<iframe src="https://www.googletagmanager.com/ns.html?id=GTM-KCV32QR" height="0" width="0" style="display:none;visibility:hidden">

4,300-Year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology

Edited by Ofer Bar-Yosef, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, and approved December 7, 2006
February 27, 2007
104 (9) 3043-3048

Abstract

Archaeological research in the African rainforest reveals unexpected results in the search for the origins of hominoid technology. The ancient Panin sites from Côte d'Ivoire constitute the only evidence of prehistoric ape behavior known to date anywhere in the world. Recent archaeological work has yielded behaviorally modified stones, dated by chronometric means to 4,300 years of age, lodging starch residue suggestive of prehistoric dietary practices by ancient chimpanzees. The “Chimpanzee Stone Age” pre-dates the advent of settled farming villages in this part of the African rainforest and suggests that percussive material culture could have been inherited from an common human–chimpanzee clade, rather than invented by hominins, or have arisen by imitation, or resulted from independent technological convergence.
Tool use among modern wild ape populations, first reported in the early 19th century (1), has been documented throughout tropical Africa, and chimpanzees from West Africa are known for their use of stone tools for nut cracking (26). In 2002, the publication of recent buried remains of unintentionally fractured stone left behind by modern chimpanzees from Côte d'Ivoire outlined the potential of using archaeological methods in cultural primatological research and also identified the type of material assemblage that would allow archaeologists to detect and characterize ancient chimpanzee nut-cracking behavior. To test the feasibility and scope of the new methodology, we conducted further work in Taï National Park, and investigated the existence of ancient chimpanzee sites and whether these sites would offer an answer to the long-standing question of the antiquity of nut-cracking behavior (6, 7). The archaeological evidence retrieved consists of behaviorally modified stones, dated by chronometric means to 4,300 years of age, whose attached food residue suggests chimpanzee manipulation, rather than natural causes or human intervention. This discovery speaks of true prehistoric great ape behavior that predates the onset of agriculture in this part of Africa (8). The chimpanzee assemblages are contemporaneous with the local Later Stone Age (8); thus, they represent a parallel “Chimpanzee Stone Age” that prompts us to ask whether percussive material culture could have been inherited from a common human–chimpanzee clade rather than invented by hominins (cf. 9, 10), have arisen by imitation, or resulted from independent technological convergence.

The Archaeological Horizon: Age and Composition

The sites of Noulo, Sacoglotis B, and Panda 100 (Fig. 1) are located at 5°N /7°E, and 200 m above sea level; in the lowland rainforest of Taï National Park, Côte d'Ivoire, on the flood plain traversed by a small meandering black water stream called Audrenisrou; within the chimpanzee territory known as “North Group.” Archaeological excavation took place over the course of two field seasons in 2001 and 2003, following standard paleolithic archaeology techniques. The research area is in a closed-canopy environment that receives ≈2,000 mm of annual precipitation (11). As part of the West African Craton, the region's geological configuration is dominated by igneous and metamorphic rocks of Precambrian origin (12). The archaeological sites studied here are located on a flat riverbank that gets regularly inundated. Sedimentation is thicker than 4 m, alternating silty sands, muddy sands, and sandy silts that include patches of pebbles and cobbles. Small fires affecting isolated trees and caused by lightning are common occurrences throughout the Guineo–Congolian rainforest (13); leaving behind a ubiquitous record of former vegetation and forest composition as well as a source of datable material. Numerous pits excavated throughout the Audrenisrou valley bottom document the effects of past forest fires that charred one or several trees, and at the sites of Noulo, Panda 100, and Sacoglotis B, a total of 15 14C assays performed on excavated charcoal samples (Table 1) indicate that (i) sedimentary deposition in this part of the landscape is older than 6,000 years, (ii) the archaeological assemblage studied in this paper dates back 4,300 years, and (iii) this assemblage is composed of relict materials deposited at different times over the course of >2,000 years.
Fig. 1.
Map of Noulo. Excavation grid covers 100 m2. Solid black squares indicate granitoid specimens. Circles represent quartz specimens. Asterisks indicate feldspar-rich granitoids, and diamonds indicate laterite pieces. Each spot represents one piece, but in the case of shatter or refitted specimens, the number of specimens per spot is as follows: no. 6, 2; no. 8, 3; no. 34, 59; no. 35, 29. Light gray shading indicates granitoid shatter. Triangles and dark gray shading indicate quartz shatter, representing specimens reduced by humans shown on Table 2 as nos. 35, 29; 38, 27; and 45, 26.
Table 1.
List of radiometric ages by 14C from the sites of Noulo, Panda 100, and Sacoglotis B
Provenance Sample data 13C/12C, per mil Conventional radiocarbon age, years B.P.
Noulo
Uppermost part of profile Beta-196391 −27.3 230 ± 40
  Beta-195439 −27.2 270 ± 40
  Beta-196390 −26.4 410 ± 40
Main archaeological horizon Beta-196953 −27.5 2,200 ± 40
  Beta-195444 −26.7 2,890 ± 40
  Beta-196392 −26.3 2,910 ± 40
  Beta-195441 −28.9 2,970 ± 40
  Beta-195440 −25.9 4,240 ± 120
Underneath archaeological horizon Beta-195442 −28.1 6,290 ± 40
Panda 100 Beta-172916 −27.6 2,330 ± 40
  Beta-164876 −27.9 2,440 ± 40
  Beta-164877 −27.2 2,440 ± 40
  Beta-172913 −26.8 3,750 ± 40
  Beta-164879 −25 4,280 ± 40
Sacoglotis B Beta-195443 −28.6 1,280 ± 40
We present stone assemblages from three sites east of the Audrenisrou, <200 m apart from each other (Fig. 1). Most of these stones, however, come from one site alone: Noulo (three specimens came from Panda 100; and one from the site of Sacoglotis B). At Noulo, we excavated a 10-m grid down to 1 m of depth, and unearthed a stone-rich horizon at 60–90 cm (three specimens were found at 29–57 cm). This horizon produced 23,168 g of modified stone (206 pieces) (Table 2). Its physical condition is fresh, except for some instances of chemical weathering in the form of surface desquamation. Stones from all size categories and diverse raw materials were excavated with shatter <20 mm excluded (mean length: 68 mm; range: 25–220 mm). Granitoid rocks represent 62%; quartz and quartzite represent 36%; laterite represents 1%, and feldspar represents 1%. The horizontal scattering of this collection clusters around three areas rich in granitoid shatter, with two smaller outliers to the north and south of these higher density spots (Fig. 1). About half of the quartz–quartzite assemblages concentrated in the southwest corner of the grid. The granitoid pieces broke off rocks that, generally, are not susceptible to percussive fracture (nonconchoidal, 88%; 20,370 g); a smaller portion of the assemblage (12%; 2,799 g) was from quartz and quartzite; rocks whose fracture results in a curved, rippled breakage surface (conchoidal). Average concentration of nonconchoidal pieces is 1.2 per m2; mean concentration for conchoidal pieces is ≈0.7 per m2.
Table 2.
Technological features of the stone assemblage excavated in the Taï National Park, Côte d'Ivoire
ID Site Blind test Fragments, n Depth, cm Technological by-product of thrusting percussion Dimensions, mm PML Mass for thr. perc. elements* Raw material Starch Proposed agency
Starch Stone Length Height Width Nonconchoidal Conchoidal Botanical origin: Nut ≥1 nut species cracked by chimpanzees
1 Noulo     1 60 Chunk 70 60 35 280 202.9 (by 4) = 811.6 Granitoid   X X C
2 Noulo     1 70 Edge/corner 85 120 20 391 622.8 (by 4.6) = 2,864.88 Granitoid   X X C
3 Noulo     1 70 Edge/corner 65 65 22 325 186 (by 5) = 930 Granitoid   X X C
4 Noulo     1 70 Edge/corner 80 25 15 368 102.1 (by 4.6) = 469.66 Granitoid   X X C
5 Noulo 31–32 4 1 70 Chunk 110 60 50 440 358.1 (by 4) = 1,432.4   Quartz X X C
6 Noulo     2 70 Edge/corner 50 40 30 250 83.1 (by 5) = 415.5 Granitoid   N/A NRE C
7 Noulo 34–36 NT 1 78 Complete piece 130 40 40 130 849.4 Granitoid   X X Unclear
8 Noulo     3 84 Chunk 90 90 30 405 395 (by 4.5) = 1,777.5 Granitoid   Unclear Unclear C
9 Noulo     1 70 Chunk 150 150 60 525 1,928.7 (by 3.5) = 6,750.4 Feldspar   X X C
10 Noulo     1 60 Flake 30 50 15 N/A 2.9 Granitoid   N/A NRE Unclear
11 Noulo     1 62 Flake 30 45 10 N/A 22.9 Granitoid   N/A NRE Unclear
12 Noulo     1 62 Flake 15 15 8 N/A 3.6 Granitoid   N/A NRE Unclear
13 Noulo 30 11 1 76 Chunk 100 80 80 150 927.6 (by 1.5) = 1,391.4 Laterite   X X Unclear
14 Noulo 33 8 1 63 Complete piece 100 70 70 100 1014.8 Granitoid   X X Unclear
15 Noulo     1 76 Chunk 100 50 40 300 295.5 (by 3) = 886.5 Quartz-rich granitoid   X X C
16 Noulo     1 83 Flake 30 40 15 N/A 20.7 Quartz-rich granitoid   N/A NRE Unclear
17 Noulo     1 80 Chunk 100 90 50 230 670.8 (by 2.3) = 1,542.84 Quartz-rich granitoid   X X C
18 Noulo     1 90 Chunk 100 90 50 300 742.4 (by 3) = 2,227.2 Granitoid   X X C
19 Noulo     1 70 Edge/corner 55 20 20 247.5 119.7 (By 4.5) = 538.65 Laterite   N/A NRE C
20 Noulo     1 83 Chunk 70 60 30 245 233.2 (by 3.5) = 816.2   Quartz X X C
21 Noulo 37 32 1 68 Chunk 100 50 50 300 956.6 (by 3) = 2,869.8   Quartz X X C
22 Noulo     1 83 Flake 70 90 10 n/a 90.5 Granitoid   N/A NRE Unclear
23 Noulo 3–9 3 1 70 Chunk 220 80 55 660 1,686.9 (by 3) = 5,060.7 Granitoid   X X C
24 Noulo 10–20 NT 1 80 Almost complete piece 70 60 60 70 556.9 Granitoid   X X Unclear
25 Noulo     1 80 Edge/corner 100 80 30 500 435.4 (by 5) = 2,177   Quartzite X X C
26 Noulo     1 74 Chunk 100 80 60 450 904.5 (by 4.5) = 4,070.26 Granitoid   X X C
27 Noulo 38 6 1 29 Edge/corner 70 70 60 231 557.6 (by 3.3) = 1,840 Diorite   X X C
28 Noulo 28–29 NT 1 50 Chunk 140 70 70 420 1,525.9 (by 3) = 4,577.7 Granitoid   X X C
29 Sacoglotis B 1–2 14 1 15 Complete piece 190 80 80 190 3761.5 Feldspar   X X C
30 Panda 100     1 121 Edge/corner 80 90 45 320 394.2 (by 4) = 1,576.8 Quartz-rich granitoid   Barren sample No residue detected after two sonication cycles C
31 Panda 100     1 125 Chunk 110 60 60 495 937.9 (by 4.5) = 4,220.5 Quartz-rich granitoid   X X C
32 Panda 100 21–27 NT 1 110 Edge/corner 70 70 25 350 159.1 (By 5) = 795.5 Granitoid   X X C
33 Noulo     1 57 Edge/corner 50 20 35 250 29.5 (by 5) = 147.5 Granitoid   N/A NRE C
PML, projected maximum length; NRE, no residue extracted; NT, not tested. For the complex data set, see supporting information (SI) Table 3.
*Boldface figures are projected.
Can the archaeological evidence from the Taï forest be attributed to natural or behavioral agency? If behavioral, can we discriminate between stone modifications such as those produced by nut-cracking versus those derived from systematic flaking? What is the direct evidence for nut-cracking, and especially, do these stone assemblages represent mid-Holocene human or chimpanzee agency?

Fluvial Geofact versus Behavioral Accumulation

Fine sands, silt, and clay are indicative of very low energy fluvial regimes such as floods and standing pools associated with braided and meandering streams. Grain size from the excavated matrix corresponds with the “Ingram–Wentworth” sedimentological profile known as “muddy sands” (14) and indicates that pebble-, cobble-, and boulder-sized rock fragments would be naturally lacking. This prediction is corroborated by the results from excavation in 26 off-site ground-truthing pits (area: 0.5–6.25 m2 taken to a maximum depth of 2 m) along a 220-m transect around the site of Noulo, in which cobble-sized stones were found in 5 pits only; the remaining 21 pits yielded ceramics, lithics, iron, or nothing. To test the proposition that the accumulation of stones reported here took place by means other than fluvial agency, a triple blind test was carried out. The lead author (J.M.) and two independent observers (J.H. and S.K.) classified stone specimens randomly numbered by J.M. as coming from (i) nonartifactual objects shaped exclusively by geological forces, (ii) objects produced by systematic flaking, or (iii) objects generated by cracking (thrusting percussion). The test collection comprised 90 rock specimens, of which 60 were not from the Taï forest. To the extent of our possibilities, we included raw materials that are likely to be found in a granitic domain, such granites, diorites, quartzite, and quartz, but limited access to geofact collections led us to include other rocks as well. Of these, 30 came from a high-energy, undisturbed glacial till in the Canadian Rockies, near Banff, Alberta, where silicified limestone pebbles and cobbles show several kinds of natural modification that gives them the appearance of artifacts (15). The other 30 came from a demonstrated behavioral site dated to the mid-Holocene (site FiPo-147; Royal Alberta Museum) that yielded hundreds of artifacts including lithic debitage in quartz and quartzite, among other raw materials (16). The remainder of 30 specimens came from the Ivorian sites of Noulo (n = 28), Panda 100 (n = 1), and Sacoglotis B (n = 1), and these were previously selected by J.M. as stone pieces that could be behaviorally modified, comprising all types and morphological variables. Criteria for this part of the triple exercise focused on worn, rounded stones with surface damage or removals, and >50% of cortical cover. Then, the examiners sought a minimum of two additional attributes thought to be common on geofacts (17): four or fewer flake scars, lack of platform preparation, differential weathering of removals, random scar alignment, striations on surfaces, or entirely cortical flakes. Statistical analysis demonstrates very high levels of agreement among all three examiners: 90.00% (κ test > 0.799; n = 90, P < 0.000). In other words, the Ivorian stones are not geofacts, as confirmed by three independent observers.

Discriminating Systematic Flaking from Thrusting Percussion Products

Systematic flaking is an exclusive behavioral characteristic of Plio–Pleistocene hominins (1821). During the Holocene, Later Stone Age lithic assemblages from the African rainforest (22) demonstrate that systematic flaking can be identified beyond any doubt when one or more of the following criteria are present: cores that display logical reduction strategies (e.g., radial), cores with more than five flake scars, intentional creation or preparation of platforms, bifacial reduction, retouch, or blade production. Agreement among three observers (J.M., J.H., and S.K.) through a blind test designed to identify systematic flaking is 93.33% (κ test < 0.754; n = 30; P < 0.000). One examiner (S.K.) interpreted 10% of the stones that were known to have been produced by systematic flaking as derived from thrusting percussion, and the other stone examiner (J.H.) interpreted a separate 10% also as fashioned by thrusting percussion. Systematic flaking can be distinguished from the products of thrusting percussion of unknown agency. The three examiners agreed that 35 of 64 stone specimens larger than 20 mm were produced by thrusting percussion, whereas 28 stones resulted from systematic flaking. Because chimpanzees have not yet been observed to flake stone intentionally, it is unquestionable that human lithic reduction is present at the site. Therefore, the possibility that humans could be the sole culprit of our stone collections must be carefully examined. For the entire sedimentary package, the amount of human lithics amounts to 0.77 per m3. This figure alone is telling; for we know that systematic quartz reduction from Later Stone Age sites in comparable geographical and ecological environments yields contrasting higher numbers, often consisting of thousands of specimens per m3 (22) (number of sites, 10; mean, 1,030; range, 70–5,000). In addition, this part of the landscape, a frequently inundated riverbank, probably was not a preferred site for humans, and its occupation seems sporadic; brief visits to the river bed to collect pebbles that could be tested or reduced for flakes. Typical archaeological features commonly found in Later Stone Age sites from the African rainforest (23, 24) were not found here, such as inhabitation structures, activity areas, hearths, or charred food remains. That is, the presence of a limited number of lithics is not, by itself, proof of agency for the other technologies present at the site. It does suggest that humans were present in this area and that, over time, they visited the river bank where the rest of the archaeological remains were also deposited.

Identifying and Classifying the Products of Thrusting Percussion

Could some of these stone pieces have been used and discarded by chimpanzees? The maximum length, three-dimensional volume, and total mass of stone products can help elucidate the agency that generated them. A review of hammer metrics from early sites (9, 10, 25) indicates that the hominin hand accommodates hammerstone up to 12 cm in maximum length, often <8 cm. Hominin hammers typically weigh <400 g, and even anvils, the largest component of this bimodal technology, on average, reach a maximum weight of 1,000 g, but often weigh much less. We know that hominins do intentionally select tabular rocks and raw materials such as quartz and quartzite for their hammers and anvils at sites such as Olduvai (Bed 1) (10). In contrast, a 1-year study of nut-cracking sites (n = 1434) and hammers (n = 133) among modern chimpanzees (26) indicates both a strong preference for granitoid rocks, and large/heavy hammers (65% weigh 1,000–9,000 g; 23% weigh <1,000 g; 12% weigh >9,000 g; the limit is 24,000 g). This marked difference probably is related to the larger size of the chimpanzee hand, its morphology, and the overall arm strength, which are several times superior to that of any hominin species known to have used hammers and anvils.
To classify the products of thrusting percussion, we modified published work on the different modalities of products generated by bashing technologies (10) and distinguished complete pieces, “chunks” (significant portion of the hammer/anvil has been preserved), “edges/corners,” flakes, and shatter (Fig. 2). Our blind test reveals that independent observers agree on the classification of thrusting percussion by-products in 70% of the cases (κ values between 0.508 and 0.565; n = 20, P < 0.001), which shows that stone pieces unintentionally generated by thrusting percussion and stone pieces intentionally produced by systematic flaking can be differentiated. The majority of the stones excavated at Noulo are the unintentional by-products of bashing technologies. To discriminate human from chimpanzee agency, we used reconstructions of original size and mass of the stone fragments before breakage. These estimates were subject to blind testing as well, and our results indicate a high level of confidence in the maximum projected length and mass by three independent observers (65%, 75%, and 90%; κ values of 0.493, 0.636, and 0.851, respectively; n = 20; P < 0.000). The mean maximum length of stone by-products derived from thrusting percussion is 97 mm (range, 35–220 mm), and the average mass is 710 g (range, 21–3,761 g). Flakes generated by thrusting percussion average 33.5 mm. Therefore, our reconstructed totals for thrusting percussion products suggest a projected maximum length of ≈320 mm (range, 70–660 mm), and maximum mass or ≈2,030 g (range, 145–6,750 g), well above what is known for by-products of hominin thrusting percussion.
Fig. 2.
Selected thrusting percussion products. Stone specimen ID, as it appears on Table 2: A, 2; B, 4; C, 11; D, 12; E, 25; F, 20; G (two pieces), 23; H, 17; I, 28; J, 29; K, 14; L, 27; M, 13. (Photograph by Gerald Newlands, Department of Archaeology, University of Calgary.)

Starch Residues as Direct Evidence for Nut-Cracking

Can we demonstrate that such stones were used by chimpanzees? Present-day chimpanzees crack five nut species that, except for one (Coula edulis), are not cracked by local human populations (26). If part of the stone assemblage retrieved at Noulo was used to crack starch-bearing nuts open, those pieces might shelter ancient starch residue still lodged in the stone crevices. If so, this may represent direct evidence for both ancient plant usage and stone function. To test this proposition, our research design followed field and laboratory protocols used by numerous researchers in the study of ancient starch grains throughout the world (see Materials and Methods) (2733). Once all examiners achieved a reasonable familiarity with the reference collection, we asked two examiners (H.B. and R.T.) to inspect 40 starch granules (10.8% of the total available granule population extracted from the archaeological stone; n = 370) previously processed, quantified, classified, and identified by J.M. to test the degree to which these granules could be consistently interpreted to typify specific plant usage. Agreement over starch grain classification has been estimated by pairing up the results produced by the different observers separately. Classification at the level of botanical origin (nut versus legume and tuber) yields high agreement (>77.8%; higher to a 50% agreement criterion, binomial test: P < 0.000, and equal to 85% agreement criterion, binomial test P > 0.2). Several nut species yielded morphotypes of very similar characteristics, making single-species identification inconsistent between analysts; a problem that can be minimized by proposing two to four species as likely identifications (J.M. and R.T. agreement = 92.5%, binomial 85% criterion, P > 0.4; yet, agreement for all three examiners is 48%, binomial 50% criterion, P > 0.5). In general, our data indicate that at least 3/4 of the tested sample derived from nuts (Fig. 3), not tubers or legumes. Moreover, all three observers agreed that three nut species cracked exclusively by chimpanzees (Parinari excelsa, Panda oleosa, and Detarium senegalense) are the only candidates 7 of 40 times (17.5%), with two observers (J.M. and R.T.) agreeing that these species could be present an additional 17 times (extra 42.5%).
Fig. 3.
Starch grains extracted from archaeological specimens amount to 370 grains, and these can be classified into three types: Type 1 (AE), comprising subrounded to subangular, medium size grains (10–25 μm) with a smooth to rugose surface and distinct quandrangular, hexagonal, or octagonal faceting. The hylum (or geometric centre of the grain) often supports a single cavity, a slit, or several fissures forming a Y, cross, or star-like patterns. The arms of the cross are often straight, but sometimes they take on wavy and hooked morphologies. Type 2 (FJ) is a small (5–10 μm) to medium (10–25 μm) spherical grain with a smooth surface. It shows a centric slit or fissures. Its arms are straight and widen out toward the periphery. Type 3 (KO) is a medium (10–25 μm) to large (25 μm) lenticular grain with a characteristic wide and diffuse cross; sometimes smooth, sometimes with variably marked lamellae. Micrographs from the reference collection illustrate selected examples of one or several morphotypes from the nuts of Parinari excelsa (P, Q, W, and X), Detarium senegalense (R and V), Coula edulis (S and T), and Panda oleosa (U). Stone specimen identification, as it appears on Table 2: A, 20; B, 29; C, 32, D and E, 35; F, 15; G, 7; H, 23; I, 5; J, 7; K, 34; L, 27; M, 3; N, 24; O, 24. (Micrographs by J.M. and Gerald Newlands.)

Discussion and Implications

From a stone perspective, it can be said that the choice of raw materials displayed in the mid-Holocene thrusting percussion assemblage from the Taï forest mirrors the choices made today by local chimpanzee populations, to whom granitoid rocks are the preferred choice for ≈3/4 of their assemblages (7). The rest is made up of laterite, diorite, feldspar, and quartz. The density of stone pieces within the excavated horizon per m2 (1.27) matches that observed in known chimpanzee nut-cracking sites excavated with identical methodologies (1.46 per m2) (7). For the same raw materials, maximum flake length at Noulo (33.5 mm) resembles maximum flake length produce in known modern chimpanzee nut-cracking assemblages (35 mm) (7). The percent of shatter <20 mm produced at Noulo is 74%, which is very similar to the percent of shatter <20 mm produced in demonstrated chimpanzee assemblages (79%) (7). Likewise, use-wear patterns from chimpanzee nut cracking include small pits and hollow concavities (6, 34), which are found in Noulo along with systematic edge and corner damage. In addition, stone metrics at Noulo are indicative of large hand size and great strength and follow the metrics of chimpanzee hammers more closely than they follow human ones.
The majority of the starch grains retrieved from the by-products of thrusting percussion comes from a set of nuts typically found at chimpanzee nut-cracking sites. In a 1-year study, Boesch and Boesch (26) reported 1,434 sites where chimpanzees exploited Coula edulis (52%), Panda oleosa (32%), Detarium senegalense (9%), Parinari excelsa (6%), and Sacoglotis gabonensis (1%). This is the set of nuts seen in the prehistoric starch sample. By contrast, human rain forest dwellers rely very heavily on starch-producing tubers and legumes, as well as the “incense” tree Canarium schweinfurthii (17.5% of oily–starchy food consumed among Congolese pygmies) and the African oil palm Elaeis guineensis (10% of oily–starchy food consumed among Congolese pygmies) (35, 36). Chimpanzees in Bossou, Republic of Guinea, >250 km north of Taï, have been observed cracking the kernels of Elaeis guineensis (37). Considering how closely associated this plant seems to be with African forest farming communities, some have gone further to suggest that chimpanzees learned to nut-crack from forest farmers (38). However, the starch assemblage from Noulo predates the appearance of farming in this part of the African forest, and yields no significant evidence of species that correspond to modern human forager or farming use. Two starch grains were matched with reference materials of the genus Dioscorea. These were recovered from stones at two separate archaeological sites, Noulo and Panda 100, and belong to wild evergreen rainforest lianas whose tubers could have accidentally came in contact with the stones, and thus are best interpreted as background noise. Moreover, none of the pounding, grinding, and pestling tools that human foragers and farmers use to process starchy foods appears at Noulo.
In conclusion, the geological, morphological, technological, metric, and mass parameters for the nut-cracking stone assemblage reported in this study rule out human or natural agency. The nut species cracked all belong to a set of taxa known to be exploited by modern chimpanzees, and part of the starch assemblage may belong to species exclusively consumed by chimpanzees. The full implications of this line of work require a reevaluation of the terms under which we can make meaningful comparisons of Oldowan and Chimpanzee cultures. The behavioral variables documented at Noulo indicate that chimpanzees and hominins share cultural attributes (3941), including the transport of stones across the landscape for a projected use elsewhere; the optimal combination of raw material, size, and weight criteria to perform a predicted activity; the re-occupation of focal points (the accumulation and concentration of both stone and botanical debris is artificially created by behavior); creation of activity areas; the use of locally available resources; and the curation and selection of specific types of stones that are most optimal for specific technological activities.
The systematic archaeological study of prehistoric chimpanzee cultures suggests that the “Chimpanzee Stone Age” started at least 4,300 years ago, that nut-cracking behavior in the Taï forest has been transmitted over the course of >200 generations, and that chimpanzee material culture has a long prehistory whose deep roots are only beginning to be uncovered. These findings substantiate the contribution of rainforest archaeology to human evolutionary studies in areas other than the classical savanna-woodlands of East and Southern Africa and add support to fossil discoveries from these other regions indicative of an ancient chimpanzee past (42).

Materials and Methods

For ancient starch analysis, we processed a large collection of residue extracted from 31 behaviorally modified stone pieces from a pool of 38 specimens, 7 of which have been positively identified by all examiners as derived from thrusting percussion. Second, we used multiple lines of control samples (3032) to test whether the tools were the primary source of starch grains, rather than random contamination. Although we cannot rule out the possibility that some of the starch discovered on the stones comes from redeposition unrelated to use (e.g., background starch derived from roots coming in contact with the stone over hundreds of years), the differential preservation of starch granules on the stone specimens compared with that in their surrounding free-standing matrix indicates that a large part of the assemblage is genuine prehistoric residue (3032). Third, for all examiners to propose potential identification for the grains, we worked with a reference collection that included not just the five nut species known to be cracked by chimpanzees, but also two other types of starch-producing plants typically used by human foragers in the African rainforest. Thus, we included all nut and tuber species relevant to modern hominoid starch use plus other species present in our study area totaling 18 nut species, 1 palm pith, and 8 rainforest yams. The specimens used for the test were made available to the examiners on 13 separate microscope slides, and these represented 37% of the total residue (n = 370 grains) obtained from the sites of Noulo (11 stones providing 37 grains), Panda 100 (1 stone providing 1 starch grain), and Sacoglotis B (1 stone providing 2 grains). Extraction protocols for both sediment and residue samples follow the methodology of Zarrillo and Kooyman (33) with minor modifications. Quantity of sediments used ranged from 0.5 g (residue samples extracted from stones through sonication) to 1 g (free-standing sediments). Column samples (n = 5) were taken at 10-cm intervals from the North wall at the site of Noulo. Modern deposits formed around decomposing, natural piles of nuts (n = 4) from Detarium senegalense, Sacoglotis gabonensis, Panda oleosa, and Coula edulis/Parinari excelsa were scraped off to a maximum depth of 2.5 cm. Modern chimpanzee hammers (n = 3) came from the uppermost levels of two sites, Loukoum and Panda 100, and were all excavated to a maximum depth of 5 cm. Natural stones on-site (n = 2), off-site (n = 1), Later Stone Age lithics (radial cores) (n = 3), stone sharpener fashioned by humans (n = 1), and matrix samples retrieved around archaeological specimens, but not in contact with them (n = 4), provided the rest of our control samples. The amount of starch granules found in these control samples varies, and the types found are similar to those seen in the archaeological specimens. Values below represent an average for a given type of control. For example, four sediment samples collected around stone specimens yielded no grains. Five free-standing, on-site column samples yielded an average of 0.4 grains. Four naturally occurring nut piles yielded a mean ≈0.5 grains. Three unmodified on- and offsite stones yielded no grains, whereas four Later Stone Age stone tools yielded a mean of 0.2 grains. In contrast, residue extracted from 31 archaeological stone produced by thrusting percussion contains a mean of 12 grains per stone (3 of the 31 stones yielded no grains; 28 stones yielded 1–90 grains).

Acknowledgments

We especially thank Roger Mundry, Sonia Zarrillo, Andrea Freeman, Scott Raymond, Gerald Newlands, A. Hladik, Alison Brooks, and Bernard Wood. We thank the Ministry of Research, Ministry of Environment and Forest in Côte d'Ivoire, and the Centre Suisse pour la Recherche Scientifique. We thank our respective institutions, but especially the main sponsor for this work, the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (Leipzig, Germany), for providing the large majority of the financial support required to carry out this research. Additional support came from the Canada Research Chairs Program, Canada Foundation for Innovation Grant 201550 (to J.M. and the Tropical Archaeology Laboratory), and the Faculty of Social Sciences and the Department of Archaeology at the University of Calgary. Institutional support came from the Department of Anthropology (George Washington University, Washington, DC) and the Human Origins Program (Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC).

Supporting Information

07909Table3.xls

References

1
T Savage, J Wyman Boston J Nat Hist 4, 362–386 (1843).
2
C Boesch, H Boesch Folia Primatologica 54, 86–99 (1990).
3
D Anderson, E Nordheim, C Boesch, T Moermond Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, eds C Boesch, G Hohmann, L Marchant (Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK), pp. 90–101 (2002).
4
G Whitesides Primates 26, 91–94 (1985).
5
W McGrew, P Baldwin, LF Marchant, JD Pruetz, SE Scott, CEG Tutin Palaeoanthropology 1, 1–20 (2003).
6
C Boesch, H Boesch-Achermann The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford, 2000).
7
J Mercader, M Panger, C Boesch Science 296, 1452–1455 (2002).
8
J Casey African Archaeology, ed A Stahl (Blackwell, Oxford), pp. 225–248 (2005).
9
SA de Beaune Curr Anthropol 45, 139–162 (2004).
10
R Mora, I de la Torre J Anthropol Archaeol 24, 179–182 (2005).
11
RV Rompaey Climate Le Parc National de Taï, Côte d'Ivoire, eds E Riezebos, A Vooren (Tropenbos, Wageningen, The Netherlands), pp. 42–50 (1994).
12
A Goodwin Precambrian Geology: The Dynamic Evolution of the Continental Crust (Academic, London, 1991).
13
T Hart, J Hart, M Dechamps, M Fournier, M Ataholo The Biodiversity of African Plants, eds LJG van der Maesen, XM van der Burgt, JM van Madenback de Kooy (Kluwer Academic, Dordrecht, The Netherlands), pp. 545–563 (1996).
14
G Rapp, C Hill Geoarchaeology: The Earth Science Approach to Archaeological Interpretation (Yale Univ Press, New Haven, CT, 1998).
15
N Rutter Can Alpine J 49, 157 (1966).
16
J Damp Archaeological Survey of Alberta Interim Report, permit no 81-67. (1982).
17
J Gillespie, S Tupakka, C Cluney Geoarchaeology 19, 615–633 (2004).
18
S Semaw J Archaeol Sci 27, 1197–1214 (2000).
19
A Delagnes, H Roche J Hum Evol 48, 435–472 (2005).
20
T Plummer Yearbook Phys Anthropol 47, 118–164 (2004).
21
I Davidson, W McGrew J Royal Anthropol Inst 1, 793–817 (2005).
22
J Mercader, A Brooks J Anthropol Res 57, 197–217 (2001).
23
J Mercader Evol Anthropol 11, 117–124 (2002).
24
J Mercader Under the Canopy: The Archaeology of Tropical Rainforests, ed J Mercader (Rutgers Univ Press, New Brunswick, NJ), pp. 93–116 (2003).
25
N Goren-Inbar, G Sharon, Y Melamed, M Kislev Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99, 2455–2460 (2002).
26
C Boesch, H Boesch Behaviour 83, 265–286 (1983).
27
DR Piperno, E Weiss, I Holst, D Nadel Nature 430, 670–673 (2004).
28
L Perry J Archaeol Sci 31, 1069–1081 (2004).
29
D Pearsall, K Chandler-Ezell, J Zeidler J Archaeol Sci 31, 423–442 (2004).
30
H Barton, R Torrence, R Fullagar J Archaeol Sci 25, 1231–1238 (1998).
31
M Haslam J Archaeol Sci 31, 1715–1734 (2004).
32
H Barton, P Matthews Ancient Starch Research, eds R Torrence, H Barton (Left Coast, Walnut Creek, CA), pp. 75–94 (2006).
33
S Zarrillo, B Kooyman Am Antiquity 71, 473–499 (2006).
34
W McGrew Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution (Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK, 1992).
35
H Tere Signification des Noms Vernaculaires des Plantes Chez les Guéré (Côte d'Ivoire) (Swiss Acad Sci, Abidjan, Côte d'Ivoire, 2000).
36
M Ichikawa Tropical Forests, Peoples and Foods, eds C Hladik, A Hladik, O Linares, H Pagezy, M Hadley (Parthenon, Paris) Vol 13, 487–496 (1993).
37
Y Sugiyama, J Koman Primates 20, 513–524 (1979).
38
A Kortlandt J Hum Evol 15, 77–132 (1986).
39
M Panger, A Brooks, BG Richmond, B Wood Evol Anthropol 11, 235–245 (2002).
40
R Potts J Anthropol Res 47, 153–176 (1991).
41
F Joulian Modelling the Early Human Mind, eds P Mellars, K Gibson (Cambridge Univ Press, Cambridge, UK), pp. 173–189 (1996).
42
S McBrearty, N Jablonsky Nature 437, 105–108 (2005).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

Go to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Go to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Vol. 104 | No. 9
February 27, 2007
PubMed: 17360606

Classifications

Submission history

Received: September 8, 2006
Published online: February 27, 2007
Published in issue: February 27, 2007

Acknowledgments

We especially thank Roger Mundry, Sonia Zarrillo, Andrea Freeman, Scott Raymond, Gerald Newlands, A. Hladik, Alison Brooks, and Bernard Wood. We thank the Ministry of Research, Ministry of Environment and Forest in Côte d'Ivoire, and the Centre Suisse pour la Recherche Scientifique. We thank our respective institutions, but especially the main sponsor for this work, the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (Leipzig, Germany), for providing the large majority of the financial support required to carry out this research. Additional support came from the Canada Research Chairs Program, Canada Foundation for Innovation Grant 201550 (to J.M. and the Tropical Archaeology Laboratory), and the Faculty of Social Sciences and the Department of Archaeology at the University of Calgary. Institutional support came from the Department of Anthropology (George Washington University, Washington, DC) and the Human Origins Program (Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC).

Notes

This article is a PNAS direct submission.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/0607909104/DC1.

Authors

Affiliations

Julio Mercader [email protected]
Department of Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4;
Huw Barton
Department of Archaeology and Ancient History, University of Leicester, Leicester LE1 7RH, United Kingdom;
Jason Gillespie
Department of Anthropology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada T6G 2H4
Jack Harris
Department of Anthropology, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, New Brunswick, NJ 08901;
Steven Kuhn
Department of Anthropology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721;
Robert Tyler
Department of Applied Microbiology and Food Science, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Canada S7N 5A8;
Christophe Boesch
Department of Primatology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany; and

Notes

To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]
Author contributions: J.M. and C.B. designed research; J.M. and C.B. performed research; J.M., H.B., J.G., J.H., S.K., R.T., and C.B. analyzed data; J.M. and C.B. wrote the paper; H.B. and R.T. were part of blind test (starch); J.G. contributed placebo sample for blind test (stone); and J.H. and S.K. were part of blind test (stone).

Competing Interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.


Citation statements




Altmetrics

Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

Cited by

    Loading...

    View Options

    View options

    PDF format

    Download this article as a PDF file

    DOWNLOAD PDF

    Get Access

    Login options

    Check if you have access through your login credentials or your institution to get full access on this article.

    Personal login Institutional Login

    Recommend to a librarian

    Recommend PNAS to a Librarian

    Purchase options

    Purchase this article to get full access to it.

    Single Article Purchase

    4,300-Year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
    • Vol. 104
    • No. 9
    • pp. 3017-3667

    Media

    Figures

    Tables

    Other

    Share

    Share

    Share article link

    Share on social media