing as prejudice toward animals? In Plous S. (Ed.), Understanding prejudice and discrimination (pp. 509–528). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
Sevillano V., Fiske S. T. (2016a). Animals as social objects: Groups, stereotypes, and intergroup threats. European Psychologist, 21, 206–217.
Sevillano V., Fiske S. T. (2016b). Warmth and competence in animals. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 46, 276–293.
Sevillano V., Fiske S. T. (2017, July). Animals as social objects: Status, cooperation, and threat towards human beings. Paper presented at the 18th General Meeting of the European Association of Social Psychology, Granada, Spain.
Sevillano V., Fiske S. T. (in press). Animals as social groups: An intergroup relations analysis of human–animal conflicts. In Dhont K., Hodson G. (Eds.), Why people love and exploit animals: Bridging insights from academia and advocacy. London, UK: Routledge.
Skogen K. (2001). Who’s is afraid of the big, bad wolf? Young people’s responses to the conflicts over large carnivores in Eastern Norway. Rural Sociology, 6, 203–226.
Smith R. H. (2000). Assimilative and contrastive emotional reactions to upward and downward social comparisons. In Suls J., Wheeler L. (Eds.), Handbook of social comparison: Theory and research (pp. 173–200). New York, NY: Plenum.
Treves A., Wallace R. B., Naughton-Treves L., Morales A. (2006). Co-managing human–wildlife conflicts: A review. Human Dimensions of Wildlife, 11, 1–14.
Van der Berg A. E., ter Heijne M. (2005). Fear versus fascination: An exploration of emotional responses to natural threats. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 261–272.
Weisman K., Dweck C. S., Markman E. M. (2017). Rethinking people’s conceptions of mental life. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 114, 11374–11379.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
EMAIL ARTICLE LINK
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: September 4, 2019
Issue published: September 2019

Keywords

  1. animals
  2. behavior
  3. emotion
  4. groups
  5. stereotype content

Rights and permissions

© The Author(s) 2019.
Request permissions for this article.

Authors

Affiliations

Verónica Sevillano
Susan T. Fiske
Princeton University, USA

Notes

Verónica Sevillano, Department of Methodology and Social Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Campus de Cantoblanco Facultad de Psicología, Madrid 28049, Spain. Email: [email protected]

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Group Processes & Intergroup Relations.

VIEW ALL JOURNAL METRICS

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 2179

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 24 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 25

  1. Differences in General and Specific Attitudes Toward Animals by Diet a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  2. Prokofiev was (almost) right: A cross-cultural investigation of audito...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  3. Understanding Disrespectful Tourist Behavior Through Tourists’ Stereot...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  4. On crocodiles and turtles. Stereotypes, emotional tendencies and impli...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  5. The Effects of Stereotypes About Animals’ Competence and Warmth on Emp...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  6. Can Gender Nouns Influence the Stereotypes of Animals?
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  7. Understanding Residents’ Attitudes Towards Tourists Through Implicit S...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  8. Animals are diverse: distinct forms of animalized dehumanization
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  9. All human social groups are human, but some are more human than others...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  10. The social stereotypes of wolves and brown bears
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  11. Managing Wolves is Managing Narratives: Views of Wolves and Nature Sha...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  12. Friends, food or worth fighting for? A proposed stereotype content mod...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  13. Warmth and competence perceptions of key protagonists are associated w...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  14. The influence of brand sustainability on purchase intentions: the medi...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  15. Do People Care about the Origin of Wildlife? The Role of Social Stereo...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  16. Wolves, Crows, Spiders, and People: A Qualitative Study Yielding a Thr...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  17. Self-Stigma Among People With Mental Health Problems in Terms of Warmt...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  18. Perceived power and smile intensity in service encounters
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  19. Understanding residents’ attitudes towards tourists: Connecting stereo...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  20. Stereotype contents, emotions, and public attitudes: How do Chinese pe...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  21. Bat in the flat — people’s reaction and further actions (results of a ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  22. Social representations of sharks, perceived communality, and attitudin...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  23. Stereotypes of Social Groups in Mainland China in Terms of Warmth and ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  24. Vertical and horizontal inequality are status and power differences: a...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  25. Rethinking human-animal relations: The critical role of social psychol...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

Get access

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Full Text

View Full Text