Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published by De Gruyter Mouton May 21, 2019

Finite semiotics: Cognitive sets, semiotic vectors, and semiosic oscillation

  • Cameron Shackell ORCID logo EMAIL logo
From the journal Semiotica

Abstract

The grounding of semiotics in the finiteness of cognition is extended into constructs and methods for analysis by incorporating the assumption that cognition can be similar within and between agents. After examining and formalizing cognitive similarity as an ontological commitment, the recurrence of cognitive states is examined in terms of a “cognitive set.” In the individual, the cognitive set is seen as evolving under the bidirectional, cyclical determination of thought by the historical environment. At the population level, the distributed “global” cognitive set is argued to be constrained to a manifold in which the cognition of individuals is determined only when their cognitive sets meet certain conditions in the world: a result seen as consistent with Lotman’s semiosphere.

With these foundations in place, dimensional modelling of the semiosic field is inaugurated. Firstly, measures of cognitive similarity are formalized as cognitive “distance” and on this basis the concept of a semiotic vector is defined. Secondly, semiotic vectors are seen to shape a general pattern of oscillation in semiosis, and thus to imply zero points in semiosic potential. Thirdly, semiosic oscillation in individual agents is shown to be consistent with a novel diachronic or longitudinal interpretation of Greimas’ semiotic square expanded into a “semiotic pipe” in which cognition traverses an n-dimensional space structured by axes of oscillation. Finally, the expanded theory of finite semiotics is advanced as a useful basis for two new complementary disciplines: (1) a computational, mathematical science of “natural semiotic processing” (NSP) to trace and model semiotic vectors and oscillation; and (2) an ethical, rhetorical art of “technological influencing” (TI) to guide its inputs and applications.

References

Baudrillard, J. 1994 [1981]. Simulacra and simulation, S. F. Glaser (trans.). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.10.3998/mpub.9904Search in Google Scholar

Chandler, D. 2017. Semiotics: The basics, 3rd edn. New York: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Clark, H. H. 1970. Word associations and linguistic theory. New Horizons in Linguistics 1. 271–286.Search in Google Scholar

Eco, U., R. Rorty, J. Culler & C. Brooke-Rose. 1992. Interpretation and overinterpretation, S. Collini (ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511627408Search in Google Scholar

Foucault, M. 2002 [1969]. The archaeology of knowledge, A. Sheridan (trans.). New York: Routledge.10.4324/9780203604168Search in Google Scholar

Greimas, A. J. & F. Rastier. 1968. The interaction of semiotic constraints. Yale French Studies 41. 86–105.10.2307/2929667Search in Google Scholar

Hayek, F. A. V. 1978 [1976]. The mirage of social justice (Law, Legislation, and Liberty 2). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226321257.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Ionesco, E. 2010 [1951]. La leçon: Drame comique. Stuttgart: Ernst Klett Sprachen.Search in Google Scholar

Jakobson, R. 1972. Verbal communication. Scientific American 227(3). 72–81.10.1038/scientificamerican0972-72Search in Google Scholar

Kahneman, D. & A. Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47(2). 263–291.10.2307/1914185Search in Google Scholar

Lotman, Y. M. 2001 [1990]. Universe of the mind: A semiotic theory of culture, A. Shukman (trans.). London: IB Tauris.Search in Google Scholar

McLuhan, M. 1994 [1964]. Understanding media: The extensions of man. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.4324/9780203992968-15Search in Google Scholar

Meyer, D. E. & R. W. Schvaneveldt. 1971. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology 90(2). 227–234.10.1037/h0031564Search in Google Scholar

Nöth, W. 1990. Handbook of semiotics. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.10.2307/j.ctv14npk46Search in Google Scholar

Parsons, T. 2017. The traditional square of opposition. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2017/entries/square/ (accessed 2 August 2017).Search in Google Scholar

Piaget, J. 2013 [1954]. The construction of reality in the child, M. Cook (trans.). London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315009650Search in Google Scholar

Quine, W. V. 1951. Two dogmas of empiricism. Philosophical Review 60(1). 20–43.10.2307/2181906Search in Google Scholar

Rumelhart, D. E. 1978. Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. San Diego, CA: Center for Human Information Processing.Search in Google Scholar

Saussure, F. de. 1959 [1916]. Course in general linguistics, C. Bally & A. Sechehaye (eds.), W. Baskin (trans.). New York: Philosophical Library.Search in Google Scholar

Shackell, C. 2018. Finite cognition and finite semiosis: A new perspective on semiotics for the information age. Semiotica 222. 225–240. doi:10.1515/sem-2018-0020.Search in Google Scholar

Shackell, C. 2019. Finite semiotics: Recovery functions, semioformation and the hyperreal. Semiotica 227. 211–226. doi:10.1515/sem-2016-0153.Search in Google Scholar

Uttal, W. R. 2013. Reliability in cognitive neuroscience: A meta-meta-analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9780262018524.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Wittgenstein, L. 1953. Philosophical investigations, G. E. M. Anscombe (trans.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Wittgenstein, L. 1974 [1922]. Tractatus logico-philosophicus, D. F. Pears & B. F. McGuinness (trans.). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2019-05-21
Published in Print: 2019-07-26

© 2019 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 24.4.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/sem-2017-0127/html
Scroll to top button