85,531
Views
680
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Basic Research Article

Trauma and PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys

, , , ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , , & show all
Article: 1353383 | Received 23 Mar 2017, Accepted 06 Jul 2017, Published online: 31 Jul 2017

ABSTRACT

Background: Although post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) onset-persistence is thought to vary significantly by trauma type, most epidemiological surveys are incapable of assessing this because they evaluate lifetime PTSD only for traumas nominated by respondents as their ‘worst.’

Objective: To review research on associations of trauma type with PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys, a series of epidemiological surveys that obtained representative data on trauma-specific PTSD.

Method: WMH Surveys in 24 countries (n = 68,894) assessed 29 lifetime traumas and evaluated PTSD twice for each respondent: once for the ‘worst’ lifetime trauma and separately for a randomly-selected trauma with weighting to adjust for individual differences in trauma exposures. PTSD onset-persistence was evaluated with the WHO Composite International Diagnostic Interview.

Results: In total, 70.4% of respondents experienced lifetime traumas, with exposure averaging 3.2 traumas per capita. Substantial between-trauma differences were found in PTSD onset but less in persistence. Traumas involving interpersonal violence had highest risk. Burden of PTSD, determined by multiplying trauma prevalence by trauma-specific PTSD risk and persistence, was 77.7 person-years/100 respondents. The trauma types with highest proportions of this burden were rape (13.1%), other sexual assault (15.1%), being stalked (9.8%), and unexpected death of a loved one (11.6%). The first three of these four represent relatively uncommon traumas with high PTSD risk and the last a very common trauma with low PTSD risk. The broad category of intimate partner sexual violence accounted for nearly 42.7% of all person-years with PTSD. Prior trauma history predicted both future trauma exposure and future PTSD risk.

Conclusions: Trauma exposure is common throughout the world, unequally distributed, and differential across trauma types with respect to PTSD risk. Although a substantial minority of PTSD cases remits within months after onset, mean symptom duration is considerably longer than previously recognized.

1. Introduction

The fact that only a small minority of people in the population develops post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Atwoli, Stein, Koenen, & McLaughlin, Citation2015) even though the vast majority are exposed to traumas at some time in their life (Benjet et al., Citation2016) has raised questions about individual differences in psychological vulnerability to PTSD. These questions are the subject of considerable research (Liberzon & Abelson, Citation2016; Sayed, Iacoviello, & Charney, Citation2015; Smoller, Citation2016). One prior consideration is the possibility that PTSD risk varies significantly by trauma type. Such differences have been documented, with highest PTSD risk thought to occur after traumas involving interpersonal violence (Caramanica, Brackbill, Stellman, & Farfel, Citation2015; Fossion et al., Citation2015). A related line of research suggests that trauma history is a risk factor for subsequent PTSD, with prior traumas involving violence again possibly of special importance (Lowe, Walsh, Uddin, Galea, & Koenen, Citation2014; Smith, Summers, Dillon, & Cougle, Citation2016). Few studies estimating these differences across trauma types did so using unbiased methods, raising questions about the validity of results regarding these differences. The issue of biasedness comes up because of a common data collection convention in general population epidemiological studies of PTSD whereby respondents are asked about lifetime exposure to each of a wide range of traumas but then assessed for PTSD only for the one trauma nominated by the respondent as their worst or most upsetting lifetime trauma. This approach makes it impossible to estimate conditional risk of PTSD after trauma exposure without upward bias because the traumas for which PTSD is assessed are atypically severe.

One approach to deal with this problem is to assess PTSD twice for epidemiological survey respondents who report experiencing multiple lifetime traumas: once for the trauma nominated by the respondent as their worst lifetime trauma and a second time for a random trauma (i.e. one randomly-selected occurrence of one randomly-selected trauma type). By weighting the random trauma reports by the inverse of their probabilities of selection at the individual level and combining these weighted reports with reports about the worst lifetime trauma, a representative sample can be generated of all lifetime traumas experienced by all survey respondents. The weight would be 1 for respondents who reported lifetime exposure to only one occurrence of one trauma type.

This weighted sample of trauma occurrences can then be used to obtain estimates both of the distribution of trauma exposure in the population and the conditional probability of PTSD after exposure to traumas of different types. These estimates are unbiased if the model is correct, although they will still be subject to the biases of recall error. The current paper reports data on between-trauma differences in the distribution of trauma exposure by trauma type and the risk of PTSD associated with each trauma type in the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. The WMH Surveys are a series of community epidemiological surveys that used this weighting scheme to generate a representative sample of trauma occurrences in the general population of participating countries (Liu et al., Citation2017).

We begin by reporting results on overall lifetime prevalence and basic socio-demographic correlates of trauma exposure in the population. We break down traumas into a number of different types for this purpose. We then examine conditional risk of PTSD after trauma exposure and compare this risk across trauma types. We also examine a number of predictors of PTSD risk among people exposed to traumas controlling for trauma type, including socio-demographic predictors, information about prior trauma exposure, and information about history of mental disorder before exposure to the focal trauma. Next, we examine data on the typical course of PTSD; that is, on the duration of PTSD episodes. We examine the same range of predictors of duration as for onset. Finally, we combine all of the above information into a consolidated portrait of the population burden of PTSD broken down by trauma type. This consolidated portrait takes into consideration differences across traumas in prevalence, conditional risk of PTSD, and the duration of PTSD.

2. Methods and materials

2.1. Sample

The WMH Surveys are a coordinated series of cross-national community epidemiological surveys using consistent sampling, field procedures, and instruments designed to facilitate pooled cross-national analyses of prevalence and correlates of common mental disorders (Kessler & Ustun, Citation2008). The subset of 26 WMH Surveys considered in this paper are those that assessed lifetime PTSD after both worst and random traumas. Five of the surveys were carried out in low/lower-middle income countries (People’s Republic of China [PRC], Colombia, Nigeria, Peru, Ukraine), seven in upper-middle income countries (Brazil, Bulgaria, Colombia [administered after the previously-mentioned Colombian survey, when the country income rating had increased], Lebanon, Mexico, Romania, South Africa), and 14 in high income countries (Australia, Belgium, France, Germany, Israel, Italy, Japan, Netherlands, New Zealand, Northern Ireland, Portugal, Spain [separate national and regional surveys], USA). Each survey was based on a multi-stage clustered area probability sample of adult household residents. Three of the 26 surveys included the urbanized area of their countries (Colombia, Mexico, Peru), and five were based in specific Metropolitan areas (Beijing and Shanghai, PRC; Sao Paulo, Brazil; Medellin, Colombia; Murcia, Spain; six cities in Japan). The Nigerian survey was restricted to specific regions and the other 17 the entire country. More details about the samples are presented in (Stein, de Jonge, Kessler, & Scott, Citationin press).

Table 1. WMH sample characteristics by World Bank income categories.a

WMH interviews were administered face-to-face in respondent homes by trained lay interviewers after obtaining informed consent using procedures approved by local Institutional Review Boards. The response rate had a weighted (by sample size) mean of 70.4% across surveys (between 45.9% in France and 97.2% in Medellin). The interviews were in two parts. Part I, administered to all respondents (n = 125,718), assessed core DSM-IV mental disorders. Part II, administered to all Part I respondents with core disorders and a probability subsample of other respondents (n = 68,894), assessed additional disorders and correlates. Traumas and PTSD were assessed in Part II. The analysis sample considered here includes the 50,855 Part II respondents who reported lifetime trauma exposure. As detailed elsewhere (Heeringa et al., Citation2008), this sample was weighted to match population geographic/socio-demographic distributions and to adjust for under-sampling of Part I non-cases.

2.2. Measures

Traumas: A total of 29 trauma types were assessed, with reports of lifetime exposure followed by questions about number of lifetime occurrences and age at first occurrence of each type. Traumas were divided into seven categories for purposes of analysis: seven related to war (e.g. combatant, civilian in war zone, relief worker, refugee); four related to physical violence (e.g. physically abused by a caregiver as a child, mugged); four related to intimate partner or sexual violence (raped, sexually assaulted, stalked, physically abused by a romantic partner); seven related to accidents (toxic chemical spill, other man-made disaster, natural disaster, life-threatening motor vehicle collision, other accident where the respondent accidentally caused serious injury to another person, other life-threatening accident, life-threatening illness); unexpected or traumatic death of a loved one; four related to traumas that happened to other people (child had life-threatening illness, other traumas that occurred to loved ones, witnessed physical fights at home as a child, witnessed any other trauma); and a residual category of ‘other’ traumas. The latter category included responses to two questions: (i) an open-ended question: Did you ever experience any other extremely traumatic or life-threatening event that I haven’t asked you about? that was backcoded into the other six categories whenever possible and the residual reports coded in an ‘other’ category; and (ii) a question about a ‘private’ trauma: Sometimes people have experiences they don’t want to talk about in interviews. I won’t ask you to describe anything like this but, without telling me what it was, did you ever have a traumatic event that you didn’t tell me about because you didn’t want to talk about it? As shown below, a surprisingly large number of respondents answered this question affirmatively.

PTSD: DSM-IV PTSD was assessed with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) (Kessler & Ustün, Citation2004), a fully-structured lay interview that assesses a wide range of common mental disorders. As noted in the Introduction, PTSD was assessed separately for one random occurrence of one randomly-selected trauma type reported by each respondent selected using a random numbers table for that respondent (the respondent’s random trauma) and separately for the respondent’s self-reported worst trauma. When DSM-IV/CIDI criteria for PTSD were met, the respondent was asked how long symptoms persisted and if the symptoms were still present at the time of interview. Clinical reappraisal interviews with the SCID (Haro et al., Citation2006) blinded to CIDI diagnoses of PTSD (but instructed to focus on the same trauma as the one assessed in the CIDI in order to guarantee valid comparison of diagnoses) documented moderate CIDI–SCID concordance (Landis & Koch, Citation1977) (AUC = .69). Sensitivity and specificity were 38.3% and 99.1%, respectively, resulting in a likelihood ratio positive (Sensitivity/[1-Specificity]) of 42.0 that is well above the 10.0 typically considered definitive for a positive screen (Gardner & Altman, Citation2000). Based on these operating characteristics, a very high proportion of CIDI cases (86.1%) were confirmed by the SCID.

2.3. Analysis methods

Cross-tabulations were used to estimate the distribution of lifetime trauma exposure at the individual level and to examine the distribution of trauma exposure as well as conditional risk of PTSD associated with each trauma type in this trauma-level dataset. Means were calculated within subsamples to estimate average number of trauma occurrences given any. Discrete-time survival analysis with time-varying predictors was used to examine the socio-demographic and prior trauma predictors of each type of trauma exposure and the predictors of persistence of PTSD among cases. As noted in the Introduction, random trauma reports were weighted by the inverse of random trauma probability of selection multiplied by the respondent’s Part II weight to generate a sample representative of all traumas experienced by all respondents. Logistic regression analysis was used to examine predictors of conditional risk of PTSD among the trauma-exposed. Design-adjusted standard errors were used to assess significance of individual predictors and design-based Wald χ2 tests to evaluate the significance of predictor sets.

3. Results

3.1. Prevalence and distribution of trauma exposure

Lifetime exposure to one or more traumas was reported by a weighted 70.4% of Part II WMH respondents (n = 50,855, with 51,196 random and/or worst events; ). Mean number of lifetime trauma types among those with any was 2.9, for 2.0 trauma types for capita (i.e. .704 × 2.9). The distribution of number of types among respondents with any was 32.1% one, 23.4% two, 16.6% three, 10.7% four, 6.5% five, 3.9% six, 2.5% seven, and 4.3% more than seven. By far the most common trauma types were unexpected death of a loved one (reported by 31.4% of respondents) and direct exposure to (i.e. witnessing or discovering) death or serious injury (23.7%). The next most common trauma types at the respondent level were muggings (14.5%), life-threatening automobile accidents (14.0%), and life-threatening illnesses (11.8%). ‘Private’ traumas were reported by 4.9% of respondents. When considered in terms of broader categories, the most common traumas at the respondent level were those that either occurred to a loved one or were witnessed (35.7% of respondents), those involving accidents (34.3%), and unexpected death of a loved one (31.4%) followed by physical violence (22.9%), intimate partner sexual violence (14.0%), war-related traumas (13.1%), and ‘other’ traumas (8.4%).

Table 2. Prevalence and distribution of lifetime traumas in the WMH Surveys (n = 68,894).

The above results do not take into consideration the fact that mean number of exposures varies significantly across trauma types for the 20 traumas that were assessed for frequency (χ219 = 11,729.9, p < .001). (The other trauma types were not assessed for frequency because they represented ongoing situations rather than discrete events.) When we multiply the proportion of respondents with any lifetime exposure to a given trauma type by mean number of exposures among those with any, we find a mean number of exposures to any trauma among people with any of 4.6. This translates into 3.2 lifetime trauma exposures per capita (i.e. .704 with any exposure × 4.6). The distribution of number of trauma occurrences among respondents with any was 25.8% one, 18.0% two, 12.9% three, 9.6% four, 7.0% five, 5.4% six, 4.2% seven, 3.2% eight, 2.4% nine, 2.2% 10, 4.7% 11–14, and 4.5% 15 or more. The most common traumas are those that happen to other people, either unexpected death of a loved one (16.5% of all traumas) or other traumas that happened to a loved one or that the respondent witnessed (25.0%), collectively accounting for over 40% of all trauma exposures. Another 24.6% are accidents and another roughly one-quarter involve either intimate partner sexual violence (9.8%) or physical violence (13.8%). The remaining categories of war-related (7.4%) and ‘other’ (2.8%) traumas are much less common.

3.2. Socio-demographic predictors of trauma exposure

Socio-demographic predictors of trauma exposure in the WMH data have been reported previously (Benjet et al., Citation2016). Using survival analysis, these analyses showed that women are much more likely than men to be exposed to intimate partner sexual violence (OR 2.3), roughly equal to men in odds of unexpected death of a loved one (OR 1.1), and significantly less likely than men to experience any of the other specific trauma types considered in our analysis (OR 0.4–0.8). Currently married respondents have significantly reduced odds of the vast majority of trauma types (OR 0.5–0.9) compared to the never married, while low education is associated with somewhat elevated risk of some (e.g. violence, accidents, natural disasters) but not all (e.g. unexpected death of a loved one) types of trauma.

Perhaps the most interesting socio-demographic correlate of trauma exposure is age. Age-of-occurrence curves in show that traumas associated with interpersonal violence have earliest median age-of-occurrence (age 17) followed by intimate partner sexual violence (age 18), war-related traumas (age 20), and traumas that happened to other people (age 20). Accidents, unexpected death of loved ones, and other traumas have later median ages-of-occurrence (ages 24–31).

3.3. Differential risk of PTSD depending on trauma type

As detailed in another recent WMH report (Liu et al., Citation2017), conditional risk of DSM-IV/CIDI PTSD after trauma exposure is 4.0%, but varies significantly by trauma type. () The highest conditional risk is associated with being raped (19.0%), physical abuse by a romantic partner (11.7%), being kidnapped (11.0%), and being sexually assaulted other than rape (10.5%). In terms of broader categories, the traumas associated with the highest PTSD risk are those involving intimate partner sexual violence (11.4%) and other traumas (9.2%), with aggregate conditional risk much lower in the other broad trauma categories (2.0–5.4%).

Table 3. Conditional risk of DSM-IV/CIDI PTSD by trauma category in the WMH Surveys.

Prevalence of trauma exposure and conditional risk of PTSD both need to be considered in evaluating the trauma-specific population burden of PTSD. Given that 3.2 trauma exposures occur per capita in the population, the 4.0% aggregate conditional risk of PTSD translates into 12.9 lifetime episodes of PTSD per 100 people in the population. The trauma type associated with by far the highest number of these PTSD cases is unexpected death of a loved one (2.9 episodes of PTSD/100 population; 22.2% of all lifetime episodes of PTSD), with rape (1.1 episodes of PTSD/100 population; 8.6% of all lifetime episodes) and sexual assault other than rape (1.2 episodes of PTSD/100 population; 9.5% of all lifetime episodes) together accounting for another 18.1% of lifetime episodes. Unexpected death is a very common type of trauma (53.2 lifetime occurrences/100 population; 16.5% of all lifetime traumas) with a high-average conditional risk of PTSD (5.4%), whereas rape and other sexual assault are less common (5.8–11.7 lifetime occurrences/100 population; 1.8–3.6% of all lifetime traumas) with much higher conditional risks of PTSD (19.0–10.5%).

Four of the six trauma types associated with highest population proportions of lifetime PTSD episodes are in the category of intimate partner sexual violence. These include 4.1% of all lifetime PTSD episodes associated with physical abuse by a romantic partner, 8.6% with rape, 9.5% with other sexual assault, and 5.6% with being stalked, for a total of 27.8% of all lifetime episodes of PTSD. Intimate partner sexual traumas account for 9.8% of all lifetime trauma exposures and are associated with comparatively high conditional risk of PTSD. The only other trauma types accounting for as many cases of PTSD are the two most commonly-occurring traumas considered here: unexpected death of a loved one (22.2% of all cases of PTSD) which, as noted above, is the second most common trauma (16.5% of all traumas) associated with high-average conditional risk of PTSD, and direct exposure to death or serious injury (5.5% of all cases of PTSD), which is the most common trauma (16.8% of all traumas) and is associated with a comparatively low risk of PTSD (1.3%).

3.4. Socio-demographic predictors of PTSD conditional on trauma exposure

Controlling for trauma type, conditional PTSD risk is significantly associated with age, with risk highest during childhood–adolescence and ages 65+. Consistent with much previous research (reviewed by Olff, Langeland, Draijer, & Gersons, Citation2007; Tolin & Foa, Citation2006), women are significantly more likely to develop PTSD than are men exposed to the same traumas. We also looked at socio-economic status and marital status but found that they are not significant predictors of PTSD after controlling trauma type and respondent age–sex.

3.5. Associations of prior trauma exposure with subsequent PTSD

The literature suggests that people with a history of prior trauma exposure are more likely than others to develop PTSD after exposure to subsequent traumas (Breslau, Peterson, & Shultz, Citation2008; Caramanica et al., Citation2015). Consistent with this evidence, a previous WMH report found that the vast majority of prior trauma types are significantly and positively associated with subsequent trauma exposure (Benjet et al., Citation2016). The strongest of these associations (OR = 2.0–2.5) are for one type of physical violence (e.g. physical abuse in childhood) predicting other types of subsequent physical violence (e.g. being mugged) and intimate partner sexual violence.

As detailed in a recent WMH report (Liu et al., Citation2017), the WMH random trauma analysis replicated earlier studies in showing that history of prior trauma exposure predicts increased vulnerability to PTSD after subsequent traumas, but also went beyond previous studies in several important ways. First, this association was found to be limited to prior traumas involving physical or sexual violence (OR = 1.3–2.5). Second, the vulnerability to future PTSD associated with these prior traumas was found to be ‘generalized’ in the sense that it existed across the full range of random trauma types considered in the analysis. Third, there was evidence for two more specific types of vulnerability associated with prior lifetime exposure to the same trauma types as in the random traumas. One involved history of traumas involving physical violence, which was associated with significantly elevated odds of PTSD after subsequent re-exposure to the same trauma types (OR 3.2). This means that it is recurrent physical violence that is most strongly associated with high PTSD risk. The other involved history of traumas involving participation in sectarian violence (e.g. combat experience, purposefully injured or killed someone), which was associated with significantly reduced odds of PTSD after subsequent re-exposure to the same trauma types (OR 0.3). The last of these results might seem counter-intuitive given that military personnel and first responders working in situations of high trauma exposure are known to be at elevated risk of PTSD (Gates et al., Citation2012; Wilson, Citation2015). However, it is important to recognize that the result refers to PTSD after re-exposure, which is quite a different thing than PTSD after initial exposure. As reviewed below in the discussion section, this finding of prior experience helping to protect against the effects of sectarian violence is consistent with previous literature.

3.6. Persistence of PTSD symptoms

The results reported up to now focused on lifetime prevalence. However, population burden is more directly a function of point prevalence. And point prevalence is a joint function of lifetime prevalence and persistence. A recent comprehensive review of the literature on PTSD remission concluded that roughly half of PTSD cases remit within six months and that probability of remission does not vary dramatically across trauma types (Morina, Wicherts, Lobbrecht, & Priebe, Citation2014). We were able to investigate this issue in the WMH data by asking respondents with a history of PTSD associated with randomly-selected traumas to report on duration of symptoms and whether they still had symptoms at the time of interview. Mean duration of PTSD symptoms () averages approximately six years (72.3 months) across all traumas but varies greatly depending on trauma type from a high of over 13 years for traumas involving combat experience in war to a low of about one year for traumas involving exposure to a natural disaster. It is noteworthy that WMH respondents were asked how long they continued to have any symptoms, so these duration estimates are for symptoms rather than for meeting full PTSD criteria. Speed-of-recovery curves () show that means in are influenced by long right tails, with 25–40% of cases of PTSD recovering within one year, many of them within six months, the major exception being a much lower rate of rapid recovery among people with war-related PTSD. A smaller proportion of cases persists for many years. The longest median duration is five years for PTSD symptoms associated with war-related traumas followed by three years for traumas involving physical or intimate partner sexual violence. Median durations are one to two years, in comparison, for PTSD symptoms due to the other broad trauma categories.

Table 4. Mean duration and years in episode of DSM-IV/CIDI PTSD by trauma type in the WMH Surveys.

Figure 1. Age-of-onset distributions of trauma exposure in the WMH Surveys.

Figure 1. Age-of-onset distributions of trauma exposure in the WMH Surveys.

Figure 2. Speed of recovery of DSM-IV/CIDI PTSD by trauma category in the WMH Surveys.1

1‘Recovery’ was defined as length of time until all symptoms remitted.

Figure 2. Speed of recovery of DSM-IV/CIDI PTSD by trauma category in the WMH Surveys.11‘Recovery’ was defined as length of time until all symptoms remitted.

We also looked for socio-demographic predictors of PTSD symptom duration. None were significant in the total sample of cases. However, we lacked the statistical power to investigate the possibility that these predictors vary depending on trauma type. Such variation has been documented in focused studies. For example, a large prospective study of victims of Hurricane Katrina found that socio-economic status was a significant predictor of speed of PTSD recovery after that natural disaster (McLaughlin et al., Citation2011). Previous research on the predictors of recovery in trauma-specific samples have focused largely on trauma characteristics and prior psychopathology (Atwoli et al., Citation2017; Bromet et al., Citation2017; Stein et al., Citation2016), neither of which was considered in the aggregate WMH analyses due to the small numbers of cases associated with each trauma type.

Our most direct estimate of the population burden of PTSD associated with each trauma type is the number of years of PTSD at the population level associated with that trauma type. An estimate of the latter can be obtained by multiplying number of lifetime PTSD episodes/100 population by mean duration. When this is done and the trauma-specific products are summed across all trauma types we estimate that there are 77.7 lifetime person-years of PTSD in the population per 100 respondents. The four trauma types with the highest proportions of these person-years are rape (13.1%; 10.2 person-years per 100 respondents), other sexual assault (15.1%; 11.7 person-years per 100 respondents), being stalked (9.8%; 7.6 person-years per 100 respondents), and unexpected death of a loved one (11.6%; 9.0 person-years per 100 respondents). The broad category of intimate partner sexual violence accounts for nearly 42.7% of all person-years with PTSD in the population (33.2 person-years per 100 respondents).

4. Discussion

The WMH results are limited in a number of ways. For one, lifetime prevalence estimates of trauma exposure are likely to be conservative due to recall error (Belli, Citation2014). In addition, some traumas are likely to be systematically under-reported because they are embarrassing or otherwise culturally sensitive (Schaeffer, Citation2000). Both types of problems can be reduced, although not entirely overcome, with data collection enhancements. Recall failure can be reduced by using memory priming strategies and event history calendars to focus memory search (Drasch & Matthes, Citation2013). Conscious nondisclosure can be reduced by increasing anonymity; for example, by having respondents privately record sensitive information in a self-report booklet that is sealed before returning it to the interviewer or via private computerized self-administration (Gnambs & Kaspar, Citation2015). There is some concern that complete anonymity can reduce motivation to report accurately (Lelkes, Krosnick, Marx, Judd, & Park, Citation2012). These strategies were not used in the WMH Surveys, so we have to consider the WMH trauma exposure prevalence estimates as lower-bound estimates.

Another limitation of the WMH results involves the diagnoses of lifetime PTSD. These diagnoses are limited by being based on retrospective reports obtained in a cross-sectional survey using a fully-structured lay-administered diagnostic interview rather than a semi-structured clinician-administered diagnostic interview. The WMH clinical reappraisal study shows that PTSD prevalence is under-estimated in the CIDI compared to blinded semi-structured clinical interviews but that the vast majority of CIDI cases are confirmed in these clinical reappraisal interviews (Haro et al., Citation2006), suggesting that the WMH prevalence estimates are conservative. WMH results regarding PTSD persistence, in comparison, are anti-conservative because they assess persistence of any symptom rather than of the full PTSD syndrome. Results regarding predictors, finally, are limited by excluding prior psychopathology, which is known to be the strongest predictor of PTSD onset given trauma type (DiGangi et al., Citation2013; Sayed et al., Citation2015), and trauma characteristics-sequelae, which are known to be strong predictors of persistence (Morina et al., Citation2014).

Within the context of these limitations, our finding that 70.4% of respondents were exposed to one or more traumas at some time in their life is broadly consistent with previous research reviewed elsewhere (Benjet et al., Citation2016) documenting that the majority of people in the general population have experienced traumas. However, WMH went beyond previous studies in assessing frequency of exposure, documenting that trauma exposure is even more common than previously known, with a per capita mean of 2.0 trauma types and 4.6 trauma exposures. These estimates are conservative as they are based on calculations in which some ongoing traumas, such as physical abuse at the hands of a caregiver during childhood, are counted as only ‘one occurrence’ even though they often persisted over many years.

WMH results regarding the most common types of trauma are consistent with previous research in finding that unexpected death of a loved one and motor vehicle accidents are the two most common types of trauma in the general population (reviewed by Benjet et al., Citation2016). We went beyond these previous results to show that traumas occurring to other people account for over 40% of all reported qualifying (for a diagnosis of PTSD) traumas (16.5% involving unexpected death of a loved one and an additional 25.0% other traumas that either occurred to a loved one or were witnessed), that accidents are the most common type of trauma occurring to people directly (24.6%), and that traumas involving intimate partner sexual violence (9.8%) and physical violence (13.8%) account for the bulk of other traumas. It is noteworthy that the objective occurrence of traumas to loved ones is clearly under-reported by WMH respondents in that we would expect each loved one of each respondent to have as many traumas as the respondent himself or herself, but this is not the case in respondent reports, implying that respondent reports about traumas occurring to loved ones are limited to the people and traumas most psychologically salient to respondents.

We also found that trauma exposure is not distributed randomly in the population. Our results are consistent in this regard with a previous study that reviewed the literature on basic socio-demographic correlates of trauma exposure (Hatch & Dohrenwend, Citation2007) in finding that women are significantly more likely than men to experience intimate partner sexual violence and men more likely than women to experience physical violence and accidents. We found that traumas involving violence and accidents are more likely to occur in adolescence and early adulthood that other parts of the life course. We also found that being married is the most consistent socio-demographic factor associated with reduced risk of many types of trauma exposure, while traumas involving violence and accidents (including natural disasters) are inversely associated with socio-economic status. And we found that trauma exposures are correlated over time, with people exposed to earlier traumas at significantly increased risk of subsequent traumas. The latter pattern presumably reflects individual differences in predispositions, coping resources, life circumstances, and lifestyles that influence risk of trauma exposure. The WMH data were too coarse to search for modifiable risk factors that might be targeted to prevent future trauma exposure, but the strong inter-temporal patterning of exposure suggests that such an investigation might make sense. Preventive interventions with this focus already exist for recurrences of drunk driving (Miller, Curtis, Sønderlund, Day, & Droste, Citation2015), intimate partner violence (Ramsay et al., Citation2009), and sexual violence (Marques, Wiederanders, Day, Nelson, & Ommeren, Citation2005), but our results raise the possibility of also developing risk models to target broader types of secondary preventive interventions.

Our estimates of conditional PTSD risk among people exposed to traumas are for the most part lower than in previous studies due to our focus on representative samples of traumas in comparison to the worst traumas examined in most other community epidemiological studies and in samples that over-represent help-seekers focused on particular trauma types (e.g. Campbell, Dworkin, & Cabral, Citation2009; Goldmann & Galea, Citation2014).

Our finding that conditional PTSD risk is elevated after traumas involving violence is broadly consistent with previous research (see reviews in Atwoli et al., Citation2015; Ozer, Best, Lipsey, & Weiss, Citation2003). We also found that prior exposure to some traumas involving violence was associated with generalized vulnerability to subsequent PTSD. Although ongoing research is investigating pathways leading to such generalized vulnerability (Daskalakis, Bagot, Parker, Vinkers, & de Kloet, Citation2013; Levy-Gigi, Richter-Levin, Okon-Singer, Kéri, & Bonanno, Citation2016; Rutter, Citation2012), we know of no work on differential effects of trauma types in this regard. However, suggestive related evidence exists on differences in associations of childhood adversities with adult mental disorders across different childhood adversity types (Kessler et al., Citation2010; Pirkola et al., Citation2005) and profiles (McLafferty et al., Citation2015; Putnam, Harris, & Putnam, Citation2013).

Our finding that prior same-type physical violence victimizations predict elevated PTSD risk after re-victimization means that these types of victimization are especially impactful when they are recurrent. That being the case, a question can be raised about our failure to find a similar pattern for intimate partner sexual violence, as the latter seems to contradict studies showing that sexual assault re-victimization is associated with poor mental health (Classen, Palesh, & Aggarwal, Citation2005; Das & Otis, Citation2016; Miner, Flitter, & Robinson, Citation2006). However, these studies focused largely on victims of childhood sexual assault who were versus were not re-victimized as adults, whereas our analysis compares adult sexual assault victims who were versus were not previously victimized.

Our finding that prior same-type participation in sectarian violence is associated with low PTSD risk after subsequent re-exposure to the same trauma is consistent with research showing low PTSD prevalence among policemen (Levy-Gigi et al., Citation2016) and other first responders (Levy-Gigi & Richter-Levin, Citation2014) and among Israeli settlers exposed to repeated bombings (Palgi, Gelkopf, & Berger, Citation2015; Somer et al., Citation2009). These results could be due either to selection and/or to prior exposures promoting resilience (Wilson et al., Citation2009). Both experimental animal studies (Liu, Citation2015) and observational human studies (Rutter, Citation2012) support the resilience possibility, although research showing that intervening psychopathology due to prior traumas mediates the association between trauma history and subsequent PTSD (Sayed et al., Citation2015) confirms that prior traumas are more likely to create vulnerability than resilience. Research on the ‘healthy warrior effect’ supports the selection possibility (Larson, Highfill-McRoy, & Booth-Kewley, Citation2008; Wilson et al., Citation2009). As a result, we suspect that both processes are at work (i.e. both selection and environmental causation), although we have no way to estimate their relative importance with the WMH data.

Our results regarding persistence are broadly consistent with previous studies in showing that a substantial minority of PTSD cases remits within months after onset. However, we found that median duration of symptoms was longer than the six months found in a recent review of the literature (Morina et al., Citation2014). This reflects upward bias in the WMH data due to the very narrow definition of remission used in WMH, which required the respondent no longer to have any PTSD symptoms, leading to artificially high estimates of duration. This means that duration of sub-threshold PTSD is considerably longer than generally appreciated. Given that sub-threshold PTSD has been shown to be associated with considerable distress, impairment, and comorbidity (McLaughlin et al., Citation2015), the latter possibility is worthy of future investigation in prospective studies.

5. Conclusions

The WMH data document clearly that trauma exposure is common throughout the world, that this exposure is unequally distributed in the population, and that PTSD risk differs substantially across trauma types due to traumas involving interpersonal violence (especially relationship–sexual violence) carrying the highest PTSD risk. There is also high population-level burden of PTSD associated with unexpected death of a loved one, a very common trauma type that is associated with low individual-level PTSD risk. Although a substantial minority of PTSD cases remits within months after onset, mean symptom duration is considerably longer than previously recognized.

The WMH Surveys were designed as needs assessment surveys to help governments gain insights into the population burden of mental disorders. Because of this, the most important implications of the results are for policy planners in recognizing that PTSD is a very commonly-occurring condition. Although we did not present any results about severity of illness, other WMH results document clearly that PTSD is a seriously impairing disorder (Kessler et al., Citation2009). PTSD causes substantial loss of human capital from a societal perspective both in the form of days out of role (Alonso et al., Citation2011) and in the form of decreased productivity on days in role (Ormel et al., Citation2008). Roughly half of people with PTSD in high income countries and about half that number in low or middle income countries seek some type of treatment (Koenen et al., Citation2017), but the type and duration of treatment seldom meet even minimal standards for treatment adequacy (Wang et al., Citation2007). These results suggest that outreach efforts are needed to increase the proportion of people with PTSD who obtain treatment and that treatment quality improvement efforts are needed for patients in treatment.

Highlights of article

  • Lifetime trauma exposure is the norm in most countries.

  • Interpersonal violence traumas carry highest PTSD risk.

  • Lifetime population burden of PTSD is 77.7 person-years/100 respondents across surveys.

  • Trauma types with highest PTSD burden include those involving intimate partner sexual violence (relatively uncommon traumas associated with high PTSD risk) and unexpected death of a loved one (a very common trauma associated with low PTSD risk).

  • Although many cases remit within months, PTSD symptoms typically are quite persistent.

Group Information

The WHO World Mental Health Survey collaborators are Sergio Aguilar-Gaxiola, MD, PhD, Ali Al-Hamzawi, MD, Mohammed Salih Al-Kaisy, MD, Jordi Alonso, MD, PhD, Laura Helena Andrade, MD, PhD, Corina Benjet, PhD, Guilherme Borges, ScD, Evelyn J. Bromet, PhD, Ronny Bruffaerts, PhD, Brendan Bunting, PhD, Jose Miguel Caldas de Almeida, MD, PhD, Graça Cardoso, MD, PhD, Somnath Chatterji, MD, Alfredo H. Cia, MD, Louisa Degenhardt, PhD, Koen Demyttenaere, MD, PhD, John Fayyad, MD, Silvia Florescu, MD, PhD, Giovanni de Girolamo, MD, Oye Gureje, MD, DSc, FRCPsych, Josep Maria Haro, MD, PhD, Yanling He, MD, Hristo Hinkov, MD, PhD, Chi-yi Hu, MD, PhD, Yueqin Huang, MD, MPH, PhD, Peter de Jonge, PhD, Aimee Nasser Karam, PhD, Elie G. Karam, MD, Norito Kawakami, MD, DMSc, Ronald C. Kessler, PhD, Andrzej Kiejna, MD, PhD, Viviane Kovess-Masfety, MD, PhD, Sing Lee, MB, BS, Jean-Pierre Lepine, MD, Daphna Levinson, PhD, John McGrath, MD, PhD, Maria Elena Medina-Mora, PhD, Jacek Moskalewicz, PhD, Fernando Navarro-Mateu, MD, PhD, Beth-Ellen Pennell, MA, Marina Piazza, MPH, ScD, Jose Posada-Villa, MD, Kate M. Scott, PhD, Tim Slade, PhD, Juan Carlos Stagnaro, MD, PhD, Dan J. Stein, FRCPC, PhD, Margreet ten Have, PhD, Yolanda Torres, MPH, Dra.HC, Maria Carmen Viana, MD, PhD, Harvey Whiteford, MBBS, PhD, David R. Williams, MPH, PhD, Bogdan Wojtyniak, ScD.

Supplemental material

Spanish abstract

Download PDF (7.9 KB)

Acknowledgments

We thank the staff of the WMH Data Collection and Data Analysis Coordination Centres for assistance with instrumentation, fieldwork, and consultation on data analysis. None of the funders had any role in the design, analysis, interpretation of results, or preparation of this paper. The views and opinions expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views or policies of the World Health Organization, other sponsoring organizations, agencies, or governments.

Disclosure statement

In the past three years, Dr Haro received personal fees from Lundbeck. In the past three years, Dr Kawakami served as a consultant for Junpukai Foundation, SB At Work, Sekisui Co., Ltd., and received grant funding from Infosoft Technologies, Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare, and Japan Society for Promotion of Science. In the past three years, Dr Kessler received support for his epidemiological studies from Sanofi Aventis; was a consultant for Johnson & Johnson Wellness and Prevention, Shire, Takeda; and served on an advisory board for the Johnson & Johnson Services Inc. Lake Nona Life Project. Kessler is a co-owner of DataStat, Inc., a market research firm that carries out healthcare research. In the past three years, Dr Stein has received research grants and/or consultancy honoraria from Biocodex, Lundbeck, Servier, and Sun.

Additional information

Funding

The World Health Organization World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; R01 MH070884 and R01 MH093612-01), the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the Pfizer Foundation, the US Public Health Service (R13-MH066849, R01-MH069864, and R01 DA016558), the Fogarty International Center (FIRCA R03-TW006481), the Pan American Health Organization, Eli Lilly and Company, Ortho-McNeil Pharmaceutical, Inc., GlaxoSmithKline, and Bristol-Myers Squibb. A complete list of all within-country and cross-national WMH publications can be found at http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmh/. The 2007 Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing was funded by the Australian Government Department of Health and Ageing. The São Paulo Megacity Mental Health Survey is supported by the State of São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP) Thematic Project Grant 03/00204-3. The Bulgarian Epidemiological Study of common mental disorders EPIBUL is supported by the Ministry of Health and the National Center for Public Health Protection. The Chinese World Mental Health Survey Initiative is supported by the Pfizer Foundation. The Colombian National Study of Mental Health (NSMH) is supported by the Ministry of Social Protection. The Mental Health Study Medellín – Colombia was carried out and supported jointly by the Center for Excellence on Research in Mental Health (CES University) and the Secretary of Health of Medellín. The ESEMeD project is funded by the European Commission (Contracts QLG5-1999-01042; SANCO 2004123, and EAHC 20081308), the Piedmont Region (Italy), Fondo de Investigación Sanitaria, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spain (FIS 00/0028), Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología, Spain (SAF 2000-158-CE), Departament de Salut, Generalitat de Catalunya, Spain, Instituto de Salud Carlos III (CIBER CB06/02/0046, RETICS RD06/0011 REM-TAP), and other local agencies and by an unrestricted educational grant from GlaxoSmithKline. The Israel National Health Survey is funded by the Ministry of Health with support from the Israel National Institute for Health Policy and Health Services Research and the National Insurance Institute of Israel. The World Mental Health Japan (WMHJ) Survey is supported by the Grant for Research on Psychiatric and Neurological Diseases and Mental Health (H13-SHOGAI-023, H14-TOKUBETSU-026, H16-KOKORO-013) from the Japan Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare. The Lebanese Evaluation of the Burden of Ailments and Needs Of the Nation (L.E.B.A.N.O.N.) is supported by the Lebanese Ministry of Public Health, the WHO (Lebanon), National Institute of Health/Fogarty International Center (R03 TW006481-01), anonymous private donations to IDRAAC, Lebanon, and unrestricted grants from Algorithm, AstraZeneca, Benta, Bella Pharma, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Lundbeck, Novartis, Servier, OmniPharma, Phenicia, Pfizer, UPO. The Mexican National Comorbidity Survey (MNCS) is supported by The National Institute of Psychiatry Ramon de la Fuente (INPRFMDIES 4280) and by the National Council on Science and Technology (CONACyT-G30544- H), with supplemental support from the PanAmerican Health Organization (PAHO). Te Rau Hinengaro: The New Zealand Mental Health Survey (NZMHS) is supported by the New Zealand Ministry of Health, Alcohol Advisory Council, and the Health Research Council. The Northern Ireland Study of Mental Health was funded by the Health & Social Care Research & Development Division of the Public Health Agency. The Peruvian World Mental Health Study was funded by the National Institute of Health of the Ministry of Health of Peru. The Portuguese Mental Health Study was carried out by the Department of Mental Health, Faculty of Medical Sciences, NOVA University of Lisbon, with collaboration of the Portuguese Catholic University, and was funded by Champalimaud Foundation, Gulbenkian Foundation, Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT) and Ministry of Health. The Romania WMH study projects ‘Policies in Mental Health Area’ and ‘National Study regarding Mental Health and Services Use’ were carried out by the National School of Public Health & Health Services Management (former National Institute for Research & Development in Health), with technical support of Metro Media Transilvania, the National Institute of Statistics-National Centre for Training in Statistics, S.C. Cheyenne Services SRL, Statistics Netherlands and were funded by the Ministry of Public Health (former Ministry of Health) with supplemental support of Eli Lilly Romania SRL. The South Africa Stress and Health Study (SASH) is supported by the US National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH059575) and National Institute of Drug Abuse with supplemental funding from the South African Department of Health and the University of Michigan. The Psychiatric Enquiry to General Population in Southeast Spain – Murcia (PEGASUS-Murcia) Project has been financed by the Regional Health Authorities of Murcia (Servicio Murciano de Salud and Consejería de Sanidad y Política Social) and Fundación para la Formación e Investigación Sanitarias (FFIS) of Murcia. The Ukraine Comorbid Mental Disorders during Periods of Social Disruption (CMDPSD) study is funded by the US National Institute of Mental Health (RO1-MH61905). The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH; U01-MH60220) with supplemental support from the National Institute of Drug Abuse (NIDA), the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (RWJF; Grant 044708), and the John W. Alden Trust. Dr Dan Stein is supported by the Medical Research Council of South Africa (MRC). Corina Benjet has received funding from the (Mexican) National Council of Science and Technology (grant CB-2010-01-155221).

Notes on contributors

Ronald C. Kessler

All authors have read and approved the final manuscript. Study design: Kessler. Data collection: All authors. Data interpretation: All authors. Manuscript writing: Kessler. Data analysis: Kessler, Petukhova. Manuscript editing: All authors.

References

  • Alonso, J., Petukhova, M., Vilagut, G., Chatterji, S., Heeringa, S., Ustun, T. B., … Kessler, R. C. (2011). Days out of role due to common physical and mental conditions: Results from the WHO World Mental Health surveys. Molecular Psychiatry, 16(12), 1–16. doi:10.1038/mp.2010.101
  • Atwoli, L., Stein, D. J., King, A., Petukhova, M., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., … Kessler, R. C. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder associated with unexpected death of a loved one: Cross-national findings from the World Mental Health Surveys. Depression and Anxiety, 34(4), 315–326. doi:10.1002/da.22579
  • Atwoli, L., Stein, D. J., Koenen, K. C., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2015). Epidemiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: Prevalence, correlates and consequences. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(4), 307–311. doi:10.1097/yco.0000000000000167
  • Belli, R. F. (2014). Autobiographical memory dynamics in survey research. In T. J. Perfect & D. S. Lindsay (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of applied memory (pp. 366–384). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
  • Benjet, C., Bromet, E., Karam, E. G., Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Ruscio, A. M., … Koenen, K. C. (2016). The epidemiology of traumatic event exposure worldwide: Results from the World Mental Health Survey Consortium. Psychological Medicine, 46(2), 327–343. doi:10.1017/s0033291715001981
  • Breslau, N., Peterson, E. L., & Schultz, L. R. (2008). A second look at prior trauma and the posttraumatic stress disorder effects of subsequent trauma: A prospective epidemiological study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(4), 431–437. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.4.431
  • Bromet, E. J., Atwoli, L., Kawakami, N., Navarro-Mateu, F., Piotrowski, P., King, A. J., … Kessler, R. C. (2017). Post-traumatic stress disorder associated with natural and human-made disasters in the World Mental Health Surveys. Psychological Medicine, 47(2), 227–241. doi:10.1017/s0033291716002026
  • Campbell, R., Dworkin, E., & Cabral, G. (2009). An ecological model of the impact of sexual assault on women’s mental health. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 10(3), 225–246. doi:10.1177/1524838009334456
  • Caramanica, K., Brackbill, R. M., Stellman, S. D., & Farfel, M. R. (2015). Posttraumatic stress disorder after Hurricane Sandy among persons exposed to the 9/11 disaster. International Journal of Emergency Mental Health, 17(1), 356–362. Retrieved from https://www.omicsonline.com/open-access/posttraumatic-stress-disorder-after-hurricane-sandy-among-persons-exposed-to-the-disaster-1522-4821-17-173.pdf
  • Classen, C. C., Palesh, O. G., & Aggarwal, R. (2005). Sexual revictimization: A review of the empirical literature. Trauma, Violence & Abuse, 6(2), 103–129. doi:10.1177/1524838005275087
  • Das, A., & Otis, N. (2016). Sexual contact in childhood, revictimization, and lifetime sexual and psychological outcomes. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45(5), 1117–1131. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0620-3
  • Daskalakis, N. P., Bagot, R. C., Parker, K. J., Vinkers, C. H., & de Kloet, E. R. (2013). The three-hit concept of vulnerability and resilience: Toward understanding adaptation to early-life adversity outcome. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 1858–1873. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.008
  • DiGangi, J. A., Gomez, D., Mendoza, L., Jason, L. A., Keys, C. B., & Koenen, K. C. (2013). Pretrauma risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder: A systematic review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(6), 728–744. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.05.002
  • Drasch, K., & Matthes, B. (2013). Improving retrospective life course data by combining modularized self-reports and event history calendars: Experiences from a large scale survey. Quality & Quantity, 47(2), 817–838. doi:10.1007/s11135-011-9568-0
  • Fossion, P., Leys, C., Kempenaers, C., Braun, S., Verbanck, P., & Linkowski, P. (2015). Beware of multiple traumas in PTSD assessment: The role of reactivation mechanism in intrusive and hyper-arousal symptoms. Aging & Mental Health, 19(3), 258–263. doi:10.1080/13607863.2014.924901
  • Gardner, M., & Altman, D. (2000). Statistics with confidence: Confidence intervals and statistical guidelines. London: BMJ Books.
  • Gates, M. A., Holowka, D. W., Vasterling, J. J., Keane, T. M., Marx, B. P., & Rosen, R. C. (2012). Posttraumatic stress disorder in veterans and military personnel: Epidemiology, screening, and case recognition. Psychological Services, 9(4), 361–382. doi:10.1037/a0027649
  • Gnambs, T., & Kaspar, K. (2015). Disclosure of sensitive behaviors across self-administered survey modes: A meta-analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1237–1259. doi:10.3758/s13428-014-0533-4
  • Goldmann, E., & Galea, S. (2014). Mental health consequences of disasters. Annual Review of Public Health, 35, 169–183. doi:10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032013-182435
  • Haro, J. M., Arbabzadeh-Bouchez, S., Brugha, T. S., De Girolamo, G., Guyer, M. E., Jin, R., … Kessler, R. C. (2006). Concordance of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview version 3.0 (CIDI 3.0) with standardized clinical assessments in the WHO World Mental Health surveys. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 15(4), 167–180. doi:10.1002/mpr.196
  • Hatch, S. L., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (2007). Distribution of traumatic and other stressful life events by race/ethnicity, gender, SES and age: A review of the research. American Journal of Community Psychology, 40(3–4), 313–332. doi:10.1007/s10464-007-9134-z
  • Heeringa, S. G., Wells, J. E., Hubbard, F., Mneimneh, Z. N., Chiu, W. T., Sampson, N. A., & Berglund, P. A. (2008). Sample designs and sampling procedures. In R. C. Kessler & T. B. Üstün (Eds.), The WHO World Mental Health Surveys: Global perspectives on the epidemiology of mental disorders (pp. 14–32). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Chatterji, S., Lee, S., Ormel, J., … Wang, P. S. (2009). The global burden of mental disorders: An update from the WHO World Mental Health (WMH) surveys. Epidemiologia E Psichiatria Sociale, 18(1), 23–33. doi:10.1017/S1121189X00001421
  • Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., … Williams, D. R. (2010). Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(5), 378–385. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499
  • Kessler, R. C., & Ustun, T. B. (Eds.). (2008). The WHO World Mental Health Surveys: Global perspectives on the epidemiology of mental disorders. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Kessler, R. C., & Ustün, T. B. (2004). The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13(2), 93–121. doi:10.1002/mpr.168
  • Koenen, K. C., Ratanatharathorn, A., Ng, L., McLaughlin, K. A., Bromet, E. J., Stein, D. J., … Kessler, R. C. (2017). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the World. Mental Health Surveys Psychological Medicine, 1–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291717000708
  • Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33(1), 159–174. doi:10.2307/2529310
  • Larson, G. E., Highfill-McRoy, R. M., & Booth-Kewley, S. (2008). Psychiatric diagnoses in historic and contemporary military cohorts: Combat deployment and the healthy warrior effect. American Journal of Epidemiology, 167(11), 1269–1276. doi:10.1093/aje/kwn084
  • Lelkes, Y., Krosnick, J. A., Marx, D. M., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (2012). Complete anonymity compromises the accuracy of self-reports. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1291–1299. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.07.002
  • Levy-Gigi, E., & Richter-Levin, G. (2014). The hidden price of repeated traumatic exposure. Stress, 17(4), 343–351. doi:10.3109/10253890.2014.923397
  • Levy-Gigi, E., Richter-Levin, G., Okon-Singer, H., Kéri, S., & Bonanno, G. A. (2016). The hidden price and possible benefit of repeated traumatic exposure. Stress, 19(1), 1–7. doi:10.3109/10253890.2015.1113523
  • Liberzon, I., & Abelson, J. L. (2016). Context processing and the neurobiology of post-traumatic stress disorder. Neuron, 92(1), 14–30. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.09.039
  • Liu, H., Petukhova, M. V., Sampson, N. A., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Andrade, L. H., … Kessler, R. C. (2017). Association of DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder with traumatic experience type and history in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAMA Psychiatry, 74(3), 270–281. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3783
  • Liu, R. T. (2015). A developmentally informed perspective on the relation between stress and psychopathology: When the problem with stress is that there is not enough. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(1), 80–92. doi:10.1037/abn0000043
  • Lowe, S. R., Walsh, K., Uddin, M., Galea, S., & Koenen, K. C. (2014). Bidirectional relationships between trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress: A longitudinal study of Detroit residents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(3), 533–544. doi:10.1037/a0037046
  • Marques, J. K., Wiederanders, M., Day, D. M., Nelson, C., & Ommeren, A. (2005). Effects of a relapse prevention program on sexual recidivism: Final results from California’s Sex Offender Treatment and Evaluation Project (SOTEP). Sexual Abuse, 17(1), 79–107. doi:10.1177/107906320501700108
  • McLafferty, M., Armour, C., McKenna, A., O’Neill, S., Murphy, S., & Bunting, B. (2015). Childhood adversity profiles and adult psychopathology in a representative Northern Ireland study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 35, 42–48. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.07.004
  • McLaughlin, K. A., Berglund, P., Gruber, M. J., Kessler, R. C., Sampson, N. A., & Zaslavsky, A. M. (2011). Recovery from PTSD following Hurricane Katrina. Depression and Anxiety, 28(6), 439–446. doi:10.1002/da.20790
  • McLaughlin, K. A., Koenen, K. C., Friedman, M. J., Ruscio, A. M., Karam, E. G., Shahly, V., … Kessler, R. C. (2015). Subthreshold posttraumatic stress disorder in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Biological Psychiatry, 77(4), 375–384. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.028
  • Miller, P. G., Curtis, A., Sønderlund, A., Day, A., & Droste, N. (2015). Effectiveness of interventions for convicted DUI offenders in reducing recidivism: A systematic review of the peer-reviewed scientific literature. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 41(1), 16–29. doi:10.3109/00952990.2014.966199
  • Miner, M. H., Flitter, J. M., & Robinson, B. B. (2006). Association of sexual revictimization with sexuality and psychological function. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21(4), 503–524. doi:10.1177/0886260505285913
  • Morina, N., Wicherts, J. M., Lobbrecht, J., & Priebe, S. (2014). Remission from post-traumatic stress disorder in adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of long term outcome studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(3), 249–255. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2014.03.002
  • Olff, M., Langeland, W., Draijer, N., & Gersons, B. P. (2007). Gender differences in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133(2), 183–204. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.183
  • Ormel, J., Petukhova, M., Chatterji, S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M. C., … Kessler, R. C. (2008). Disability and treatment of specific mental and physical disorders across the world. British Journal of Psychiatry, 192(5), 368–375. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.107.039107
  • Ozer, E. J., Best, S. R., Lipsey, T. L., & Weiss, D. S. (2003). Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 52–73. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.52
  • Palgi, Y., Gelkopf, M., & Berger, R. (2015). The inoculating role of previous exposure to potentially traumatic life events on coping with prolonged exposure to rocket attacks: A lifespan perspective. Psychiatry Research, 227(2–3), 296–301. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.03.020
  • Pirkola, S., Isometsä, E., Aro, H., Kestilä, L., Hämäläinen, J., Veijola, J., … Lonnqvist, J. (2005). Childhood adversities as risk factors for adult mental disorders: Results from the Health 2000 study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40(10), 769–777. doi:10.1007/s00127-005-0950-x
  • Putnam, K. T., Harris, W. W., & Putnam, F. W. (2013). Synergistic childhood adversities and complex adult psychopathology. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26(4), 435–442. doi:10.1002/jts.21833
  • Ramsay, J., Carter, Y., Davidson, L., Dunne, D., Eldridge, S., Feder, G., … Warburton, A. (2009). Advocacy interventions to reduce or eliminate violence and promote the physical and psychosocial well-being of women who experience intimate partner abuse. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (3)(Cd005043). doi:10.1002/14651858.CD005043.pub2
  • Rutter, M. (2012). Resilience as a dynamic concept. Development and Psychopathology, 24(2), 335–344. doi:10.1017/s0954579412000028
  • Sayed, S., Iacoviello, B. M., & Charney, D. S. (2015). Risk factors for the development of psychopathology following trauma. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17(8), 70. doi:10.1007/s11920-015-0612-y
  • Schaeffer, N. C. (2000). Asking questions about threatening topics: A selective overview. In A. A. Stone, J. S. Turkkan, C. A. Bachrach, J. B. Jobe, H. S. Kurtzman, & V. S. Cain (Eds.), The science of self-report: Implications for research and practice (pp. 105–122). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Smith, H. L., Summers, B. J., Dillon, K. H., & Cougle, J. R. (2016). Is worst-event trauma type related to PTSD symptom presentation and associated features? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 38, 55–61. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.01.007
  • Smoller, J. W. (2016). The genetics of stress-related disorders: PTSD, depression, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology, 41(1), 297–319. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.266
  • Somer, E., Zrihan-Weitzman, A., Fusé, T., Parker, H., Dickstein, B., Maguen, S., & Litz, B. T. (2009). Israeli civilians under heavy bombardment: Prediction of the severity of post-traumatic symptoms. Prehospital and Disaster Medicine, 24(5), 389–394. doi:10.1017/S1049023X00007196
  • Stein, D. J., de Jonge, P., Kessler, R. C., & Scott, K. M. (In press). The cross-national epidemiology of mental disorders. In K. M. Scott, P. de Jonge, D. J. Stein, & R. C. Kessler (Eds.), Mental disorders around the world: Facts and figures from the World Mental Health Surveys. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Stein, D. J., Karam, E. G., Shahly, V., Hill, E. D., King, A., Petukhova, M., … Kessler, R. C. (2016). Post-traumatic stress disorder associated with life-threatening motor vehicle collisions in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. BMC Psychiatry, 16, 257. doi:10.1186/s12888-016-0957-8
  • Tolin, D. F., & Foa, E. B. (2006). Sex differences in trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder: A quantitative review of 25 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 959–992. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.959
  • Wang, P. S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Angermeyer, M. C., Borges, G., Bromet, E. J., … Wells, J. E. (2007). Use of mental health services for anxiety, mood, and substance disorders in 17 countries in the WHO World Mental Health surveys. Lancet, 370(9590), 841–850. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(07)61414-7
  • Wilson, J., Jones, M., Fear, N. T., Hull, L., Hotopf, M., Wessely, S., & Rona, R. J. (2009). Is previous psychological health associated with the likelihood of Iraq War deployment? An investigation of the “healthy warrior effect”. American Journal of Epidemiology, 169(11), 1362–1369. doi:10.1093/aje/kwp044
  • Wilson, L. C. (2015). A systematic review of probable posttraumatic stress disorder in first responders following man-made mass violence. Psychiatry Research, 229(1–2), 21–26. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.06.015