Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Mol. Neurosci., 24 June 2022
Sec. Methods and Model Organisms
Volume 15 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2022.900213

Larval Zebrafish as a Model for Mechanistic Discovery in Mental Health

  • Institute of Molecular and Cell Biology, Agency for Science, Technology and Research (A*STAR), Singapore, Singapore

Animal models are essential for the discovery of mechanisms and treatments for neuropsychiatric disorders. However, complex mental health disorders such as depression and anxiety are difficult to fully recapitulate in these models. Borrowing from the field of psychiatric genetics, we reiterate the framework of ‘endophenotypes’ – biological or behavioral markers with cellular, molecular or genetic underpinnings – to reduce complex disorders into measurable behaviors that can be compared across organisms. Zebrafish are popular disease models due to the conserved genetic, physiological and anatomical pathways between zebrafish and humans. Adult zebrafish, which display more sophisticated behaviors and cognition, have long been used to model psychiatric disorders. However, larvae (up to 1 month old) are more numerous and also optically transparent, and hence are particularly suited for high-throughput screening and brain-wide neural circuit imaging. A number of behavioral assays have been developed to quantify neuropsychiatric phenomena in larval zebrafish. Here, we will review these assays and the current knowledge regarding the underlying mechanisms of their behavioral readouts. We will also discuss the existing evidence linking larval zebrafish behavior to specific human behavioral traits and how the endophenotype framework can be applied. Importantly, many of the endophenotypes we review do not solely define a diseased state but could manifest as a spectrum across the general population. As such, we make the case for larval zebrafish as a promising model for extending our understanding of population mental health, and for identifying novel therapeutics and interventions with broad impact.

Population Mental Health and Endophenotypes

Mental health disorder prevalence has increased globally, and has recently been aggravated by the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, with an estimated worldwide increase of 682.4 cases of major depressive disorder and 977.5 cases of anxiety disorders per 100,000 population in 2020 alone (Santomauro et al., 2021). Depression and anxiety arising due to COVID-19 are estimated to have caused 49.4 and 44.5 million Disability adjusted life years (DALYs), respectively, the metric used by the biennial global burden of disease study to represent the number of years of healthy life lost to disability or death caused by disease (Santomauro et al., 2021).

Even though the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) has clear-cut criteria for mental illness diagnosis, there is also increasing evidence that mental health exists as a spectrum, or continuum of states, rather than a binary classification of healthy vs. ill (Keyes, 2002; Kupfer and Regier, 2011). Multiple disorders share common risk factors, and disorders arise through a combination of gene effects and environmental factors. Hence, Hyman (2010) summarizes the arguments for a mental health continuum model, highlighting the current limitations of a categorical system.

Despite the prevalence and severity of mental illnesses or ill-being, the development of interventions has lagged behind. We attribute this to at least two reasons. Firstly, mental health disorders are highly complex and heterogeneous, involving multiple interacting mechanisms. Secondly, no animal model can fully recapitulate the intricacies of these disorders, leaving us limited in studying disease mechanisms and searching for potential therapeutics. Rather than tackling such a multidimensional problem head on, it may be helpful to focus on individual quantifiable features of such disorders, which could then be dissected in diverse model systems. Hence, the endophenotypic approach is an attractive solution, wherein complex disorders are dissected into simpler components (“endophenotypes”).

The term “endophenotype” was first used in the field of psychiatric genetics by Gottesman and Shields (1967, 1973) to discuss schizophrenia, and was further refined by Gottesman and Gould (2003). According to their definition, an endophenotype must be associated with a disease, heritable, independent of disease state, cosegregate with the disease within families and found in unaffected relatives more than the general population. Different patients, even under the same diagnosis, may present with distinct endophenotypes (Chen et al., 2000; Nandi et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2016; Buch and Liston, 2021), hence dissecting a complex disorder into these individual components can also allow for more personalized and targeted treatments for their illness.

An example of an endophenotype is prepulse inhibition (PPI), which reflects compromised sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia (Parwani et al., 2000), as well as in obsessive-compulsive disorder and Huntington’s disease (Swerdlow et al., 1995; Hoenig et al., 2005). Healthy individuals have a startle response when presented with a strong sensory stimulus, which can be attenuated if a weaker stimulus precedes it by around 100 ms – this attenuation is not observed in patients with these disorders. Genome-wide association studies identified a strong correlation between the AKT3 gene locus and schizophrenia (Ripke et al., 2013), where patients’ brains had lower expression levels of AKT3 protein (van Beveren et al., 2012). In zebrafish, PPI defects were similarly found in akt3 mutants (Thyme et al., 2019). Since sensorimotor gating impairment via prepulse inhibition has a genetic underpinning and segregates schizophrenia patients from healthy individuals, it is a strong endophenotype, which shares conserved mechanisms across species including zebrafish.

Whereas schizophrenia only affects a small subset of the population, other mental health disorders such as depression and anxiety exist as a broad spectrum (Angst et al., 2000; Lang and McTeague, 2009; Dichter et al., 2012; McTeague and Lang, 2012) and reflect dysregulation of motivated behavioral drives that are deeply conserved across evolution (LeDoux, 2012, 2021). In this review, we will more broadly define endophenotypes as behavioral markers that are associated with a human disorder and have a neural circuit or molecular-genetic underpinning (Table 1). We will identify parallel behaviors in larval zebrafish and compare the extent to which they can recapitulate human endophenotypes and their underlying mechanisms. Importantly, the identified behaviors can be assayed in a high-throughput manner using this model, facilitating both drug screening and mechanistic dissection which are key for discovering treatments for mental health disorders and improving mental health across the population.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Definitions.

Advantages of Larval Zebrafish

Zebrafish are popular models in studying neurological phenomena as summarized by Kalueff et al. (2014a) and Stewart et al. (2014). Sharing 70% of their genes with humans and conserved physiological and neuronal pathways (Howe et al., 2013; Stewart et al., 2015), many neurobehavioral parallels have been uncovered between zebrafish and mammals, including humans. For example, both the human and zebrafish hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axes [also known as the hypothalamic-pituitary-interrenal (HPI) axis in zebrafish] are activated in response to stress, leading to the secretion of corticotropin releasing hormone/factor (CRH/CRF) which stimulates pituitary corticotroph cells to release adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH). ACTH then activates the adrenal glands to produce and release cortisol into the blood. Unlike in rodent models, where corticosterone is the main stress hormone (Joëls et al., 2018), cortisol is the main stress hormone in both fish and humans, and exerts wide-ranging effects on physiology to allow the body to cope with stress. Depression and anxiety, amongst other mental health disorders, have been associated with HPA anomalies and cortisol dysregulation (Feder et al., 2004; Erhardt et al., 2006; Faravelli et al., 2012; Nandam et al., 2019). Hence, the zebrafish shares a core feature of human stress biology.

Not surprisingly, zebrafish have been used extensively to model psychiatric disorders and identify pharmacological interventions. Many such studies and reviews have focused on adult zebrafish (Cachat et al., 2010; Maximino et al., 2018) as they have more complex behaviors and a fully-developed brain. Nevertheless, there are significant benefits to using larvae, such as increased scalability and tractability. At the larval stage (up to 30 days old), zebrafish are numerous, small, and optically transparent. Larvae only require small volumes of water and can be contained in 48- or 96-well plates. A top view camera can be used to acquire videos of many larvae simultaneously during behavioral assays for live or post-acquisition analysis. This makes it well-suited for drug screening which emphasizes throughput. Beyond drug screening, dietary interventions (O’Neil et al., 2014; Godos et al., 2020) and the role of probiotics (Liu, 2017; Skonieczna-Żydecka et al., 2018) can also be explored. Furthermore, brain imaging to study the underlying biological mechanisms of endophenotypes or interventions is more convenient due to their transparency. Hence, this review will focus primarily on the potential of larval zebrafish to model human endophenotypes in mental health and disease.

Human and Zebrafish Anxiety and Mood Endophenotypes

While there are many mental health disorders, we will primarily be discussing anxiety/stress-related and mood disorders (see Table 1 for definitions). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM5) details a specific set of criteria for diagnosis of these disorders based on symptoms and behaviors exhibited (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Anxiety is a brain state induced by dangerous or stressful situations that continues to persist even after the threat has subsided (Rosen and Schulkin, 1998; Martin et al., 2010; Lang et al., 2014). Anxiety and stress-related disorders are diagnosed when the individual experiences persistent and excessive fear and anxiety and related behavioral disturbances (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). These are accompanied by physical symptoms such as restlessness, fatigue, difficulty concentrating, irritability and sleep disturbances (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Depressive disorders are part of a broader class of mood disorders in which emotional and motivational states are dysregulated. In depression, individuals experience persistent feelings of sadness, irritability, emptiness, or anhedonia as well as a reduction in ability and motivation to perform normal survival functions (Pizzagalli et al., 2008; Admon and Pizzagalli, 2015; Proulx et al., 2018). Diagnostic criteria include exhibiting some subset of symptoms that include diminished interest in daily activities, insomnia, appetite or weight gain or loss, psychomotor agitation or retardation, fatigue, feelings of guilt or suicidal ideation (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

These mental illnesses have high prevalence in the population as described in Section “Population Mental Health and Endophenotypes.” In addition, they tend to exist as a continuum – patient diagnosis occurs for more severe cases where the disorder interferes significantly with their daily lives; however, many undiagnosed individuals span the spectrum between mental wellness and illness and could also benefit from measures to improve mental health (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Conceptual relationship between endophenotypes and disease spectrum. Depression and anxiety exists along a spectrum of mental wellness to illness. There are overlapping and distinct aspects of these disorders that are also reflected in their shared and individual endophenotypes. Although we have categorized the endophenotypes into four main groups, these endophenotypes are interconnected and their relationships are worth investigating. The endophenotypes are supported by various underlying mechanisms, many of which are also interconnected. Multiple mechanisms may govern a single endophenotype while likewise, many endophenotypes can share similar pathways.

Current treatments for mental illnesses are directed at symptom management. Thus, we dissect these 2 mental disorders into simpler behavioral markers (Table 2) by considering their symptoms and diagnosis criteria. We also classify the endophenotypes into four broad categories: (1) Activity and Arousal, (2) Flight or Freeze, (3) Exploration and Avoidance, and (4) Responses to Appetitive Stimuli (Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of endophenotypes.

Activity and Arousal

Changes to activity and arousal levels are observed in mood and anxiety disorders. Agitation (e.g., fidgeting, pacing, and restlessness) and retardation (e.g., lethargy) represent two ends of the activity level spectrum but are often exhibited in mood and anxiety disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Individuals also tend to experience sleep disturbances, particularly insomnia, which is the result of lowered arousal thresholds (Riemann et al., 2010).

Locomotion

In mood and anxiety disorders, humans may display hypo- or hyperactivity. Hypoactive behaviors include reduced movement, staring, or selective mutism. In extreme cases, patients can develop catatonia, which if left untreated, has lethal effects (Wilcox and Reid Duffy, 2015). On the other hand, some patients with anxiety may be restless, easily distracted, and irritable. In severe cases, patients can be diagnosed with agitated depression (Park, 2018).

Locomotor behavior can be easily studied in zebrafish larvae. Various locomotion parameters such as swim speed, or detailed bout kinematics (interbout intervals and bout duration) can be obtained, both spontaneously and in response to stimuli, and may help distinguish increases or reductions in locomotion from specific defensive behaviors such as escapes and freezing (see Section “Flight or Freeze”) or sleep behavior (see Section “Sleep”) (Prober et al., 2006; Ingebretson and Masino, 2013). In zebrafish, environmental stressors such as low pH, high salt, high temperatures and water flow have been shown to cause changes in locomotion (Clark et al., 2011; De Marco et al., 2016; Vom Berg-Maurer et al., 2016; Lee et al., 2019; Cleal et al., 2020) which correlates also with the recruitment and increased synchronization of CRH neuron (part of HPA axis) activity (Vom Berg-Maurer et al., 2016). CRH neurons and downstream pituitary corticotrophs (De Marco et al., 2013, 2016) have been shown to modulate zebrafish locomotor responses to these, as well as other stimuli, however, the precise direction of locomotor change appears to be context-dependent (see Sections “Sleep,” “Light-Dark Transition,” “Freezing,” and “Other Defensive Behaviors” for elaboration).

Multiple neuropeptidergic pathways regulate locomotion in zebrafish, as studied in detail in Woods et al. (2014). In another study, galanin-producing neurons in the zebrafish preoptic hypothalamus were found to be activated by hyperosmotic stress. The ablation of these neurons resulted in significant stress-induced hyperactivity, suggesting the galanin-producing neurons are involved in the inhibition of the stress response. The HPA axis was also more active in larvae with ablated galanin-producing neurons, as indicated by higher cortisol levels (Corradi et al., 2022). In mammals, the neuropeptide galanin similarly regulates the stress response, particularly via the HPA axis to prevent overactivation which is often the case in stress-related anxiety and mood disorders (Karlsson and Holmes, 2006; Picciotto et al., 2010; Juhasz et al., 2014).

Another important regulator of zebrafish locomotor activity is the dopaminergic system. Dopamine receptor agonist apomorphine caused increased hyperactivity at low and high doses while antagonist butaclamol caused hypoactivity at high doses (Irons et al., 2013). Zebrafish have multiple dopaminergic populations that modulate locomotion (Barrios et al., 2020). Notably, dopamine plays a major role in anxiety (Zarrindast and Khakpai, 2015) and mood disorders (Belujon and Grace, 2017).

Hypoactivity or decreased locomotion could result in decreased exploratory behavior (see also Section “Exploration and Avoidance”) as the range of novel space that can be explored is reduced. In rodents (Crawley, 1985) and zebrafish (Stewart et al., 2012), decreased exploratory behavior is also frequently observed in anxiety models. Thus, the locomotion assay can be used to test the anxiety endophenotypes relating to both activity and exploration. However, locomotion can also be affected by a multiplex of causes such as muscle or nerve impairment, and can be influenced by many genetic, developmental or nutritional factors. Hence, it is not a specific endophenotype for anxiety and mood disorders and cannot be used exclusively.

Sleep

Sleep dysregulation is a common symptom in mood and anxiety disorders. Patients often face trouble maintaining sleep, resulting in early awakening, or difficulty initiating sleep, resulting in insomnia. Furthermore, sleep disturbances worsen these mental disorders, resulting in a positive feedback loop (Yang et al., 2022). Sleep-enhancing drugs are often prescribed in anxiety or mood disorders (Saletu-Zyhlarz et al., 2003; World Health Organization, 2009). Zebrafish exhibit similar sleep-like behavior primarily at night as regulated by their circadian cycle (Chiu and Prober, 2013). Like humans, zebrafish have characteristic sleep postures, increased arousal threshold during sleep as well as after sleep deprivation. Additionally, hypnotic or anesthetic drugs can be used to induce sleep and a sleep-like brain-state in both humans and zebrafish (Zhdanova et al., 2001; Leung et al., 2019). Regulation of sleep in humans and zebrafish is also conserved at the molecular level (Corradi and Filosa, 2021). The human Hypocretin Neuropeptide Precursor (HCRT) and zebrafish homolog hcrt decrease the arousal threshold when overexpressed, resulting in insomnia in humans and increased locomotion in zebrafish (Prober et al., 2006). Norepinephrine, an acute stress response hormone and neurotransmitter, has been shown to be a downstream effector of hcrt activity in zebrafish (Singh et al., 2015). Hypocretin is a neuropeptide involved directly in sleep regulation but a hyperactive hypocretin system is also linked to anxiety and depression. For instance, hypocretin activity results in wakefulness which is associated with heightened vigilance that is common in anxiety disorders (Johnson et al., 2012) while hypocretin protects against depression by promoting stress resilience (Nollet and Leman, 2013; Ji et al., 2019).

A rest/wake behavioral assay can be used to screen for drugs that modulate sleep (Prober et al., 2006; Rihel et al., 2010) in a high-throughput manner. Rihel et al. (2010) tested several classes of drugs that are used in anxiety and mood disorder treatment [e.g., serotonin, dopamine, GABA (Lydiard, 2003), epinephrine (Dooley, 2015) agonists and antagonists]. The drugs varied in their effects on different aspects of rest. For example, dopamine D2-receptor agonists reduced waking activity and increased rest while antagonists increased both waking activity and rest. Investigating drug effects on sleep might identify more specific mechanisms or treatments for mood or anxiety-induced sleep dysregulation.

The neuropeptide neuromedin U (Nmu) causes a reduction in sleep, and hyperactivity (Chiu et al., 2016). Overexpression of Nmu in larval zebrafish increased activity both in the day and at night, reduced sleep initiation and led to poorer sleep maintenance, suggesting a reduction in arousal threshold. A CRH receptor 1 (CRHR1) antagonist blocked the Nmu overexpression-induced waking hyperactivity in a dose-dependent manner. Hence, just as CRH is involved in locomotion regulation (see Section “Locomotion”), CRH also mediates the sleep and locomotion-related effects of Nmu overexpression.

There are certainly correlations between measures of sleep and locomotion (see also Cheng et al., 2022) though sleep can be distinguished from general activity using specific criteria (Prober et al., 2006). Similar to locomotion, sleep dysregulation itself is not exclusively an endophenotype for mental disorders, and needs to be considered alongside other more specific behaviors.

Light-Dark Transition

In the light-dark transition assay, zebrafish larvae are subjected to 5–30 min periods of whole-field darkness. During light periods, the larvae display low levels of activity while in the dark, they are hyperactive, which may reflect a heightened anxiety state (Irons et al., 2010; Golla et al., 2020). Chronically stressed (from 10 to 17 dpf) larval zebrafish displayed increased locomotion in response to the light-dark transition compared to non-chronically stressed counterparts (Golla et al., 2020). This light-dark transition assay was carried out in the light/dark partitioned well to simultaneously study light/dark choice (described in Section “Light/Dark Choice”) (Golla et al., 2020). However, the control and the group that had been exposed to chronic stress did not exhibit any difference in light/dark preference, suggesting a dissociation between these behaviors.

In another study, addition of anxiolytic compounds ethanol and cocaine caused a reduction in hyperactivity during dark periods while anxiogenic D-amphetamine caused an increase in hyperactivity during the dark period (Irons et al., 2010). Furthermore, increased thigmotaxis (see Section “Exploration and Avoidance”) was seen in dark periods compared to light periods even in control conditions. When anxiolytic aconitine was added, the extent of thigmotaxis behavior observed in the dark periods was reduced significantly (Ellis et al., 2012). In a recent study we observed that acute salt stress increased thigmotaxis in a light/dark choice assay (see Section “Light/Dark Choice”) relative to uniform light condition, suggesting a potential interaction of darkness with thigmotactic behavior (Cheng et al., 2022).

Monoaminergic systems have been implicated in both mood and anxiety disorders, and many antidepressants and anxiolytic drugs target monoamine function (Heninger and Charney, 1988; Tyrer and Shawcross, 1988; Delgado, 2000; Meyer et al., 2006; Liu et al., 2018). The vesicular monoamine transporter 2 (Vmat2) transports monoamines such as dopamine, serotonin, norepinephrine, and histamine into secretory vesicles for release from monoaminergic cells of the nervous system. Vmat mutants have reduced levels of the aforementioned monoamines as well as increased thigmotaxis (see also Section “Thigmotaxis”) (Sveinsdóttir et al., 2022; Wang, 2022). The vmat mutant larvae displayed a larger increase in movement immediately after dark transition but also reduced locomotion after the light was turned back on compared to wild-type larvae (Sveinsdóttir et al., 2022). However, neither dopamine agonist pramipexole nor precursor L-Dopa could rescue the immediate peak in activity after dark transition, suggesting the involvement of other monoamines or mechanisms (Sveinsdóttir et al., 2022).

While the above evidence supports the use of light-dark transitions as a measure of anxiety, there may be other mechanisms underlying dark hyperactivity that could confound the interpretation of results. For example, the hyperactivity observed in the dark could also be a sensorimotor response to environmental change. Heightened locomotion in the dark has been shown to be mediated by deep brain photoreceptors as a non-visually-mediated undirected photokinesis reflex (Fernandes et al., 2012). This reflex allows the larvae to swim to illuminated areas without having to visually detect light and orientate towards it like in phototaxis behavior. If and how this reflex relates to anxiety pathways remains to be seen. Given such alternative explanations, caution needs to be taken in interpreting this assay as a specific anxiety endophenotype.

Additionally, light of different wavelengths may also induce different behavioral responses. In a variation of the light-dark transition assay, De Marco et al. (2013, 2016) also showed that in dark-adapted larvae, blue or yellow light exposure induced hypoactivity during the period of exposure followed by recovery following light offset. Optically enhancing pituitary corticotroph activity via blue light stimulation of Beggiatoa photo-activated adenylyl cyclase (bPAC+) larvae further reduced locomotor activity, which also correlated with an increase in whole-body cortisol. Hence, blue/yellow light-induced hypoactivity can be potentiated by HPA axis activation downstream of CRH neuron activity. Further, under exposure to external stress (high temperatures and water flow), blue-light activated bPAC+ larvae exhibited enhanced locomotor activity, compared to controls in which CRH activation was solely through external stressor exposure. This means that pituitary corticotroph activity, which is downstream of CRH activity, potentiates and hence increases the magnitude of cortisol release and locomotor responses to stressors (De Marco et al., 2016). Such evidence points toward this assay as a promising anxiety endophenotype. Further, dark-adapted larvae avoided blue light and showed enhanced escape behaviors in a blue-light versus dark choice assay. Hence, HPA axis activation at multiple levels regulates diverse anxiety behaviors.

Flight or Freeze

Across all species, predator, pain, and other aversive stimuli activate withdrawal or escape reflexes, or alternatively, a freezing response (Crawford et al., 1977; Blanchard et al., 1986; Blanchard, 1997; Roelofs, 2017). Anxiety and mood disorders have been shown to modulate these reflexes, as summarized in this section.

Freezing

Similar to mammals, freezing, or prolonged immobility, is a fear response in zebrafish and suggested to be the most reliable measure of anxiety in adults (Blaser et al., 2010). In larval zebrafish, freezing in response to an electric shock has also been observed, where freezing is defined as a period of inactivity exceeding 1.99s (Duboué et al., 2017). Severing the left fasciculus retroflexus (FR) caused an increase in freezing behavior post-shock while severing the right FR did not have a significant effect on shock recovery. The FR connects the dorsal habenula to the interpeduncular nucleus. These results suggest that the left dorsal habenulo-interpeduncular pathway is responsible for recovery from a heightened fear and anxiety state (Duboué et al., 2017). The HPA axis is also involved in the freezing response. A loss-of-function mutation to the glucocorticoid receptor ablated the negative feedback glucocorticoid cortisol provides to terminate the stress response (Gjerstad et al., 2018). This results in chronic HPA activation. Adult zebrafish with the mutation freeze for longer periods when placed in a novel tank and do not habituate even on subsequent exposures to a novel tank (Ziv et al., 2013). It remains to be seen if this freezing response in adults is related to stressor-induced hypoactivity observed in larvae (e.g., in response to water flow or blue light), since both are HPA-axis mediated (De Marco et al., 2016).

Since there are underlying neural circuits involved in freezing behavior, it can be an endophenotype for anxiety. Other genetic, molecular, and neural pathways may also interact to regulate freezing in zebrafish [see Sections “Alarm (Schreckstoff) Response” and “Learned Helplessness”].

Startle Responsiveness

Startle potentiation is a phenomenon where exposure to aversive stimuli causes an increase in the startle reflex. In humans, startle potentiation can be tested by measuring electromyographic activity of the orbicularis oculi muscle during blinking when shown aversive pictures. In humans, people with higher fear or anxiety levels display stronger startle, especially toward pictures relating to their phobias (Vaidyanathan et al., 2009). Repeated startle may lead to habituation where the stimuli no longer triggers a response; however, in some subjects, deficits in habituation may occur, which correlates with an anxiety-like state. Notably, Campbell et al. (2014) found a positive correlation between anxiety sensitivity and abnormal startle habituation.

Habituation and startle potentiation (Bhandiwad et al., 2018) has also been observed in zebrafish. Predator-like stimuli typically elicits escape reflexes such as O-bends and C-starts. O-bends enable reorientation of the body 180° to the direction of swimming and is commonly observed as a response to dark flashes (whole-field darkness) (Burgess and Granato, 2007). Vibrational-acoustic, mechanosensory and looming visual stimuli typically elicit C-starts mediated by the giant Mauthner cell and its homologs, during which the body curves in a ‘C’ shape to propel itself in a different direction from the stimulus (Hatta and Korn, 1998; Kohashi and Oda, 2008; Dunn et al., 2016). Randlett et al. (2019) developed a high-throughput dark-flash habituation assay where the aversive stimuli (darkflash) is delivered repetitively at spaced intervals. They found that the larvae gradually learnt not to respond to each darkflash, thus displaying habituation. The habituation response could be dissected into several components of behavior, and these were regulated by multiple molecular mechanisms (Randlett et al., 2019).

Multiple mechanisms of C-start habituation have also been discovered (Wolman et al., 2011; Corradi and Filosa, 2021; Nelson et al., 2022). For example, the persistent form of habituation response in zebrafish that lasts 25 min to 1 h could be inhibited by N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors (NMDAR) antagonists (Roberts et al., 2011). In humans, NMDAR antagonists such as ketamine have been used as antidepressants in mood (Amidfar et al., 2019) and anxiety disorders (Taylor et al., 2018b), though the precise mechanism behind the success of NMDAR antagonist action in humans still requires research (Sattar et al., 2018). In addition, deficiency of the neuropeptide parathyroid hormone 2 (Pth2) causes increased responsiveness in larval zebrafish to vibrational startle and reduces the delay between startle and response (Anneser et al., 2022). Larval zebrafish with reduced serotonin levels in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) had increased habituation to acoustic startle, whereas optogenetic activation or a serotonin agonist was able to reduce habituation. This evidence points toward a significant role of DRN serotonin activity in startle habituation (Pantoja et al., 2016).

Fragile X syndrome arises from the silencing of the fmr1 gene, causing habituation deficiencies in humans, mice, and zebrafish (Restivo et al., 2005; Kim et al., 2014; Ethridge et al., 2016; Marquez-Legorreta et al., 2022). Fmr1 mutants displayed slower habituation to looming visual stimuli compared to wild type. Brain-wide imaging attributed this to overly-connected neural networks that failed to disconnect upon sensorimotor learning during habituation, leading to hypersensitivity in fmr1 mutants (Marquez-Legorreta et al., 2022). Overall, changes in habituation kinematics could potentially be used as an endophenotype for anxiety, although they may also reflect more general differences in sensorimotor learning circuits.

Alarm (Schreckstoff) Response

Schreckstoff is an alarm substance that is released when a fish is injured. In adult zebrafish, this substance serves to alert the shoal to danger and therefore triggers a fear response indicated by darting and freezing behavior (Speedie and Gerlai, 2008; Mathuru et al., 2012; Chia et al., 2019). Alarm response behavior has recently been reported in larval zebrafish (Jesuthasan et al., 2021). After exposure to high concentrations of Schreckstoff, 50% of larval zebrafish [slightly lower than the 70% response rate observed in adults (Chia et al., 2019)] displayed a sustained increase in immobility of up to 20 min, which may reflect a prolonged heightened anxiety state. Consistently, neuroimaging showed a prolonged change in brain state after exposure to Schreckstoff in several areas (Jesuthasan et al., 2021). Of those areas the midbrain tegmentum, norepinephrinergic locus coeruleus and serotonergic raphe nucleus have been found to be involved in anxiety disorders (Lowry et al., 2008; Spiacci et al., 2012; DeGroot et al., 2020; Morris et al., 2020).

Interestingly, after exposure to Schreckstoff, larval zebrafish display reduced startle as compared to before (Basnakova et al., 2021). The authors’ interpretation of these results is that Schreckstoff exposure caused a heightened state of anxiety in the larval fish and hence increased their expectation of danger. The fish were therefore not startled when they encountered a second shock. Alternatively, the neural pathways regulating the Schreckstoff freezing response may override or inhibit those regulating startle. Disrupting Clock gene signaling in the habenula of larval zebrafish caused abnormal circadian regulation of dopamine and serotonin, and modulated Schreckstoff responses (Basnakova et al., 2021). Hence, both the acute Schreckstoff response and its influence on subsequent behaviors could potentially be useful endophenotypes for anxiety.

Learned Helplessness

Learned helplessness is the phenomenon where animals fail to exert control over aversive scenarios, which usually occurs after they are exposed to prolonged, unavoidable aversive events (Maier and Seligman, 2016). Depressed individuals display learned helplessness to a greater extent than healthy individuals. Miller et al. (1975) found that depressed subjects were more likely to wrongly perceive skilled responding as being up to chance. In other words, they believed they were helpless when in reality, they could learn to control the outcome. Hiroto (1974) experimentally demonstrated the learned helplessness effect in humans. During training, one group was able to control exposure to an aversive noise by pushing a button while for the other pressing the button had no effect. During the test, the group that had learned that they were helpless in dealing with the trauma did not attempt to end the trauma while the other two group took action. Hence, learned helplessness results in a reduced motivation to deal with difficult situations.

Animals will initially respond vigorously to an aversive stimulus but switch to a passive coping response when the inescapability of the stimulus becomes apparent (Koolhaas et al., 1999). In 10–15 dpf larval zebrafish, passive coping can be induced by inescapable shock and is characterized by immobility (Andalman et al., 2019). Whole-brain activity mapping revealed that the ventral habenula was uniquely hyperactive in the transition to passive coping. Modeling and optogenetic manipulation of neural connectivity also showed that aversive stimuli triggered significant changes in the intra-habenula and habenula-raphe connections, suggesting that the habenula-raphe pathway is involved in modulating the passive coping response (Andalman et al., 2019). In a prior study, 3–5 weeks old late larval/juvenile zebrafish were trained to avoid electric shock by moving away from a red light, a form of avoidance learning (Lee et al., 2010). When instead exposed to inescapable shock, the fish learnt that they were helpless and hence did not move away from the red light. The study also found that disabling the dorsal habenula using tetanus toxin caused larval zebrafish to behave as if they were exposed to inescapable shock even though they could have avoided the shock (Lee et al., 2010). Both the habenula and raphe nucleus have been implicated in human depression (Matthews and Harrison, 2012; Hahn et al., 2014; Gold and Kadriu, 2019). Hence, learned helplessness is a promising endophenotype for depression in larval zebrafish.

Larval zebrafish have also been shown to reduce their motivation to act in mildly aversive contexts. While they usually swim reflexively when encountering forward-flowing visual stimuli (an adaptive response that allows them to maintain their position in water currents) they will struggle and then become passive when the visual stimuli remains forward-flowing regardless of the fish’s swim attempts (Mu et al., 2019). Through activation and ablation experiments, the authors Mu et al. (2019) showed that glial cells play a critical role in integrating information from multiple failed attempts, which they receive from norepinephrinergic circuits, leading to the behavioral switch to passiveness. Hence, both neuronal and non-neuronal populations are involved in modulating the animal’s propensity to react to unsuccessful behavior. Given the distinct mechanisms, whether this phenomenon is related to learned helplessness or is enhanced in a depressive brain state remains to be seen.

Other Defensive Behaviors

Noxious stimuli activate the nociceptive system of animals, to trigger the conscious perception of pain or aversion (Pogatzki-Zahn and Schaible, 2020). Different noxious stimuli activate distinct pathways for pain perception in humans which have been reviewed extensively by Dubin and Patapoutian (2010). In zebrafish, hypothalamic oxytocin neurons transform noxious stimuli to motor output, evoking large-angle tail bends that can be described as a defensive response (Wee et al., 2019a). Lovett-Barron et al. (2020) similarly found that oxytocin neurons as well as other neuropeptidergic populations in the preoptic hypothalamus respond to homeostatic threats such as salinity, acidity, and heat. Calcium imaging, optogenetic and ablation experiments confirmed that both hypothalamic oxytocin and CRH neurons contribute toward these defensive behaviors via downstream brainstem targets which are distinct from those mediating escape responses (Wee et al., 2019a; Lovett-Barron et al., 2020).

Anomalies in sensitivity to noxious stimuli, magnitude of reaction, and the duration taken to recover are possible endophenotypes for anxiety disorders. Notably, oxytocin has also been implicated in anxiety and pain in mammals, including humans (Grillon et al., 2013; Poisbeau et al., 2018).

Aggression can also co-occur with anxiety and depression (Sánchez and Meier, 1997; van Praag, 1998; Neumann et al., 2010; Blain-Arcaro and Vaillancourt, 2017). However, since aggression only develops after the larval stage, aggression assays have mainly been designed for older zebrafish, as summarized by Way et al. (2015). In the mirror-biting assay, aggression is measured based on how much time a zebrafish spends ‘fighting’ with its mirror image or exhibiting aggressive postures (e.g., nose touching the mirror). Juvenile zebrafish (1 month old) exhibit similar responses to various aggression-reducing compounds including caffeine and sildenafil (Gutiérrez et al., 2020). Genetic inactivation of the histamine H3 receptor (Hrh3) in adult zebrafish caused reduced aggression and heightened anxiety, likely via altered serotonergic signaling in the telencephalon and hypothalamus (Reichmann et al., 2020). This suggests a link between aggression and anxiety, although more research is needed as aggression modulation pathways have yet to be fully characterized.

Exploration and Avoidance

Avoidance and the reduction of exploratory behavior are typically observed in anxiety disorders, which will be elaborated on in the subsections below. However, these behaviors could also be modulated in depression and other mood disorders (Cella et al., 2010; Blanco et al., 2013; Haskell et al., 2020). We will focus on light/dark choice, thigmotaxis and novel tank diving, however other assays for exploration and avoidance have also been developed, as discussed in Corradi and Filosa (2021).

Light/Dark Choice

The state of anxiety often triggers an increase in avoidance when presented with an approach-avoidance conflict (AAC). An AAC occurs when two situations – one safe and one risky – are present simultaneously. A heightened anxiety state would typically cause the subject to choose the safer options over riskier unknowns (Aupperle et al., 2011). In rodent models, a choice between light and dark presents an AAC where the dark zone is perceived as safer. Imaizumi and Onodera (1993) found that the addition of anxiogenic drugs zolantidine and thioperamide increased the time spent in the dark zone. In mice, the type 2 CRF receptor neurons in the lateral septum regulate anxiety by projecting to the anterior hypothalamus. Optogenetic studies to activate these neurons had an anxiogenic effect, where mice preferred to remain in the dark side of the light/dark partitioned box (Anthony et al., 2014). These studies suggest that behavior in a light/dark test could be an indicator of anxiety.

The light/dark choice assay has also been successfully implemented in zebrafish. Larval zebrafish exhibit a natural preference for light (phototaxis) and avoidance of dark (scotophobia). Notably a behavioral switch to dark preference has been reported in adulthood (Lau et al., 2011). Larval zebrafish are placed individually in an arena with a well-lit and dark region and the proportion of time in the light area can be used to quantify anxiety (Steenbergen et al., 2011). When the larvae are exposed to stressful stimuli such as UV light, their cortisol levels and dark-avoidance behavior increase (Bai et al., 2016). This provides evidence that increased dark avoidance is an indicator of heightened stress and anxiety levels. Hence, the light/dark choice assay could be used to assess anxiety state in zebrafish. However, it is worth noting that heat stress and mechanical disturbance did not trigger the expected correlated increase in dark avoidance and cortisol levels. We show that prolonged, but not acute osmotic stress enhances dark avoidance in larvae (Cheng et al., 2022). This could suggest that different stressors and stressor durations generate distinct anxiety states that are regulated by diverse pathways.

Steenbergen et al. (2011) further validated the larval zebrafish light/dark choice assay using clinically-established anxiolytic or anxiogenic compounds. They found that larval zebrafish spent more time in the dark region when treated with buspirone, a serotonin receptor (5-HT1A) agonist, while the converse was true for anxiogenic compounds such as caffeine. Hence, serotonin signaling as well as other conserved signaling pathways may share common roles in modulating anxiety across species. The role of serotonin in regulating zebrafish anxiety has been further supported by studies in adult zebrafish using novel tank, light/dark, light/dark plus maze assays (Sackerman et al., 2010; Maximino et al., 2013a; Nowicki et al., 2014).

Recently, hypothalamic CRH neurons were shown to promote dark avoidance behavior (Wagle et al., 2022). These CRH neurons displayed lower overall activity in the presence of light, and their ablation increased brainwide representations of light and reduced dark avoidance (Wagle et al., 2022). CRH receptor antagonism or crhb knockout similarly reduced dark avoidance. In contrast, oxytocin neuron ablation enhanced dark avoidance, suggesting distinct roles of hypothalamic peptidergic neurons in light/dark choice regulation.

Additionally, dark avoidance is a form of reduction in exploratory behavior as the animals show reduced propensity to enter unfamiliar places. Such decreased exploratory behavior has been observed in anxiety models in both rodents (Crawley, 1985) and zebrafish (Stewart et al., 2012). Overall, the light/dark choice assay might be one of the more specific anxiety endophenotypes in zebrafish.

Thigmotaxis

Thigmotaxis, or wall-hugging behavior, is also conserved in many animal species. The fear of exposure to predators in open spaces causes animals to prefer staying close to the perimeter of the environment. Thigmotaxis can also be interpreted as a reduction in exploratory behavior as there is a reduction in willingness to enter the unfamiliar open space. Decreased exploratory behavior has been observed in animal models of anxiety (Crawley, 1985; Stewart et al., 2012). An open field test showed that humans with high anxiety sensitivity and agoraphobia also have a higher tendency to keep to the edges of the arena while their healthy counterparts spent most time in the center of the arena (Walz et al., 2016).

Similarly, larval zebrafish display increased thigmotaxis in stressed conditions such as in the presence of chemical irritants (Roberts et al., 2020). Therefore, a version of the open field test can be used as a measure of anxiety levels. Zebrafish larvae are placed in an arena and their movement is recorded for a set amount of time. Thigmotaxis is quantified as a percentage of the distance moved in the outer zone of the well out of the total distance moved. Anxiolytic drugs have been found to reduce thigmotaxis while anxiogenic compounds increase thigmotaxis. For example, Schnörr et al. (2012) found that diazepam, an FDA-approved drug for anxiety treatment, significantly reduced thigmotaxis. In humans, diazepam binds allosterically to gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) receptors in the limbic system, reducing the excitability of neurons, which mediates its anxiolytic effect. While the mechanism of diazepam action in zebrafish has not been precisely elucidated, diazepam has been shown to bind to GABA receptors in other species of fish (Wilkinson et al., 1983; Friedl et al., 1988).

Through selective breeding, Wagle et al. (2017) found that dark aversion and thigmotaxis are heritable traits. Fish that display strong dark avoidance also displayed increased thigmotaxis in an open field test, indicating increased propensity of being in a high anxiety state. This suggests that both thigmotaxis and dark avoidance have a genetic underpinning, making them candidate endophenotypes for anxiety.

In a recent study, we showed that acute salt stress enhances thigmotaxis in a light/dark choice assay, with thigmotaxis only during the light/dark period as compared to during uniform light. However, dark avoidance was unaffected, suggesting that dark avoidance and thigmotaxis behaviors are dissociable traits with potentially different underlying pathways. In contrast, prolonged, high salt stress enhanced dark avoidance but had weaker effects on thigmotaxis (Cheng et al., 2022).

However, thigmotaxis can also be induced by other biological phenomena such as seizures (Baraban et al., 2005), and may be influenced by experimental factors (see Section “Controls and Considerations in Endophenotype Investigation”), hence it should be interpreted with caution as an anxiety phenotype.

Novel Tank Diving

In the novel tank diving (NTD) assay, the zebrafish is placed into a narrow tank to constraint most of its movement to the vertical axis. The unfamiliar environment increases stress and anxiety, causing the zebrafish to initially dwell at the bottom of the tank. In the next 5 min, they gradually begin to explore the upper regions of the tank. NTD assays are typically conducted on adult zebrafish, but have been successfully conducted using 25 dpf larval zebrafish (Golla et al., 2020). Larval zebrafish exposed to chronic stress (net-chasing, water turbulence, salt stress, low pH, and light flashes applied randomly during the day) have increased bottom-dwelling times (Golla et al., 2020) which is in agreement with increased responsiveness to light-dark transition. However, they do not display increased thigmotaxis, dark avoidance and shoaling.

A variation of the NTD has been proposed for younger larvae. Seven dpf larvae were shown to display depth preference in the first 30 min of introduction to a novel tank, with caffeine enhancing, and diazepam alleviating bottom-dwelling behavior (Fontana and Parker, 2022).

In adults, the anxiolytics nicotine, diazepam and buspirone caused a reduction in the time spent bottom dwelling (Bencan and Levin, 2008; Bencan et al., 2009), suggesting that bottom-dwelling behavior is indicative of anxiety. The leo mutant strain has increased monoamine oxidase (MAO) activity and serotonin turnover rate, leading to increased thigmotaxis and light avoidance in adult zebrafish (Maximino et al., 2013b). In a NTD assay, leo mutant zebrafish displayed increased bottom-dwelling (Cachat et al., 2011; Maximino et al., 2013b), suggesting that there are genetic underpinnings to the NTD.

In addition to the propensity to explore novel environments, the NTD has been used to measure habituation in adult zebrafish (Wong et al., 2010). In an unfamiliar environment, time is needed for the zebrafish to acclimatize before normal behavior is resumed. As described above (see Section “Startle Responsiveness”), habituation deficits are also characteristics of anxiety-related disorders. Hence, the novel tank diving assay could be one of the more specific anxiety endophenotypes in zebrafish.

Responses to Appetitive Stimuli

Disruptions in reward pathways of the brain have been associated with mood and anxiety disorders (Ulrich-Lai et al., 2010; Russo and Nestler, 2013). Atypical responses to pleasurable stimuli such as food or positive social interaction might be an indication of dysfunctional reward signaling which could be a sign of mood or anxiety disorders, as summarized in this section.

Appetite Dysregulation

Appetite, or the drive to consume food, is often dysregulated in mood and anxiety disorders, as observed behaviorally and supported by numerous studies (Rudenga et al., 2013; Weidenfeld and Ovadia, 2017). Feeding when stressed is correlated with reduced activation in reward areas of the brain (amygdala, hippocampus, and cingulate cortex), suggesting that stress causes reduced sensitivity to the pleasurable rewards derived from food, which leads to excessive eating (Born et al., 2010). Furthermore, excessive cortisol causes strong increases in appetite. This effect has been confirmed in studies on Cushing’s syndrome patients who face prolonged exposure to glucocorticoids, resulting in significant weight gain (Fleseriu et al., 2012), as well as depression and anxiety as comorbidities (Sonino and Fava, 2001). On the other extreme, stress-induced anorexia is also commonly observed in humans and other mammals [see Section “Stress Feeding Behavior/Stress-Induced Anorexia” (Shimizu et al., 1989; Liu et al., 2007; Guarda et al., 2015)]. Hence, appetite dysregulation can be an endophenotype for mood and anxiety disorders. However, since appetite is regulated by many homeostatic and hedonic factors, it is also not a specific endophenotype.

Zebrafish larvae begin feeding by 5 dpf, often hunting small aquatic organisms such as paramecia or rotifers. While paramecia hunting assays have been commonly used to quantify zebrafish feeding, they are relatively low-throughput and require specialized imaging setups (Gahtan, 2005; McElligott and O’malley, 2005). More recently, variations of gut fluorescence feeding assays (Shimada et al., 2012; Jordi et al., 2015, 2018; Wee et al., 2019b; Cheng et al., 2022) have been used to quantify paramecia intake in individuals or groups of larval zebrafish. Larvae are starved before fluorescently-labeled paramecia are added to the arena. Gut fluorescence can be imaged continuously using customized setups (Jordi et al., 2015, 2018), or alternatively, larvae can be anesthetized or fixed after specific time windows (e.g., 15 min) and fluorescently imaged (Wee et al., 2019b,2021; Cheng et al., 2022). This feeding assay can be adapted accordingly to study more specific aspects of appetite dysregulation, such as under stress or in social contexts, as described in the next sections.

Stress Feeding Behavior/Stress-Induced Anorexia

During stress, HPA activation causes increased release of cortisol, which has been shown to stimulate hunger and increased food intake (Tataranni et al., 1996). However, there is some evidence that the type of stress (e.g., physical vs. social, traumatic vs. non-traumatic) can have different effects on appetite (Adam and Epel, 2007). While some may binge when stressed, others might have reduced food intake. In rats, restraint and forced swimming stress results in anorexic behavior (Calvez et al., 2011). Regardless of the directional change in appetite, dysregulation of eating patterns is implicated in anxiety and stress-related disorders. Moreover, there is a high comorbidity between anxiety and eating disorders (Swinbourne et al., 2012), further supporting the relationship between dysregulated appetite and anxiety.

Larval zebrafish are freshwater fish, hence high salinity can be used as a stressor. De Marco et al. (2014) found that salt stress at 50 and 100 mM NaCl suppressed feeding motivation – in normal conditions, larval zebrafish would preferentially occupy the region of the arena that contained paramecia, whereas when placed in hyperosmotic salt solutions, the larvae occupied all regions equally. Since whole body cortisol levels of the larvae increased with increasing salt concentration, the HPA axis was activated by the salt stress (De Marco et al., 2014). To improve scalability, we have adapted our gut fluorescence feeding assay to examine stress-induced anorexia (Wee et al., 2019b; Cheng et al., 2022). Using this assay and a new machine-based gut segmentation approach, we found that both acute and prolonged salt exposure reduced paramecia consumption in a dose-dependent manner (Cheng et al., 2022). Given the high homology between the zebrafish and human HPA stress axes, and the fact that appetite dysregulation has been linked to this stress pathway, stress feeding behavior could be a viable endophenotype for anxiety disorders.

When stressed, humans tend to choose caloric dense food such as sweet, high-carbohydrate and high-fat snacks (Oliver et al., 2000; Zellner et al., 2006; Adam and Epel, 2007), often leading to stress-associated weight gain. The increase in consumption of such foods also leads to reprogramming of stress pathways and behavioral changes, as detailed by Hardaway et al. (2015). For example, carbohydrate-rich food is thought to increase serotonergic activity, thus decreasing anxiety and depression (Rogers, 1995). Consumption of these different meal compositions also likely has corresponding effects on their mood and stress levels (Born et al., 2010). It remains to be seen if these phenomena can be modeled in larval zebrafish.

Social Feeding Behavior

Social facilitation of eating is the phenomenon where individuals eat more when in groups than when alone (Zajonc, 1965; Higgs and Thomas, 2016; Herman, 2017; Ruddock et al., 2019). Similar observations have been made in many animal models (Harlow, 1932; Tolman, 1964; Reynaud et al., 2015), including freshwater fish (Uematsu and Ogawa, 1975). While it may not be possible to fully model such a complex phenomenon in zebrafish, the ability to regulate feeding based on social context could be a useful indicator of mental function, with abnormal reactions to social context (e.g., reduced feeding in groups) reflecting mental ill-being (Patel and Schlundt, 2001; Brown et al., 2003).

Larval zebrafish have been shown to eat more when in groups than alone (Wee et al., 2022). Larvae at 6–8 dpf were plated either individually or in small groups for 2 h and the amount of food consumed by each zebrafish was compared. Fish density and crowding were controlled for by limiting the groups to small numbers (e.g., 3 fish) in the same-sized arena as isolated fish (see also Section “Controls and Considerations in Endophenotype Investigation”). The authors found that acutely (2 h) isolated larvae displayed reduced feeding compared to the larvae that were in groups. Notably, the odor of kin conspecifics was sufficient to enhance feeding in isolated fish. The authors identified a olfactory-subpallial-oxytocinergic pathway that integrates conspecific social cues, and showed that ablation of the oxytocinergic neurons or application of oxytocin antagonists could rescue the effects of isolation on feeding (Wee et al., 2022). This oxytocinergic circuit also promotes defensive behavior (see Section “Other Defensive Behaviors”), which is similarly modulated by conspecific social cues (Wee et al., 2022). In humans, oxytocin neurons can be activated by stressful stimuli, and have been implicated in appetite regulation, aversive responses, and social behavior (Striepens et al., 2012; Lieberwirth and Wang, 2014; Neumann and Slattery, 2016). Further, the basal forebrain (subpallial) neurons connecting olfactory to oxytocin circuits have been previously implicated in visually-mediated social orienting behaviors, and may be conserved in mammals (Stednitz et al., 2018).

Alternatively, this social feeding assay can also be used to study isolation-induced anxiety. To socially-reared larval zebrafish, isolation activates overlapping brain regions as aversive or stressful stimulus, including the oxytocin network (Wee et al., 2022), hence the isolation state may also reflect an anxiety state that affects feeding (also see Section “Controls and Considerations in Endophenotype Investigation”). Currently, the effects of chronic social isolation on zebrafish feeding are still unknown – in Drosophila, this leads to enhanced, rather than reduced food intake as well as reduced sleep (Li et al., 2021). In short, by comparing the relative effects of interventions on isolated and group feeding behavior, it may be possible to quantify both anxiety and social behavior endophenotypes.

Social Preference or Avoidance Behavior

Social avoidance is a common theme in mood and anxiety disorders – patients become more isolated and partake in fewer social activities (Coyle and Dugan, 2012; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015; Taylor et al., 2018a; Santini et al., 2020). Zebrafish, like humans, are a social species. Group behavior allows better defense against predators, more efficient hunting and improved foraging. In larvae, aggregation can be used as a proxy for social cohesion. Interestingly, younger larvae (7 dpf) have a natural tendency for mutual repulsion while older larvae (21 dpf) exhibit mutual attraction. Harpaz et al. (2021) suggests that this could be an adaptation to the lower oxygen levels in sheltered waters where younger larvae dwell. In the larval group assay, groups of 5 larvae were placed in custom arenas and aggregation was measured as the negative log of the sum of nearest-neighbor distance between each of the fish of test groups divided by that of the shuffled control groups. Disc1 mutants, previously shown to have abnormal stress responses (Eachus et al., 2017), displayed increased social cohesion in this group assay (Harpaz et al., 2021). Interestingly, mutated forms of the human homolog DISC1 protein have been found in the brains of major depression patients (Sawamura et al., 2005). Overall there is potential for the group assay to be used to study social aggregation or avoidance in the context of anxiety and mood disorders.

Social avoidance in larval zebrafish is characterized by increased frequencies of high acceleration escapes and short latency c-start swim bout types upon social interaction. Mechanosensory stimuli in the form of water vibrations generated by a piezoelectric bender actuator mimics social interaction. When these water vibrations are detected within certain proximity, the larvae will attempt to swim to avoid what they perceive to be other fish. Larvae raised in isolation will react when vibrations are detected at closer proximity (smaller avoidance distance) compared to those raised in groups. Ablation of the lateral line, through which larval zebrafish sense water motion, caused larval zebrafish raised in isolation to have similar avoidance distance as those raised in groups (Groneberg et al., 2020). In addition, mechanosensory stimuli generated by the movements of nearby fish modulates the expression of pth2. Isolation of socially-reared zebrafish larvae caused a reduction in pth2 transcripts (Anneser et al., 2020). Pth mutants not only show increased startle responsiveness (see Section “Startle Responsiveness”) but also reduced social preference and shoaling at late juvenile stages (Anneser et al., 2022).

The early growth response factor 1 (Egr1) has been implicated in depression in humans (Covington et al., 2010) and social anxiety-like behaviors in mice (Stack et al., 2010). In zebrafish, isolation induced downregulation of the egr1 transcript (Anneser et al., 2020). In a different social interaction assay - the larval dyad assay (Stednitz and Washbourne, 2020) – the loss-of-function egr1 mutant 14 dpf zebrafish spent less time near the divider between itself and another similar-size zebrafish (Tallafuss et al., 2022). Furthermore, it displayed less social orienting behavior, indicating weak social interaction behavior. In a biological motion assay (Larsch and Baier, 2018), the egr1 mutants displayed less inclination to follow projected dots that had size and movement kinematics mimicking that of a 14 dpf larva (Tallafuss et al., 2022), also supporting the hypothesis that Egr1 is essential for normal social behavior. These egr1 mutants also had fewer tyrosine hydroxylase 2 (Th2) expressing neurons in the basal forebrain. Ablation of 30% of this neuronal population in wild types yielded similar behavior as seen in the egr1 mutants. Notably, the zebrafish basal forebrain contains other cell types, such as cholinergic neurons, which have been shown to be important for social development and function, and may interact with these Th2 neurons (Stednitz et al., 2018; Wee et al., 2022).

Another variation of the social preference assay allows larvae to choose between a region where it can see conspecifics and a region where it cannot. The assay is conducted using 14–21 dpf larval zebrafish in which social preference has already developed (Dreosti et al., 2015). Larvae raised in groups displayed social interaction behavior, orienting themselves at a 45-degree angle and synchronizing their motion with conspecifics. Conversely, larvae raised in isolation were found to freeze or simply watch the conspecifics. Treatment of isolated larvae with buspirone (a 5-HT1A receptor agonist) rescued such anti-social behavior, further supporting an involvement of monoamines in the regulation of social behavior (Tunbak et al., 2020).

Overall, social avoidance or abnormal social interactions have been shown to be supported by multiple underlying circuit and hormonal mechanisms and is a candidate mood and anxiety endophenotype in larval zebrafish.

Responses to Addictive Substances

There is a high comorbidity between drug/alcohol addiction and mood or anxiety disorders (Robbins, 1974; Merikangas et al., 1998; Grant et al., 2004). The self-medication hypothesis suggests that individuals first develop mood or anxiety disorders as a result of external stressors or trauma before they turn to drug abuse to cope through a form of ‘self-medication’ (Khantzian, 1985). Over time, this develops into an addiction. Several assays have been developed for mainly late larval/juvenile (> 3–4 weeks) and adult zebrafish to study addiction behaviors, including conditioned place preference (CPP) assays where the animal associates the drug with a neutral environmental stimulus (Tzschentke, 2007; Webb et al., 2009; Mathur et al., 2011; Collier and Echevarria, 2013) and self-administration assays in which the zebrafish triggers the delivery of addictive substances by its own actions (Bossé and Peterson, 2017; Nathan et al., 2022). Müller et al. (2020) reviews these and additional assays in more detail. Addictive drugs such as nicotine, ethanol, opioids, and cannabinoids have also been shown to affect anxiety and endophenotypes in both zebrafish and mammals, such as light/dark choice and light-dark transition (Irons et al., 2010; Steenbergen et al., 2011). Conserved genes and pathways such as the dopaminergic and cholinergic systems have been shown to underlie addictive drug responses (Darland and Dowling, 2001; Ninkovic et al., 2006; Mathur and Guo, 2010). As there are many shared mechanisms between addiction behaviors and mood and anxiety disorders (Goeders, 2004; Lüthi and Lüscher, 2014), behavioral responses to addictive substances could be useful endophenotypes particularly for cases of addiction and mood/anxiety disorder comorbidity.

Controls and Considerations in Endophenotype Investigation

As described in Section “Human and Zebrafish Anxiety and Mood Endophenotypes,” multiple assays can be used to characterize endophenotypes in different mental disorders using the larval zebrafish model. For many assays, variations in specific protocols could potentially affect interpretation of results. It is therefore important to consider the choice of protocol in relation to the aims of the study. Here, beyond protocol differences, we briefly describe other variables that can affect experimental outcomes, which may need to be controlled or accounted for Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Summary of factors influencing behavioral assay outcomes.

Age

While our review focuses on larval stages, many studies have shown a strong age dependence of behaviors even within the first 4 weeks of development. For example, in an operant conditioning task, Valente et al. (2012) found that the ability to learn began at week 3 and reached the max performance index at week 6. Hence, many learned helplessness and addiction assays are conducted at later ages (Lee et al., 2010; Andalman et al., 2019; Müller et al., 2020). Similarly, larval age was found to be a factor affecting locomotion in light-dark transitions (Padilla et al., 2011). In light periods, larval zebrafish increased their locomotor activity as they developed from 4–6 dpf (Padilla et al., 2011). Age was similarly shown to affect baseline locomotion and locomotor variability in a different study (Ingebretson and Masino, 2013). Larval zebrafish are known to exhibit scotophobia in the light/dark choice assay but Lau et al. (2011) has shown that the maturation of fishes into adulthood reversed this paradigm and resulted in light avoidance behavior. Finally, age also influences social behavior. Multiple studies have shown that preference for conspecifics and social aggregation or orienting tends to occur only after 2 weeks of age (Dreosti et al., 2015; Stednitz and Washbourne, 2020; Harpaz et al., 2021), although other social behaviors emerge sooner (Wee et al., 2022).

Arena Size and Depth

Media volume used in arenas was also found to affect larval zebrafish behavior. Christou et al. (2020) reported that the distance moved and activity time of 4–7 dpf larval zebrafish increased when more media volume was used in wells. Similarly, locomotor activity was found to be affected by well depth in an age-dependent manner (Ingebretson and Masino, 2013). While Ingebretson and Masino (2013) showed that variability but not motility was affected by wells of different diameters, Padilla et al. (2011) demonstrated an effect of arena size on larvae locomotion, with larvae kept in 24-well plates exhibiting more movement than larvae kept in 48 and 96-well plates. Other behaviors such as thigmotaxis are also likely affected by arena size (Cheng et al., 2022).

Genotype/Strain

The use of different wild-type strains, such as the AB and Tüpfel long-fin (TL), can also produce variable results. In the light-dark transition assay, both strains responded differently to light-dark/dark-light transitions, with AB strains showing stronger behavioral responses (van den Bos et al., 2017). Furthermore, AB strains have been demonstrated to display stronger habituation to repeated acoustic stimuli and thus showed weaker responses when compared to TL strains (van den Bos et al., 2017). The use of nacre mutants in imaging protocols has been favored due to their low pigmentation and transparency. Differences in behavior between nacre and wild-type fishes have not been fully studied, but O’Malley et al. (2004) has shown that nacre larvae displayed locomotor behaviors identical to those of wild-type larvae. We also found that salt stress-induced anorexia differed across AB, TL and nacre genotypes, with AB fish showing the strongest reduction in feeding (Cheng et al., 2022).

Feeding State and Diet

The effects of feeding on different behavioral parameters have also been studied. Relative to unfed fish, fed 6 dpf larvae displayed differences in avoidance responses, swim speeds and resting periods, while fed 7 dpf larvae additionally displayed changes in thigmotaxis and social distancing behaviors (Clift et al., 2014). These changes in behaviors might be associated with increased energy availability in the fed state, but did not imply that the behaviors were linked (Clift et al., 2014). When fed with dry food, larvae were found to have shorter body lengths and displayed social deficits, suggesting that diet is an important factor in social behavior (Stednitz and Washbourne, 2020). Hunger state also influences feeding-related endophenotypes and threat avoidance behaviors. Filosa et al. (2016) demonstrated an effect of feeding status on the propensity to avoid an ambiguous visual stimulus representing either prey or predator, which is mediated by serotonergic raphe neurons and the HPA axis. Wee et al. (2019a,b) found that neuronal populations such as the caudal hypothalamus are convergently activated by starvation and aversive stimuli. Hence, it is possible that the feeding state can affect anxiety/depression-like behaviors via convergent neural pathways.

Environmental State

Not surprisingly, environmental context, such as water quality, temperature, noise, or water flow/agitation could similarly affect the manifestation of anxiety or depression-like behaviors, and should be avoided or controlled for in behavioral assays. As described above, a hyperosmotic environment and mechanosensory stress can increase cortisol and reduce feeding, and can themselves be utilized as a behavioral assay for stress-induced anorexia (De Marco et al., 2014; Cheng et al., 2022). Clark et al. (2011) found that hyperosmotic shock caused a dose-dependent effect on larval fish movement. However, De Marco et al. (2014) reported no changes in locomotor activity due to salt stress. Our experiments suggest that prolonged osmotic stress reduces sleep while leaving swim speed mostly intact (Cheng et al., 2022). Temperature changes (cold or hot stressors) have also been shown to affect locomotion and dark avoidance behaviors (Bai et al., 2016). Changes in pH also increased cortisol levels in fish, with cortisol levels being correlated to HCl concentration (Yeh et al., 2013).

Environmental noise levels can alter behavioral and physiological responses of larvae. Prolonged exposure to 20 dB re 1 ms–2 flat-spectrum noise caused decreased startle response thresholds and increased sensitization to startle stimuli (Bhandiwad et al., 2018). These noise-exposed larvae also displayed reduced locomotor activity (Bhandiwad et al., 2018). Water flow stimuli can also evoke an escape, avoidance, or hypoactive locomotor response in larvae, correlating with cortisol levels (McHenry et al., 2009; Olszewski et al., 2012; De Marco et al., 2016), and continuous water flow also induces rheotaxis (Olszewski et al., 2012; Suli et al., 2012; Olive et al., 2016; Oteiza et al., 2017). Hence, excessive agitation should be avoided before and during behavioral experiments.

Crowding/Fish Density

Fish density has also been found to affect zebrafish behavior. As described in Section “Other Defensive Behaviors” and “Social Feeding Behavior,” the presence or absence of conspecifics influences feeding and defensive behavior (Wee et al., 2022). Circling behavior – defined as multiple conspecifics moving in an organized manner along the edges of the arena towards one direction – was observed in dishes with dense zebrafish populations and occurred more frequently along with increased densities (Zaki et al., 2021). Furthermore, it was also shown that larvae in dense arenas preferred edges and the outer circumferences of the arena. These behaviors resemble thigmotaxis as observed in anxious zebrafish, though it is unclear if it is an anxiety phenotype. Isolation also affects larval behavioral responses. Singly-reared larvae were found to exhibit lower locomotor activity in dark periods compared to group-reared larvae (Zellner et al., 2011). Larvae raised in isolation had impaired social preference (Tunbak et al., 2020), increased social avoidance, and also swam fewer but longer bouts (Groneberg et al., 2020). Burgess and Granato (2008) reported that larvae raised in lower density had increased sensitivity to startle. Aggregation behavior is also likely affected by fish density. Similarly, feeding increases with fish density though overcrowding may counteract this effect (Wee et al., 2022). Crowding stress was affected by tank size and water volume in adult zebrafish, but studies on crowding stress have not been reported in larval zebrafish. However, holding densities of larvae were also inversely related to larval growth, with higher densities (>22 fish/L) leading to lower mean individual larvae weights (Delomas and Dabrowski, 2019). Thus, adhering to a standard raising protocol for zebrafish larvae is important to avoid complications from confounding factors.

Time of Day

MacPhail et al. (2009) tested the locomotion of larvae from 10:00 to 15:30 h in darkness using infrared and found that locomotor behavior was affected by time of day. Activity was highest when testing began and decreased to a stable level by the afternoon, with no changes being noted between 13:00 to 15:30 h (MacPhail et al., 2009). At night, larvae display lower activity and characteristics of sleep, including increased arousal thresholds (Zhdanova et al., 2001; Hurd and Cahill, 2002; Prober et al., 2006). Disturbances to sleep also affected locomotion. Mechanical stimuli applied during the last 6 h of night resulted in decreased daytime locomotor activity and increased arousal thresholds in the following day (Zhdanova et al., 2001). Larvae display stronger dark flash habituation responses in subjective night versus day phases (Randlett et al., 2019). Larvae also show circadian effects in their startle responses following alarm substance exposure (see Section “Alarm (Schreckstoff) Response”) which are abolished by habenula clock disruption (Basnakova et al., 2021).

Microbiome

The microbial status of larvae also influences their anxiety or mood-related behavior. Germ-free (GF) zebrafish exhibited increased locomotor activity and reduced thigmotactic behavior compared to conventionalized (CV) and conventionally-raised (CR) larvae (Davis et al., 2016; Weitekamp et al., 2021). Furthermore, bacterial load was found to be inversely related to locomotor activity (Weitekamp et al., 2021). However, Phelps et al. (2017) reported no difference in thigmotaxis regardless of colonization, despite GF larvae displaying hyperactivity in dark periods by 10 dpf. No locomotor differences were found between CV and CR groups despite compositional differences in the microbiota profiles (Davis et al., 2016). Microbial colonization was also found to modulate stress responses. In GF larvae, expression of genes related to stress response and cortisol levels did not increase after osmotic stress, and microbial colonization within 24 h was able to restore this cortisol response (Davis et al., 2016). Similarly, colonization of axenic larvae by 6 dpf was shown to block hyperactivity (Phelps et al., 2017). Lactobacillus plantarum colonization was also found to reduce thigmotactic behavior, though it did not significantly affect cortisol levels in response to stress (Davis et al., 2016). The microbiome has also been reported to regulate social behavior development by shaping basal forebrain development (Bruckner et al., 2020). Hence, the variation between microbial communities across different locations and zebrafish facilities (Roeselers et al., 2011) could also contribute to differences in behavioral endophenotypes observed.

Open Questions, Challenges, and Future Prospects

Despite its many advantages, there are certainly limitations of the larval zebrafish in modeling mental health endophenotypes. For instance, larval zebrafish may insufficiently capture more complex emotional, social, and cognitive phenomena as compared to adult zebrafish or even rodent or primate models. The zebrafish brain lacks a prefrontal cortex which is heavily involved in reward and emotional processing (Drevets, 2001; Keedwell et al., 2005). In addition, subcortical circuits that are largely involved in socio-emotional behavior and learning, such as the hippocampus and amygdala, may be present but not structurally well-conserved in the zebrafish model (Geng and Peterson, 2019). The midbrain dopaminergic system which is crucial for reward learning and motivational salience (Bissonette and Roesch, 2016) is also lacking in zebrafish (Schweitzer and Driever, 2009; Du et al., 2016). While some learning assays have been developed in larvae (Roberts et al., 2013; Dempsey et al., 2022) (see Sections “Learned Helplessness” and “Age”), larval zebrafish are generally less capable of higher-order cognitive processes such as learning and memory-related tasks (Lovett-Barron, 2021). Hence, the full range of endophenotypes may not be accessible in this model, and complementary models will have to be used in cases where the costs outweigh the benefits.

An important criterion for a candidate endophenotype is that it has an underlying genetic, biochemical or neural circuitry mechanism. However, many of the underlying pathways mechanisms are not exclusive and interact extensively. The HPA axis, including CRH and pituitary corticotrophs, plays a central role in regulating stress levels, sleep, locomotion and dark avoidance (De Marco et al., 2013, 2016; Wagle et al., 2022) in zebrafish. The monoamine class of neurotransmitters, including serotonin, is also involved in avoidance, arousal, and appetitive behavior endophenotypes. Habenula-raphe circuitry has been implicated both in the alarm response and learned helplessness (Lee et al., 2010; Andalman et al., 2019; Basnakova et al., 2021). Feeding, social, and defensive behaviors have been shown to be modulated by oxytocin and other hypothalamic neuropeptides (Wee et al., 2019a,2022; Landin et al., 2020; Lovett-Barron et al., 2020). Another neuropeptide, Pth, is involved in both social behavior as well as startle responsiveness (Anneser et al., 2020, 2022). When endophenotypes share an underlying pathway, it could suggest that these behaviors are related. At the same time, two behaviors with distinct pathways can be connected by interactions across these pathways. Brain and organism-wide imaging or transcriptomic approaches (Ahrens et al., 2013; Thyme et al., 2019; Marques et al., 2020), which are growing increasingly prevalent in the field, as well as other novel experimental or computational modeling techniques probing molecular, genetic, and circuit interactions (Vladimirov et al., 2018; Wanner and Vishwanathan, 2018; Tabor et al., 2019; Loring et al., 2020), will be crucial to dissecting these intricate relationships.

Recent studies have also begun to probe the relationships between mental health-related endophenotypes, by conducting different behavioral assays on the same fish or under the same contexts. For example, Blaser and Rosemberg (2012) found that light avoidance and the tank diving response in adult zebrafish are dissociable behavioral traits. In another study, van den Bos et al. (2019) showed dissociation between thigmotaxis and startle responsiveness. We have now shown similar dissociations between light/dark choice and thigmotaxis, and thigmotaxis from light-dark transition behavior or night-time startle in response to acute or prolonged osmotic stress (Cheng et al., 2022).

Further, the relationships between endophenotypes are likely context-dependent and may be influenced by factors such as those reviewed in Section “Controls and Considerations in Endophenotype Investigation.” For example, maternal stress had either correlated or dissociable effects on habituation and thigmotaxis depending on strain background (AB or TL) (van den Bos et al., 2019). In our light/dark choice experiments, salt-induced thigmotaxis was observed during the light/dark but not uniform light period, suggesting an interaction between environmental state and anxiety behavior (Cheng et al., 2022). Increasing salt or dark intensities also reduced the strength of correlations between endophenotypes. Finally, we found that osmotic stress suppressed feeding in a strain-dependent manner (Cheng et al., 2022). Importantly, research into endophenotype correlations, dissociations, and context dependence goes beyond helping us understand endophenotypes better – the convergent pathways underlying such relationships are potential novel drug targets (Kalueff et al., 2014b; Kalueff and Stewart, 2015).

In this review, we focus on behavioral endophenotypes. However, the value of biological endophenotypes in studying mental health cannot be discounted. Changes in heart rate or other cardiac parameters could be indicative of anxiety state. Mild electric shock to cause startle in larval zebrafish caused transient bradycardia while repeated shocks triggered delayed tachycardia (Mann et al., 2010). A sustained increase in heart rate was also induced by a chemical irritant (Roberts et al., 2020). Heart rate can be measured via imaging larvae embedded in agarose (Mann et al., 2010) while more complex features such as blood flow velocity and circulation can be assessed concurrently with behavioral responses (fin beating) using microfluidic set-ups (Subendran et al., 2021). Another biological endophenotype is cortisol levels that can be assayed via radioimmunoassays (Steenbergen et al., 2012) or enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays (ELISAs) (Yeh et al., 2013). The endpoint of HPA axis activation in the stress response is cortisol release. Thus, cortisol levels could be a good proxy for the stress state of the fish and have been used in the validation of behavioral endophenotypes (De Marco et al., 2014, 2016; Bai et al., 2016). However, as Bai et al. (2016) showed, there was again a dissociation between changes in cortisol levels and changes in dark avoidance behavior. When using biological endophenotypes, extra caution must be taken to ensure that changes in these biological markers were the result of changes in mood/anxiety state as opposed to off-target physiological effects. Also, behavioral assays can better distinguish between different aspects and mechanisms of anxiety or mood disorders while biological markers predominantly reflect overall stress or mood levels.

In summary, a nuanced and detailed understanding of the state-dependent relationships between endophenotypes and their underlying genetic, molecular and circuit mechanisms will be a crucial next step in leveraging the zebrafish for mental health modeling.

Author Contributions

JT wrote the manuscript with the assistance of RA. CW conceived and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by the National Research Foundation Fellowship (NRF-NRFF13-2021-0003) and the Confirma Programme from the Agency of Science, Technology and Research (A*STAR), Singapore, both awarded to CW.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ruey-Kuang Cheng & Ajay Mathuru for comments on the manuscript.

References

Adam, T. C., and Epel, E. S. (2007). Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiol. Behav. 91, 449–458. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Admon, R., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). Dysfunctional reward processing in depression. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 4, 114–118. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahrens, M. B., Orger, M. B., Robson, D. N., Li, J. M., and Keller, P. J. (2013). Whole-brain functional imaging at cellular resolution using light-sheet microscopy. Nat. Methods 10, 413–420. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Amidfar, M., Woelfer, M., Réus, G. Z., Quevedo, J., Walter, M., and Kim, Y.-K. (2019). The role of NMDA receptor in neurobiology and treatment of major depressive disorder: evidence from translational research. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 94:109668. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andalman, A. S., Burns, V. M., Lovett-Barron, M., Broxton, M., Poole, B., Yang, S. J., et al. (2019). Neuronal dynamics regulating brain and behavioral state transitions. Cell 177, 970.e–985.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.02.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angst, J., Sellar, R., and Merikangas, K. R. (2000). Depressive spectrum diagnoses. Compr. Psychiatry 41, 39–47. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(00)80007-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anneser, L., Alcantara, I. C., Gemmer, A., Mirkes, K., Ryu, S., and Schuman, E. M. (2020). The neuropeptide Pth2 dynamically senses others via mechanosensation. Nature 588, 653–657. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2988-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anneser, L., Gemmer, A., Eilers, T., Alcantara, I. C., Loos, A.-Y., Ryu, S., et al. (2022). The neuropeptide Pth2 modulates social behavior and anxiety in zebrafish. iScience 25:103868. doi: 10.1101/2021.07.28.454115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anthony, T. E., Dee, N., Bernard, A., Lerchner, W., Heintz, N., and Anderson, D. J. (2014). Control of stress-induced persistent anxiety by an extra-amygdala septohypothalamic circuit. Cell 156, 522–536. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.12.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aupperle, R. L., Sullivan, S., Melrose, A. J., Paulus, M. P., and Stein, M. B. (2011). A reverse translational approach to quantify approach-avoidance conflict in humans. Behav. Brain Res. 225, 455–463. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bai, Y., Liu, H., Huang, B., Wagle, M., and Guo, S. (2016). Identification of environmental stressors and validation of light preference as a measure of anxiety in larval zebrafish. BMC Neurosci. 17:63. doi: 10.1186/s12868-016-0298-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, G. F., and Balthazart, J. (2008). How useful is the appetitive and consummatory distinction for our understanding of the neuroendocrine control of sexual behavior? Horm. Behav. 53, 307–311. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A., and Menlove, F. L. (1968). Factors determining vicarious extinction of avoidance behavior through symbolic modeling. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 8, 99–108. doi: 10.1037/h0025260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baraban, S. C., Taylor, M. R., Castro, P. A., and Baier, H. (2005). Pentylenetetrazole induced changes in zebrafish behavior, neural activity and c-fos expression. Neurosci. 131, 759–768. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.11.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basnakova, A., Cheng, R.-K., Chia, J. S. M., D’Agostino, G., Suryadi, Tan, G. J. H., et al. (2021). The habenula clock influences response to a stressor. Neurobiol. Stress 15:100403. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrios, J. P., Wang, W.-C., England, R., Reifenberg, E., and Douglass, A. D. (2020). Hypothalamic dopamine neurons control sensorimotor behavior by modulating brainstem premotor nuclei in zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 30, 4606–4618.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belujon, P., and Grace, A. A. (2017). Dopamine system dysregulation in major depressive disorders. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 1036–1046. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyx056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bencan, Z., and Levin, E. D. (2008). The role of alpha7 and alpha4beta2 nicotinic receptors in the nicotine-induced anxiolytic effect in zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 95, 408–412. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bencan, Z., Sledge, D., and Levin, E. D. (2009). Buspirone, chlordiazepoxide and diazepam effects in a zebrafish model of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 94, 75–80. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhandiwad, A. A., Raible, D. W., Rubel, E. W., and Sisneros, J. A. (2018). Noise-induced hypersensitization of the acoustic startle response in larval zebrafish. J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol. 19, 741–752. doi: 10.1007/s10162-018-00685-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bissonette, G. B., and Roesch, M. R. (2016). Development and function of the midbrain dopamine system: what we know and what we need to. Genes Brain Behav. 15, 62–73. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blain-Arcaro, C., and Vaillancourt, T. (2017). Longitudinal associations between depression and aggression in children and adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 45, 959–970. doi: 10.1007/s10802-016-0204-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, D. C. (1997). “Stimulus, environmental, and pharmacological control of defensive behaviors,” in Proceedings of the Learning, Motivation, and Cognition: The Functional Behaviorism of Robert C Bolles, (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 283–303. doi: 10.1037/10223-014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, R. J., Flannelly, K. J., and Blanchard, D. C. (1986). Defensive behaviors of laboratory and wild Rattus norvegicus. J. Comp. Psychol. 100, 101–107. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.100.2.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanco, N. J., Otto, A. R., Maddox, W. T., Beevers, C. G., and Love, B. C. (2013). The influence of depression symptoms on exploratory decision-making. Cognition 129, 563–568. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaser, R. E., and Rosemberg, D. B. (2012). Measures of anxiety in zebrafish (Danio rerio): dissociation of black/white preference and novel tank test. PLoS One 7:e36931. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blaser, R. E., Chadwick, L., and McGinnis, G. C. (2010). Behavioral measures of anxiety in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 208, 56–62. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Born, J. M., Lemmens, S. G. T., Rutters, F., Nieuwenhuizen, A. G., Formisano, E., Goebel, R., et al. (2010). Acute stress and food-related reward activation in the brain during food choice during eating in the absence of hunger. Int. J. Obes. 34, 172–181. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bossé, G. D., and Peterson, R. T. (2017). Development of an opioid self-administration assay to study drug seeking in zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 335, 158–166. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L. S., Waller, G., Meyer, C., Bamford, B., Morrison, T., and Burditt, E. (2003). Socially driven eating and restriction in the eating disorders. Eat. Behav. 4, 221–228. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00027-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruckner, J. J., Stednitz, S. J., Grice, M. Z., Zaidan, D., Massaquoi, M. S., Larsch, J., et al. (2020). The microbiota promotes social behavior by modulating microglial remodeling of forebrain neurons. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2020.05.01.071373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buch, A. M., and Liston, C. (2021). Dissecting diagnostic heterogeneity in depression by integrating neuroimaging and genetics. Neuropsychopharmacology 46, 156–175. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00789-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, H. A., and Granato, M. (2007). Modulation of locomotor activity in larval zebrafish during light adaptation. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 2526–2539. doi: 10.1242/jeb.003939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgess, H. A., and Granato, M. (2008). The neurogenetic frontier–lessons from misbehaving zebrafish. Brief. Funct. Genomic. Proteomic. 7, 474–482. doi: 10.1093/bfgp/eln039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cachat, J. M., Canavello, P. R., Elegante, M. F., Bartels, B. K., Elkhayat, S. I., Hart, P. C., et al. (2011). “Modeling stress and anxiety in zebrafish,” in Zebrafish Models in Neurobehavioral Research, eds A. V. Kalueff and J. M. Cachat (Totowa, NJ: Humana Press), 73–88. doi: 10.1007/978-1-60761-922-2_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cachat, J., Stewart, A., Grossman, L., Gaikwad, S., Kadri, F., Chung, K. M., et al. (2010). Measuring behavioral and endocrine responses to novelty stress in adult zebrafish. Nat. Proto. 5, 1786–1799. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2010.140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvez, J., Fromentin, G., Nadkarni, N., Darcel, N., Even, P., Tomé, D., et al. (2011). Inhibition of food intake induced by acute stress in rats is due to satiation effects. Physiol. Behav. 104, 675–683. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, M. L., Gorka, S. M., McGowan, S. K., Nelson, B. D., Sarapas, C., Katz, A. C., et al. (2014). Does anxiety sensitivity correlate with startle habituation? An examination in two independent samples. Cogn. Emot. 28, 46–58. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.799062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cella, M., Dymond, S., and Cooper, A. (2010). Impaired flexible decision-making in major depressive disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 124, 207–210. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, L., Eaton, W. W., Gallo, J. J., and Nestadt, G. (2000). Understanding the heterogeneity of depression through the triad of symptoms, course and risk factors: a longitudinal, population-based study. J. Affect. Disord. 59, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(99)00132-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M., and Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 25, 215–224. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025002007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, R. C., Tan, X. M. J., Chua, K. X., Tan, J. X. C., and Wee, C. L (2022). Osmotic stress uncovers correlations and dissociations between larval zebrafish anxiety endophenotypes. Front. Mol. Neurosci. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2022.900223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chia, J. S. M., Wall, E. S., Wee, C. L., Rowland, T. A. J., Cheng, R.-K., Cheow, K., et al. (2019). Bacteria evoke alarm behaviour in zebrafish. Nat. Commun. 10:3831. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11608-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, C. N., and Prober, D. A. (2013). Regulation of zebrafish sleep and arousal states: current and prospective approaches. Front. Neural Circuits 7:58. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, C. N., Rihel, J., Lee, D. A., Singh, C., Mosser, E. A., Chen, S., et al. (2016). A zebrafish genetic screen identifies neuromedin U as a regulator of sleep/wake states. Neuron 89, 842–856. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christou, M., Kavaliauskis, A., Ropstad, E., and Fraser, T. W. K. (2020). DMSO effects larval zebrafish (Danio rerio) behavior, with additive and interaction effects when combined with positive controls. Sci. Total Environ. 709:134490. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.134490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrousos, G. P. (2009). Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 5, 374–381. doi: 10.1038/nrendo.2009.106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, K. J., Boczek, N. J., and Ekker, S. C. (2011). Stressing zebrafish for behavioral genetics. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 49–62. doi: 10.1515/RNS.2011.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleal, M., Gibbon, A., Fontana, B. D., and Parker, M. O. (2020). The importance of pH: how aquarium water is affecting behavioural responses to drug exposure in larval zebrafish. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 199:173066. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2020.173066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clift, D., Richendrfer, H., Thorn, R. J., Colwill, R. M., and Creton, R. (2014). High-throughput analysis of behavior in zebrafish larvae: effects of feeding. Zebrafish 11, 455–461. doi: 10.1089/zeb.2014.0989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collier, A. D., and Echevarria, D. J. (2013). The utility of the zebrafish model in conditioned place preference to assess the rewarding effects of drugs. Behav. Pharmacol. 24, 375–383. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e328363d14a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corradi, L., and Filosa, A. (2021). Neuromodulation and behavioral flexibility in larval zebrafish: from neurotransmitters to circuits. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 14:718951. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2021.718951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corradi, L., Bruzzone, M., Maschio, M. D., Sawamiphak, S., and Filosa, A. (2022). Hypothalamic galanin-producing neurons regulate stress in zebrafish through a peptidergic, self-inhibitory loop. Curr. Biol. 32, 1497.e–1510.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Covington, H. E. , Lobo, M. K., Maze, I., Vialou, V., Hyman, J. M., Zaman, S., et al. (2010). Antidepressant effect of optogenetic stimulation of the medial prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 30, 16082–16090. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coyle, C. E., and Dugan, E. (2012). Social isolation, loneliness and health among older adults. J. Aging Health 24, 1346–1363. doi: 10.1177/0898264312460275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, M., Masterson, F., and Wilson, D. (1977). Species-specific defense reactions in escape-from-fear situations. Anim. Learn. Behav. 5, 63–72. doi: 10.3758/bf03209133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawley, J. N. (1985). Exploratory behavior models of anxiety in mice. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 9, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(85)90030-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darland, T., and Dowling, J. E. (2001). Behavioral screening for cocaine sensitivity in mutagenized zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 11691–11696. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191380698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, D. J., Bryda, E. C., Gillespie, C. H., and Ericsson, A. C. (2016). Microbial modulation of behavior and stress responses in zebrafish larvae. Behav. Brain Res. 311, 219–227. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Marco, R. J., Groneberg, A. H., Yeh, C.-M., Castillo Ramírez, L. A., and Ryu, S. (2013). Optogenetic elevation of endogenous glucocorticoid level in larval zebrafish. Front. Neural Circuits 7:82. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Marco, R. J., Groneberg, A. H., Yeh, C.-M., Treviño, M., and Ryu, S. (2014). The behavior of larval zebrafish reveals stressor-mediated anorexia during early vertebrate development. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:367. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Marco, R. J., Thiemann, T., Groneberg, A. H., Herget, U., and Ryu, S. (2016). Optogenetically enhanced pituitary corticotroph cell activity post-stress onset causes rapid organizing effects on behaviour. Nat. Commun. 7:12620. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeGroot, S. R., Zhao-Shea, R., Chung, L., Klenowski, P. M., Sun, F., Molas, S., et al. (2020). Midbrain dopamine controls anxiety-like behavior by engaging unique interpeduncular nucleus microcircuitry. Biol. Psychiatry 88, 855–866. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado, P. L. (2000). Depression: the case for a monoamine deficiency. J. Clin. Psychiatry 61, 7–11.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Delomas, T. A., and Dabrowski, K. (2019). Improved protocol for rapid zebrafish growth without reducing reproductive performance. Aquac. Res. 50, 457–463. doi: 10.1111/are.13912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dempsey, W. P., Du, Z., Nadtochiy, A., Smith, C. D., Czajkowski, K., Andreev, A., et al. (2022). Regional synapse gain and loss accompany memory formation in larval zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci U.S.A. 119:e2107661119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2107661119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dichter, G. S., Damiano, C. A., and Allen, J. A. (2012). Reward circuitry dysfunction in psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders and genetic syndromes: animal models and clinical findings. J. Neurodev. Disord. 4:19. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dooley, T. P. (2015). Treating anxiety with either beta blockers or antiemetic antimuscarinic drugs: a review. Ment. Health Fam. Med. 11, 89–99. doi: 10.25149/1756-8358.1102013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreosti, E., Lopes, G., Kampff, A. R., and Wilson, S. W. (2015). Development of social behavior in young zebrafish. Front. Neural Circuits 9:39. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drevets, W. C. (2001). Neuroimaging and neuropathological studies of depression: implications for the cognitive-emotional features of mood disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 11, 240–249. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00203-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, Y., Guo, Q., Shan, M., Wu, Y., Huang, S., Zhao, H., et al. (2016). Spatial and temporal distribution of dopaminergic neurons during development in zebrafish. Front. Neuroanat. 10:115. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubin, A. E., and Patapoutian, A. (2010). Nociceptors: the sensors of the pain pathway. J. Clin. Invest. 120, 3760–3772. doi: 10.1172/JCI42843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duboué, E. R., Hong, E., Eldred, K. C., and Halpern, M. E. (2017). Left habenular activity attenuates fear responses in larval zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 27, 2154.e–2162.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, T. W., Gebhardt, C., Naumann, E. A., Riegler, C., Ahrens, M. B., Engert, F., et al. (2016). Neural circuits underlying visually evoked escapes in larval zebrafish. Neuron 89, 613–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eachus, H., Bright, C., Cunliffe, V. T., Placzek, M., Wood, J. D., and Watt, P. J. (2017). Disrupted-in-schizophrenia-1 is essential for normal hypothalamic-pituitary-interrenal (HPI) axis function. Hum. Mol. Genet. 26, 1992–2005. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddx076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A. J. (2013). Handbook of Approach and Avoidance Motivation. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Ellis, L. D., Seibert, J., and Soanes, K. H. (2012). Distinct models of induced hyperactivity in zebrafish larvae. Brain Res. 1449, 46–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erhardt, A., Ising, M., Unschuld, P. G., Kern, N., Lucae, S., Pütz, B., et al. (2006). Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical system in patients with panic disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 2515–2522. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ethridge, L. E., White, S. P., Mosconi, M. W., Wang, J., Byerly, M. J., and Sweeney, J. A. (2016). Reduced habituation of auditory evoked potentials indicate cortical hyper-excitability in fragile X syndrome. Transl Psychiatry 6:e787. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faravelli, C., Lo Sauro, C., Godini, L., Lelli, L., Benni, L., Pietrini, F., et al. (2012). Childhood stressful events, HPA axis and anxiety disorders. World J. Psychiatry 2, 13–25. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v2.i1.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feder, A., Coplan, J. D., Goetz, R. R., Mathew, S. J., Pine, D. S., Dahl, R. E., et al. (2004). Twenty-four-hour cortisol secretion patterns in prepubertal children with anxiety or depressive disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandes, A. M., Fero, K., Arrenberg, A. B., Bergeron, S. A., Driever, W., and Burgess, H. A. (2012). Deep brain photoreceptors control light-seeking behavior in zebrafish larvae. Curr. Biol. 22, 2042–2047. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filosa, A., Barker, A. J., Dal Maschio, M., and Baier, H. (2016). Feeding state modulates behavioral choice and processing of prey stimuli in the zebrafish tectum. Neuron 90, 596–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleseriu, M., Biller, B. M. K., Findling, J. W., Molitch, M. E., Schteingart, D. E., Gross, C., et al. (2012). Mifepristone, a glucocorticoid receptor antagonist, produces clinical and metabolic benefits in patients with cushing’s syndrome. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 97, 2039–2049. doi: 10.1210/jc.2011-3350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontana, B. D., and Parker, M. O. (2022). A high-throughput test of anxiety for zebrafish (Danio rerio) larvae: the larval diving response (LDR). bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2022.05.16.492196v2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedl, W., Hebebrand, J., Rabe, S., and Propping, P. (1988). Phylogenetic conservation of the benzodiazepine binding sites: pharmacological evidence. Neuropharmacology 27, 163–170. doi: 10.1016/0028-3908(88)90166-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gahtan, E. (2005). Visual prey capture in larval zebrafish is controlled by identified reticulospinal neurons downstream of the tectum. J. Neurosci. 25, 9294–9303. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2678-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geng, Y., and Peterson, R. T. (2019). The zebrafish subcortical social brain as a model for studying social behavior disorders. Dis. Model. Mech. 12:dmm039446. doi: 10.1242/dmm.039446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gjerstad, J. K., Lightman, S. L., and Spiga, F. (2018). Role of glucocorticoid negative feedback in the regulation of HPA axis pulsatility. Stress 21, 403–416. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2018.1470238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godos, J., Currenti, W., Angelino, D., Mena, P., Castellano, S., Caraci, F., et al. (2020). Diet and mental health: review of the recent updates on molecular mechanisms. Antioxidants (Basel) 9:346. doi: 10.3390/antiox9040346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goeders, N. E. (2004). Stress, motivation, and drug addiction. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 13, 33–35. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301009.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, P. W., and Kadriu, B. (2019). A major role for the lateral habenula in depressive illness: physiologic and molecular mechanisms. Front. Psychiatry 10:320. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00320

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golla, A., Østby, H., and Kermen, F. (2020). Chronic unpredictable stress induces anxiety-like behaviors in young zebrafish. Sci. Rep. 10:10339. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-67182-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottesman, I. I., and Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottesman, I. I., and Shields, J. (1967). Schizophrenia and Genetics: A Twin Study Vantage Point. New York, NY: Academic Press, 349–365.

Google Scholar

Gottesman, I. I., and Shields, J. (1973). Genetic theorizing and schizophrenia. Br. J. Psychiatry 122, 15–30. doi: 10.1192/bjp.122.1.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, B. F., Stinson, F. S., Dawson, D. A., Patricia Chou, S., Dufour, M. C., Compton, W., et al. (2004). Prevalence and co-occurrence of substance use disorders and independentmood and anxiety disorders. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 61:807. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grillon, C., Krimsky, M., Charney, D. R., Vytal, K., Ernst, M., and Cornwell, B. (2013). Oxytocin increases anxiety to unpredictable threat. Mol. Psychiatry 18, 958–960. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Groneberg, A. H., Marques, J. C., Martins, A. L., Diez Del Corral, R., de Polavieja, G. G., and Orger, M. B. (2020). Early-life social experience shapes social avoidance reactions in larval zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 30, 4009.e–4021.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guarda, A. S., Schreyer, C. C., Boersma, G. J., Tamashiro, K. L., and Moran, T. H. (2015). Anorexia nervosa as a motivated behavior: relevance of anxiety, stress, fear and learning. Physiol. Behav. 152, 466–472. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez, H. C., Vacca, I., Schoenmacker, G., Cleal, M., Tochwin, A., O’Connor, B., et al. (2020). Screening for drugs to reduce zebrafish aggression identifies caffeine and sildenafil. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 30, 17–29. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2019.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn, A., Haeusler, D., Kraus, C., Höflich, A. S., Kranz, G. S., Baldinger, P., et al. (2014). Attenuated serotonin transporter association between dorsal raphe and ventral striatum in major depression. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 3857–3866. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardaway, J. A., Crowley, N. A., Bulik, C. M., and Kash, T. L. (2015). Integrated circuits and molecular components for stress and feeding: implications for eating disorders. Genes Brain Behav. 14, 85–97. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harlow, H. F. (1932). Social facilitation of feeding in the albino rat. J. Genet. Psychol. 41, 211–221. doi: 10.1080/08856559.1932.9944151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harpaz, R., Aspiras, A. C., Chambule, S., Tseng, S., Bind, M.-A., Engert, F., et al. (2021). Collective behavior emerges from genetically controlled simple behavioral motifs in zebrafish. Sci. Adv. 7:eabi7460. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abi7460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haskell, A. M., Britton, P. C., and Servatius, R. J. (2020). Toward an assessment of escape/avoidance coping in depression. Behav. Brain Res. 381:112363. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatta, K., and Korn, H. (1998). Physiological properties of the mauthner system in the adult zebrafish. J. Comp. Neurol. 395, 493–509. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19980615)395:4<493::aid-cne6<3.0.co;2-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heninger, G. R., and Charney, D. S. (1988). Monoamine receptor systems and anxiety disorders. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 11, 309–326. doi: 10.1016/s0193-953x(18)30499-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herman, C. P. (2017). The social facilitation of eating or the facilitation of social eating? J. Eat. Disord. 5:16. doi: 10.1186/s40337-017-0146-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgs, S., and Thomas, J. M. (2016). Social influences on eating. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 9, 1–6. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-75388-1_27-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiroto, D. S. (1974). Locus of control and learned helplessness. J. Exp. Psychol. 102, 187–193. doi: 10.1037/h0035910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoebel, B. G. (1997). Neuroscience and appetitive behavior research: 25 years. Appetite 29, 119–133. doi: 10.1006/appe.1997.0126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoenig, K., Hochrein, A., Quednow, B. B., Maier, W., and Wagner, M. (2005). Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1153–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., Baker, M., Harris, T., and Stephenson, D. (2015). Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howe, K., Clark, M. D., Torroja, C. F., Torrance, J., Berthelot, C., Muffato, M., et al. (2013). The zebrafish reference genome sequence and its relationship to the human genome. Nature 496, 498–503. doi: 10.1038/nature12111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurd, M. W., and Cahill, G. M. (2002). Entraining signals initiate behavioral circadian rhythmicity in larval zebrafish. J. Biol. Rhythms 17, 307–314. doi: 10.1177/074873002129002618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyman, S. E. (2010). The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 155–179. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imaizumi, M., and Onodera, K. (1993). The behavioral and biochemical effects of thioperamide, a histamine H3-receptor antagonist, in a light/dark test measuring anxiety in mice. Life Sci. 53, 1675–1683. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(93)90204-g

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingebretson, J. J., and Masino, M. A. (2013). Quantification of locomotor activity in larval zebrafish: considerations for the design of high-throughput behavioral studies. Front. Neural Circuits 7:109. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irons, T. D., Kelly, P. E., Hunter, D. L., Macphail, R. C., and Padilla, S. (2013). Acute administration of dopaminergic drugs has differential effects on locomotion in larval zebrafish. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 103, 792–813. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irons, T. D., MacPhail, R. C., Hunter, D. L., and Padilla, S. (2010). Acute neuroactive drug exposures alter locomotor activity in larval zebrafish. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 32, 84–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2009.04.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jesuthasan, S., Krishnan, S., Cheng, R.-K., and Mathuru, A. (2021). Neural correlates of state transitions elicited by a chemosensory danger cue. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 111:110110. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.110110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ji, M.-J., Zhang, X.-Y., Chen, Z., Wang, J.-J., and Zhu, J.-N. (2019). Orexin prevents depressive-like behavior by promoting stress resilience. Mol. Psychiatry 24, 282–293. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0127-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joëls, M., Karst, H., and Sarabdjitsingh, R. A. (2018). The stressed brain of humans and rodents. Acta Physiol. 223:e13066. doi: 10.1111/apha.13066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, P. L., Molosh, A., Fitz, S. D., Truitt, W. A., and Shekhar, A. (2012). Orexin, stress, and anxiety/panic states. Prog. Brain Res. 198, 133–161. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-59489-1.00009-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordi, J., Guggiana-Nilo, D., Bolton, A. D., Prabha, S., Ballotti, K., Herrera, K., et al. (2018). High-throughput screening for selective appetite modulators: a multibehavioral and translational drug discovery strategy. Sci. Adv. 4:eaav1966. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav1966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordi, J., Guggiana-Nilo, D., Soucy, E., Song, E. Y., Lei Wee, C., and Engert, F. (2015). A high-throughput assay for quantifying appetite and digestive dynamics. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 309, R345–R357. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00225.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Juhasz, G., Hullam, G., Eszlari, N., Gonda, X., Antal, P., Anderson, I. M., et al. (2014). Brain galanin system genes interact with life stresses in depression-related phenotypes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, E1666–E1673. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1403649111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V., and Stewart, A. M. (2015). Modeling neuropsychiatric spectra to empower translational biological psychiatry. Behav. Brain Res. 276, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.01.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V., Stewart, A. M., and Gerlai, R. (2014a). Zebrafish as an emerging model for studying complex brain disorders. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 35, 63–75. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2013.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V., Stewart, A. M., and Gottesman, I. I. (2014b). Rethinking CNS disorders: time for new drug targets? Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 35, 491–492. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2014.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karlsson, R.-M., and Holmes, A. (2006). Galanin as a modulator of anxiety and depression and a therapeutic target for affective disease. Amino Acids 31, 231–239. doi: 10.1007/s00726-006-0336-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keedwell, P. A., Andrew, C., Williams, S. C. R., Brammer, M. J., and Phillips, M. L. (2005). The neural correlates of anhedonia in major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 843–853. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, C. L. M. (2002). The mental health continuum: from languishing to flourishing in life. J. Health Soc. Behav. 43, 207–222. doi: 10.2307/3090197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khantzian, E. J. (1985). The self-medication hypothesis of addictive disorders: focus on heroin and cocaine dependence. Am. J. Psychiatry 142, 1259–1264. doi: 10.1176/ajp.142.11.1259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, L., He, L., Maaswinkel, H., Zhu, L., Sirotkin, H., and Weng, W. (2014). Anxiety, hyperactivity and stereotypy in a zebrafish model of fragile X syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 40–49. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohashi, T., and Oda, Y. (2008). Initiation of mauthner- or non-mauthner-mediated fast escape evoked by different modes of sensory input. J. Neurosci. 28, 10641–10653. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1435-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van Der Vegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 925–935. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00026-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupfer, D. J., and Regier, D. A. (2011). Neuroscience, clinical evidence, and the future of psychiatric classification in DSM-5. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 672–674. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landin, J., Hovey, D., Xu, B., Lagman, D., Zettergren, A., Larhammar, D., et al. (2020). Oxytocin receptors regulate social preference in zebrafish. Sci. Rep. 10:5435. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-61073-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., and McTeague, L. M. (2009). The anxiety disorder spectrum: fear imagery, physiological reactivity, and differential diagnosis. Anxiety Stress Coping 22, 5–25. doi: 10.1080/10615800802478247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., McTeague, L. M., and Bradley, M. M. (2014). Pathological anxiety and function/dysfunction in the brain’s fear/defense circuitry. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 32, 63–77. doi: 10.3233/rnn-139012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larsch, J., and Baier, H. (2018). Biological motion as an innate perceptual mechanism driving social affiliation. Curr. Biol. 28, 3523.e–3532.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, B. Y. B., Mathur, P., Gould, G. G., and Guo, S. (2011). Identification of a brain center whose activity discriminates a choice behavior in zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 2581–2586. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018275108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. (2012). Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73, 653–676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E. (2021). What emotions might be like in other animals. Curr. Biol. 31, R824–R829. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, A., Mathuru, A. S., Teh, C., Kibat, C., Korzh, V., Penney, T. B., et al. (2010). The habenula prevents helpless behavior in larval zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 20, 2211–2216. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H. B., Schwab, T. L., Sigafoos, A. N., Gauerke, J. L., Krug, R. G. II, Serres, M. R., et al. (2019). Novel zebrafish behavioral assay to identify modifiers of the rapid, nongenomic stress response. Genes Brain Behav. 18:e12549. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M., Aggen, S. H., Otowa, T., Castelao, E., Preisig, M., Grabe, H. J., et al. (2016). Assessment and characterization of phenotypic heterogeneity of anxiety disorders across five large cohorts. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 25, 255–266. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leung, L. C., Wang, G. X., Madelaine, R., Skariah, G., Kawakami, K., Deisseroth, K., et al. (2019). Neural signatures of sleep in zebrafish. Nature 571, 198–204. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1336-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Wang, Z., Syed, S., Lyu, C., Lincoln, S., O’Neil, J., et al. (2021). Chronic social isolation signals starvation and reduces sleep in Drosophila. Nature 597, 239–244. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03837-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberwirth, C., and Wang, Z. (2014). Social bonding: regulation by neuropeptides. Front. Neurosci. 8:171. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Garza, J. C., Truong, H. V., Henschel, J., Zhang, W., and Lu, X.-Y. (2007). The melanocortinergic pathway is rapidly recruited by emotional stress and contributes to stress-induced anorexia and anxiety-like behavior. Endocrinology 148, 5531–5540. doi: 10.1210/en.2007-0745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, R. T. (2017). The microbiome as a novel paradigm in studying stress and mental health. Am. Psychol. 72, 655–667. doi: 10.1037/amp0000058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., Zhao, J., and Guo, W. (2018). Emotional roles of mono-aminergic neurotransmitters in major depressive disorder and anxiety disorders. Front. Psychol. 9:2201. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loring, M. D., Thomson, E. E., and Naumann, E. A. (2020). Whole-brain interactions underlying zebrafish behavior. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 65, 88–99. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2020.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovett-Barron, M. (2021). Learning-dependent neuronal activity across the larval zebrafish brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 67, 42–49. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2020.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovett-Barron, M., Chen, R., Bradbury, S., Andalman, A. S., Wagle, M., Guo, S., et al. (2020). Multiple convergent hypothalamus–brainstem circuits drive defensive behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 959–967. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0655-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowry, C. A., Hale, M. W., Evans, A. K., Heerkens, J., Staub, D. R., Gasser, P. J., et al. (2008). Serotonergic systems, anxiety, and affective disorder: focus on the dorsomedial part of the dorsal raphe nucleus. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1148, 86–94. doi: 10.1196/annals.1410.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lüthi, A., and Lüscher, C. (2014). Pathological circuit function underlying addiction and anxiety disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1635–1643. doi: 10.1038/nn.3849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lydiard, R. B. (2003). The role of GABA in anxiety disorders. J. Clin. Psychiatry 64, 21–27.

Google Scholar

MacPhail, R. C., Brooks, J., Hunter, D. L., Padnos, B., Irons, T. D., and Padilla, S. (2009). Locomotion in larval zebrafish: influence of time of day, lighting and ethanol. Neurotoxicology 30, 52–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2008.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, S. F., and Seligman, M. E. P. (2016). Learned helplessness at fifty: insights from neuroscience. Psychol. Rev. 123, 349–367. doi: 10.1037/rev0000033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, K. D., Hoyt, C., Feldman, S., Blunt, L., Raymond, A., and Page-McCaw, P. S. (2010). Cardiac response to startle stimuli in larval zebrafish: sympathetic and parasympathetic components. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 298, R1288–R1297. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00302.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marques, J. C., Li, M., Schaak, D., Robson, D. N., and Li, J. M. (2020). Internal state dynamics shape brainwide activity and foraging behaviour. Nature 577, 239–243. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1858-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marquez-Legorreta, E., Constantin, L., Piber, M., Favre-Bulle, I. A., Taylor, M. A., Blevins, A. S., et al. (2022). Brain-wide visual habituation networks in wild type and fmr1 zebrafish. Nat. Commun. 13:895. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-28299-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, E. I., Ressler, K. J., Binder, E., and Nemeroff, C. B. (2010). The neurobiology of anxiety disorders: brain imaging, genetics, and psychoneuroendocrinology. Clin. Lab. Med. 30, 865–891. doi: 10.1016/j.cll.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathur, P., and Guo, S. (2010). Use of zebrafish as a model to understand mechanisms of addiction and complex neurobehavioral phenotypes. Neurobiol. Dis. 40, 66–72. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2010.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathur, P., Lau, B., and Guo, S. (2011). Conditioned place preference behavior in zebrafish. Nat. Protoc. 6, 338–345. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2010.201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathuru, A. S., Kibat, C., Cheong, W. F., Shui, G., Wenk, M. R., Friedrich, R. W., et al. (2012). Chondroitin fragments are odorants that trigger fear behavior in fish. Curr. Biol. 22, 538–544. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, P. R., and Harrison, P. J. (2012). A morphometric, immunohistochemical, and in situ hybridization study of the dorsal raphe nucleus in major depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and suicide. J. Affect. Disord. 137, 125–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.10.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Meinerz, D. L., Fontana, B. D., Mezzomo, N. J., Stefanello, F. V., de, S., et al. (2018). Extending the analysis of zebrafish behavioral endophenotypes for modeling psychiatric disorders: fear conditioning to conspecific alarm response. Behav. Processes 149, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Puty, B., Benzecry, R., Araújo, J., Lima, M. G., de Jesus Oliveira Batista, E., et al. (2013a). Role of serotonin in zebrafish (Danio rerio) anxiety: relationship with serotonin levels and effect of buspirone, WAY 100635, SB 224289, fluoxetine and para-chlorophenylalanine (pCPA) in two behavioral models. Neuropharmacology 71, 83–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Puty, B., Matos Oliveira, K. R., and Herculano, A. M. (2013b). Behavioral and neurochemical changes in the zebrafish leopard strain. Genes Brain Behav. 12, 576–582. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McElligott, M. B., and O’malley, D. M. (2005). Prey tracking by larval zebrafish: axial kinematics and visual control. Brain Behav. Evol. 66, 177–196. doi: 10.1159/000087158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHenry, M. J., Feitl, K. E., Strother, J. A., and Van Trump, W. J. (2009). Larval zebrafish rapidly sense the water flow of a predator’s strike. Biol. Lett. 5, 477–479. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McTeague, L. M., and Lang, P. J. (2012). The anxiety spectrum and the reflex physiology of defense: from circumscribed fear to broad distress. Depress. Anxiety 29, 264–281. doi: 10.1002/da.21891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merikangas, K. R., Mehta, R. L., Molnar, B. E., Walters, E. E., Swendsen, J. D., Aguilar-Gaziola, S., et al. (1998). Comorbidity of substance use disorders with mood and anxiety disorders: results of the international consortium in psychiatric epidemiology. Addict. Behav. 23, 893–907. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(98)00076-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, J. H., Ginovart, N., Boovariwala, A., Sagrati, S., Hussey, D., Garcia, A., et al. (2006). Elevated monoamine oxidase a levels in the brain: an explanation for the monoamine imbalance of major depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 1209–1216. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, W. R., Seligman, M. E., and Kurlander, H. M. (1975). Learned helplessness, depression, and anxiety. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 161, 347–357.

Google Scholar

Misslin, R. (2003). The defense system of fear: behavior and neurocircuitry. Neurophysiol. Clin. 33, 55–66. doi: 10.1016/s0987-7053(03)00009-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, L. S., McCall, J. G., Charney, D. S., and Murrough, J. W. (2020). The role of the locus coeruleus in the generation of pathological anxiety. Brain Neurosci. Adv. 4:2398212820930321. doi: 10.1177/2398212820930321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mu, Y., Bennett, D. V., Rubinov, M., Narayan, S., Yang, C.-T., Tanimoto, M., et al. (2019). Glia accumulate evidence that actions are futile and suppress unsuccessful behavior. Cell 178, 27.e–43.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.05.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, T. E., Fontana, B. D., Bertoncello, K. T., Franscescon, F., Mezzomo, N. J., Canzian, J., et al. (2020). Understanding the neurobiological effects of drug abuse: lessons from zebrafish models. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 100:109873. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2020.109873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munck, A., Guyre, P. M., and Holbrook, N. J. (1984). Physiological functions of glucocorticoids in stress and their relation to pharmacological actions. Endocr. Rev. 5, 25–44. doi: 10.1210/edrv-5-1-25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nandam, L. S., Brazel, M., Zhou, M., and Jhaveri, D. J. (2019). Cortisol and major depressive disorder-translating findings from humans to animal models and back. Front. Psychiatry 10:974. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00974

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nandi, A., Beard, J. R., and Galea, S. (2009). Epidemiologic heterogeneity of common mood and anxiety disorders over the lifecourse in the general population: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 9:31. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-9-31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nathan, F. M., Kibat, C., Goel, T., Stewart, J., Claridge-Chang, A., and Mathuru, A. S. (2022). Contingent stimulus delivery assay for zebrafish reveals a role for CCSER1 in alcohol preference. Addict. Biol. 27:e13126. doi: 10.1111/adb.13126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, J. C., Shoenhard, H. M., and Granato, M. (2022). Pharmacogenetic and whole-brain activity analyses uncover integration of distinct molecular and circuit programs that drive learning. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2022.02.05.479242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, I. D., and Slattery, D. A. (2016). Oxytocin in general anxiety and social fear: a translational approach. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 213–221. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neumann, I. D., Veenema, A. H., and Beiderbeck, D. I. (2010). Aggression and anxiety: social context and neurobiological links. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:12. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ninkovic, J., Folchert, A., Makhankov, Y. V., Neuhauss, S. C. F., Sillaber, I., Straehle, U., et al. (2006). Genetic identification of AChE as a positive modulator of addiction to the psychostimulant D-amphetamine in zebrafish. J. Neurobiol. 66, 463–475. doi: 10.1002/neu.20231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nollet, M., and Leman, S. (2013). Role of orexin in the pathophysiology of depression: potential for pharmacological intervention. CNS Drugs 27, 411–422. doi: 10.1007/s40263-013-0064-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowicki, M., Tran, S., Muraleetharan, A., Markovic, S., and Gerlai, R. (2014). Serotonin antagonists induce anxiolytic and anxiogenic-like behavior in zebrafish in a receptor-subtype dependent manner. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 126, 170–180. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Malley, D. M., Sankrithi, N. S., Borla, M. A., Parker, S., Banden, S., Gahtan, E., et al. (2004). Optical physiology and locomotor behaviors of wild-type and nacre zebrafish. Methods Cell Biol. 76, 261–284. doi: 10.1016/s0091-679x(04)76013-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Neil, A., Quirk, S. E., Housden, S., Brennan, S. L., Williams, L. J., Pasco, J. A., et al. (2014). Relationship between diet and mental health in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Am. J. Public Health 104, e31–e42. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olive, R., Wolf, S., Dubreuil, A., Bormuth, V., Debrégeas, G., and Candelier, R. (2016). Rheotaxis of larval zebrafish: behavioral study of a multi-sensory process. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:14. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, G., Wardle, J., and Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom. Med. 62, 853–865. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olszewski, J., Haehnel, M., Taguchi, M., and Liao, J. C. (2012). Zebrafish larvae exhibit rheotaxis and can escape a continuous suction source using their lateral line. PLoS One 7:e36661. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oteiza, P., Odstrcil, I., Lauder, G., Portugues, R., and Engert, F. (2017). A novel mechanism for mechanosensory-based rheotaxis in larval zebrafish. Nature 547, 445–448. doi: 10.1038/nature23014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padilla, S., Hunter, D. L., Padnos, B., Frady, S., and MacPhail, R. C. (2011). Assessing locomotor activity in larval zebrafish: influence of extrinsic and intrinsic variables. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 33, 624–630. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantoja, C., Hoagland, A., Carroll, E. C., Karalis, V., Conner, A., and Isacoff, E. Y. (2016). Neuromodulatory regulation of behavioral individuality in zebrafish. Neuron 91, 587–601. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, Y.-M. (2018). The mixed-features specifier of major depressive disorder in DSM-5: is it practical? Psychiatry Investig. 15, 1009–1010. doi: 10.30773/pi.2018.11.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parwani, A., Duncan, E. J., Bartlett, E., Madonick, S. H., Efferen, T. R., Rajan, R., et al. (2000). Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 662–669.

Google Scholar

Patel, K. A., and Schlundt, D. G. (2001). Impact of moods and social context on eating behavior. Appetite 36, 111–118. doi: 10.1006/appe.2000.0385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, D., Brinkman, N. E., Keely, S. P., Anneken, E. M., Catron, T. R., Betancourt, D., et al. (2017). Microbial colonization is required for normal neurobehavioral development in zebrafish. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-10517-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picciotto, M. R., Brabant, C., Einstein, E. B., Kamens, H. M., and Neugebauer, N. M. (2010). Effects of galanin on monoaminergic systems and HPA axis: potential mechanisms underlying the effects of galanin on addiction- and stress-related behaviors. Brain Res. 1314, 206–218. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., and Fava, M. (2008). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 76–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pogatzki-Zahn, E., and Schaible, H.-G. (2020). “Neurotrophins and Pain,” in The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, ed. B. Fritzsch (Cambridge, MA: Elsevier, Academic Press), 142–162. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-809324-5.24139-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poisbeau, P., Grinevich, V., and Charlet, A. (2018). Oxytocin signaling in pain: cellular, circuit, system, and behavioral levels. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 35, 193–211. doi: 10.1007/7854_2017_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prober, D. A., Rihel, J., Onah, A. A., Sung, R.-J., and Schier, A. F. (2006). Hypocretin/orexin overexpression induces an insomnia-like phenotype in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 26, 13400–13410. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4332-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Proulx, C. D., Aronson, S., Milivojevic, D., Molina, C., Loi, A., Monk, B., et al. (2018). A neural pathway controlling motivation to exert effort. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 5792–5797. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1801837115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randlett, O., Haesemeyer, M., Forkin, G., Shoenhard, H., Schier, A. F., Engert, F., et al. (2019). Distributed plasticity drives visual habituation learning in larval zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 29, 1337.e–1345.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reichmann, F., Rimmer, N., Tilley, C. A., Dalla Vecchia, E., Pinion, J., Al Oustah, A., et al. (2020). The zebrafish histamine H3 receptor modulates aggression, neural activity and forebrain functional connectivity. Acta Physiol. 230:e13543. doi: 10.1111/apha.13543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restivo, L., Ferrari, F., Passino, E., Sgobio, C., Bock, J., Oostra, B. A., et al. (2005). Enriched environment promotes behavioral and morphological recovery in a mouse model for the fragile X syndrome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 11557–11562. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504984102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynaud, A. J., Guedj, C., Hadj-Bouziane, F., Meunier, M., and Monfardini, E. (2015). Social facilitation of cognition in rhesus monkeys: audience Vs. Coaction. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:328. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riemann, D., Spiegelhalder, K., Feige, B., Voderholzer, U., Berger, M., Perlis, M., et al. (2010). The hyperarousal model of insomnia: a review of the concept and its evidence. Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 19–31. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rihel, J., Prober, D. A., Arvanites, A., Lam, K., Zimmerman, S., Jang, S., et al. (2010). Zebrafish behavioral profiling links drugs to biological targets and rest/wake regulation. Science 327, 348–351. doi: 10.1126/science.1183090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ripke, S., O’Dushlaine, C., Chambert, K., Moran, J. L., Kähler, A. K., Akterin, S., et al. (2013). Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nat. Genet. 45, 1150–1159. doi: 10.1038/ng.2742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, P. R. (1974). Depression and drug addiction. Psychiatr. Q. 48, 374–386. doi: 10.1007/bf01562160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A. C., Bill, B. R., and Glanzman, D. L. (2013). Learning and memory in zebrafish larvae. Front. Neural Circuits 7:126. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A. C., Alzagatiti, J. B., Ly, D. T., Chornak, J. M., Ma, Y., Razee, A., et al. (2020). Induction of short-term sensitization by an aversive chemical stimulus in zebrafish larvae. eNeuro 7:ENEURO.0336-19.2020. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0336-19.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, A. C., Reichl, J., Song, M. Y., Dearinger, A. D., Moridzadeh, N., Lu, E. D., et al. (2011). Habituation of the C-start response in larval zebrafish exhibits several distinct phases and sensitivity to NMDA receptor blockade. PLoS One 6:e29132. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, K. (2017). Freeze for action: neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 372:20160206. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roeselers, G., Mittge, E. K., Stephens, W. Z., Parichy, D. M., Cavanaugh, C. M., Guillemin, K., et al. (2011). Evidence for a core gut microbiota in the zebrafish. ISME J. 5, 1595–1608. doi: 10.1038/ismej.2011.38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, P. J. (1995). Food, mood and appetite. Nutr. Res. Rev. 8, 243–269.

Google Scholar

Rosen, J. B., and Schulkin, J. (1998). From normal fear to pathological anxiety. Psychol. Rev. 105, 325–350. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.105.2.325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruddock, H. K., Brunstrom, J. M., Vartanian, L. R., and Higgs, S. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the social facilitation of eating. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 110, 842–861. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudenga, K. J., Sinha, R., and Small, D. M. (2013). Acute stress potentiates brain response to milkshake as a function of body weight and chronic stress. Int. J. Obes. 37, 309–316. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, J. A., and Mehrabian, A. (1978). Approach-avoidance and affiliation as functions of the emotion-eliciting quality of an environment. Environ. Behav. 10, 355–387. doi: 10.1177/0013916578103005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, S. J., and Nestler, E. J. (2013). Erratum: the brain reward circuitry in mood disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 736–736. doi: 10.1038/nrn3589

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sackerman, J., Donegan, J. J., Cunningham, C. S., Nguyen, N. N., Lawless, K., Long, A., et al. (2010). zebrafish behavior in novel environments: effects of acute exposure to anxiolytic compounds and choice of Danio rerio line. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 23, 43–61.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Saletu-Zyhlarz, G. M., Anderer, P., Arnold, O., and Saletu, B. (2003). Confirmation of the neurophysiologically predicted therapeutic effects of trazodone on its target symptoms depression, anxiety and insomnia by postmarketing clinical studies with a controlled-release formulation in depressed outpatients. Neuropsychobiology 48, 194–208. doi: 10.1159/000074638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez, C., and Meier, E. (1997). Behavioral profiles of SSRIs in animal models of depression, anxiety and aggression. Psychopharmacology 129, 197–205. doi: 10.1007/s002130050181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santini, Z. I., Jose, P. E., York Cornwell, E., Koyanagi, A., Nielsen, L., Hinrichsen, C., et al. (2020). Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and symptoms of depression and anxiety among older Americans (NSHAP): a longitudinal mediation analysis. Lancet Public Health 5, e62–e70. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(19)30230-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santomauro, D. F., Mantilla Herrera, A. M., Shadid, J., Zheng, P., Ashbaugh, C., Pigott, D. M., et al. (2021). Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet 398, 1700–1712. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)02143-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sattar, Y., Wilson, J., Khan, A. M., Adnan, M., Azzopardi Larios, D., Shrestha, S., et al. (2018). A review of the mechanism of antagonism of n-methyl-d-aspartate receptor by ketamine in treatment-resistant depression. Cureus 10:e2652. doi: 10.7759/cureus.2652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sawamura, N., Sawamura-Yamamoto, T., Ozeki, Y., Ross, C. A., and Sawa, A. (2005). A form of DISC1 enriched in nucleus: altered subcellular distribution in orbitofrontal cortex in psychosis and substance/alcohol abuse. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 1187–1192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0406543102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnörr, S. J., Steenbergen, P. J., Richardson, M. K., and Champagne, D. L. (2012). Measuring thigmotaxis in larval zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 228, 367–374. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweitzer, J., and Driever, W. (2009). “Development of the dopamine systems in zebrafish,” in Development and Engineering of Dopamine Neurons, eds R. J. Pasterkamp, M. P. Smidt, and J. P. H. Burbach (New York, NY: Springer New York), 1–14. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-0322-8_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimada, Y., Hirano, M., Nishimura, Y., and Tanaka, T. (2012). A high-throughput fluorescence-based assay system for appetite-regulating gene and drug screening. PLoS One 7:e52549. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimizu, N., Oomura, Y., and Kai, Y. (1989). Stress-induced anorexia in rats mediated by serotonergic mechanisms in the hypothalamus. Physiol. Behav. 46, 835–841. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90045-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, E. H., and Balsam, P. D. (2016). The behavioral neuroscience of motivation: an overview of concepts, measures, and translational applications. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 27, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/7854_2015_402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, C., Oikonomou, G., and Prober, D. A. (2015). Norepinephrine is required to promote wakefulness and for hypocretin-induced arousal in zebrafish. Elife 4:e07000. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skonieczna-Żydecka, K., Marlicz, W., Misera, A., Koulaouzidis, A., and Łoniewski, I. (2018). Microbiome—the missing link in the gut-brain axis: focus on its role in gastrointestinal and mental health. J. Clin. Med. 7:521. doi: 10.3390/jcm7120521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonino, N., and Fava, G. A. (2001). Psychiatric disorders associated with cushing’s syndrome. CNS Drugs 15, 361–373. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200115050-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speedie, N., and Gerlai, R. (2008). Alarm substance induced behavioral responses in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 188, 168–177. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.10.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiacci, A., Coimbra, N. C., and Zangrossi, H. (2012). Differential involvement of dorsal raphe subnuclei in the regulation of anxiety- and panic-related defensive behaviors. Neuroscience 227, 350–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.09.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stack, A., Carrier, N., Dietz, D., Hollis, F., Sorenson, J., and Kabbaj, M. (2010). Sex differences in social interaction in rats: role of the immediate-early gene zif268. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 570–580. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stednitz, S. J., and Washbourne, P. (2020). Rapid progressive social development of zebrafish. Zebrafish 17, 11–17. doi: 10.1089/zeb.2019.1815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stednitz, S. J., McDermott, E. M., Ncube, D., Tallafuss, A., Eisen, J. S., and Washbourne, P. (2018). Forebrain control of behaviorally driven social orienting in zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 28, 2445.e–2451.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenbergen, P. J., Metz, J. R., Flik, G., Richardson, M. K., and Champagne, D. L. (2012). Methods to quantify basal and stress-induced cortisol response in larval zebrafish. Neuromethods 66, 121–141. doi: 10.1007/978-1-61779-597-8_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenbergen, P. J., Richardson, M. K., and Champagne, D. L. (2011). Patterns of avoidance behaviours in the light/dark preference test in young juvenile zebrafish: a pharmacological study. Behav. Brain Res. 222, 15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternson, S. M. (2013). Hypothalamic survival circuits: blueprints for purposive behaviors. Neuron 77, 810–824. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A. M., Braubach, O., Spitsbergen, J., Gerlai, R., and Kalueff, A. V. (2014). Zebrafish models for translational neuroscience research: from tank to bedside. Trends Neurosci. 37, 264–278. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A. M., Ullmann, J. F. P., Norton, W. H. J., Parker, M. O., Brennan, C. H., Gerlai, R., et al. (2015). Molecular psychiatry of zebrafish. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 2–17. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A., Gaikwad, S., Kyzar, E., Green, J., Roth, A., and Kalueff, A. V. (2012). Modeling anxiety using adult zebrafish: a conceptual review. Neuropharmacology 62, 135–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Striepens, N., Scheele, D., Kendrick, K. M., Becker, B., Schäfer, L., Schwalba, K., et al. (2012). Oxytocin facilitates protective responses to aversive social stimuli in males. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 18144–18149. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208852109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Subendran, S., Wang, Y.-C., Lu, Y.-H., and Chen, C.-Y. (2021). The evaluation of zebrafish cardiovascular and behavioral functions through microfluidics. Sci. Rep. 11:13801. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93078-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suli, A., Watson, G. M., Rubel, E. W., and Raible, D. W. (2012). Rheotaxis in larval zebrafish is mediated by lateral line mechanosensory hair cells. PLoS One 7:e29727. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sveinsdóttir, H. S., Decker, A., Christensen, C., Lucena, P. B., porsteinsson, H., Richert, E., et al. (2022). Motility phenotype in a zebrafish vmat2 mutant. PLoS One 17:e0259753. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0259753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swerdlow, N. R., Paulsen, J., Braff, D. L., Butters, N., Geyer, M. A., and Swenson, M. R. (1995). Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 58, 192–200. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.58.2.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swinbourne, J., Hunt, C., Abbott, M., Russell, J., St Clare, T., and Touyz, S. (2012). The comorbidity between eating disorders and anxiety disorders: prevalence in an eating disorder sample and anxiety disorder sample. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 46, 118–131. doi: 10.1177/0004867411432071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabor, K. M., Marquart, G. D., Hurt, C., Smith, T. S., Geoca, A. K., Bhandiwad, A. A., et al. (2019). Brain-wide cellular resolution imaging of Cre transgenic zebrafish lines for functional circuit-mapping. Elife 8:e42687. doi: 10.7554/eLife.42687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallafuss, A., Stednitz, S. J., Voeun, M., Levichev, A., Larsch, J., Eisen, J., et al. (2022). Egr1 is necessary for forebrain dopaminergic signaling during social behavior. eNeuro 9. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0035-22.2022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tataranni, P. A., Larson, D. E., Snitker, S., Young, J. B., Flatt, J. P., and Ravussin, E. (1996). Effects of glucocorticoids on energy metabolism and food intake in humans. Am. J. Physiol. 271, E317–E325. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.1996.271.2.E317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. H., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., Coughlin, C., Mulqueen, J., Johnson, J. A., Gabriel, D., et al. (2018b). Ketamine for social anxiety disorder: a randomized, placebo-controlled crossover trial. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 325–333. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, H. O., Taylor, R. J., Nguyen, A. W., and Chatters, L. (2018a). Social isolation, depression, and psychological distress among older adults. J. Aging Health 30, 229–246. doi: 10.1177/0898264316673511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thyme, S. B., Pieper, L. M., Li, E. H., Pandey, S., Wang, Y., Morris, N. S., et al. (2019). Phenotypic landscape of schizophrenia-associated genes defines candidates and their shared functions. Cell 177, 478.e–491.e. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.01.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tolman, C. W. (1964). Social facilitation of feeding behaviour in the domestic chick. Anim. Behav. 12, 245–251. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(64)90008-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsigos, C., Kyrou, I., Kassi, E., and Chrousos, G. P. (2020). “Stress: endocrine physiology and pathophysiology,” in Endotext, eds K. R. Feingold, B. Anawalt, A. Boyce, G. Chrousos, W. W. de Herder, and K. Dhatariya (South Dartmouth, MA: MDText.com, INC).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tunbak, H., Vazquez-Prada, M., Ryan, T. M., Kampff, A. R., and Dreosti, E. (2020). Whole-brain mapping of socially isolated zebrafish reveals that lonely fish are not loners. eLife 9:e55863. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyrer, P., and Shawcross, C. (1988). Monoamine oxidase inhibitors in anxiety disorders. J. Psychiatr. Res. 22, 87–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(88)90070-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzschentke, T. M. (2007). Measuring reward with the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm: update of the last decade. Addict. Biol. 12, 227–462. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2007.00070.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uematsu, T., and Ogawa, S. (1975). Social facilitation of feeding behavior in fresh-water fish. (3) the himedaka, Oryzias latipes. Ann. Anim. Psychol. 25, 57–64. doi: 10.2502/janip1944.25.57

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., Christiansen, A. M., Ostrander, M. M., Jones, A. A., Jones, K. R., Choi, D. C., et al. (2010). Pleasurable behaviors reduce stress via brain reward pathways. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 20529–20534. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007740107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaidyanathan, U., Patrick, C. J., and Bernat, E. M. (2009). Startle reflex potentiation during aversive picture viewing as an indicator of trait fear. Psychophysiology 46, 75–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00751.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valente, A., Huang, K.-H., Portugues, R., and Engert, F. (2012). Ontogeny of classical and operant learning behaviors in zebrafish. Learn. Mem. 19, 170–177. doi: 10.1101/lm.025668.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Beveren, N. J. M., Buitendijk, G. H. S., Swagemakers, S., Krab, L. C., Röder, C., de Haan, L., et al. (2012). Marked reduction of AKT1 expression and deregulation of AKT1-associated pathways in peripheral blood mononuclear cells of schizophrenia patients. PLoS One 7:e32618. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0032618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bos, R., Mes, W., Galligani, P., Heil, A., Zethof, J., Flik, G., et al. (2017). Further characterisation of differences between TL and AB zebrafish (Danio rerio): gene expression, physiology and behaviour at day 5 of the larval stage. PLoS One 12:e0175420. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bos, R., Althuizen, J., Tschigg, K., Bomert, M., Zethof, J., Filk, G., et al. (2019). Early life exposure to cortisol in zebrafish (Danio rerio): similarities and differences in behaviour and physiology between larvae of the AB and TL strains. Behav. Pharmacol. 30, 260–271. doi: 10.1097/fbp.0000000000000470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praag, H. M. (1998). Anxiety and increased aggression as pacemakers of depression. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. Suppl. 393, 81–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1998.tb05971.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vladimirov, N., Wang, C., Höckendorf, B., Pujala, A., Tanimoto, M., Mu, Y., et al. (2018). Brain-wide circuit interrogation at the cellular level guided by online analysis of neuronal function. Nat. Methods 15, 1117–1125. doi: 10.1038/s41592-018-0221-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vom Berg-Maurer, C. M., Trivedi, C. A., Bollmann, J. H., De Marco, R. J., and Ryu, S. (2016). The severity of acute stress is represented by increased synchronous activity and recruitment of hypothalamic CRH neurons. J. Neurosci. 36, 3350–3362. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3390-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagle, M., Nguyen, J., Lee, S., Zaitlen, N., and Guo, S. (2017). Heritable natural variation of an anxiety-like behavior in larval zebrafish. J. Neurogenet. 31, 138–148. doi: 10.1080/01677063.2017.1343827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagle, M., Zarei, M. Lovett-Barron, M., Poston, K. T., Xu, J., Ramey, V., et al. (2022). Brain-wide perception of the emotional valence of light is regulated by distinct hypothalamic neurons. Mol. Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/s41380-022-01567-x [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walz, N., Mühlberger, A., and Pauli, P. (2016). A human open field test reveals thigmotaxis related to agoraphobic fear. Biol. Psychiatry 80, 390–397. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H. (2022). Roles of vesicular monoamine transporter 2 in neuronal development and histaminergic signalling—insights from zebrafish. Acta Physiol. 234:e13739. doi: 10.1111/apha.13739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wanner, A. A., and Vishwanathan, A. (2018). Methods for mapping neuronal activity to synaptic connectivity: lessons from larval zebrafish. Front. Neural Circuits 12:89. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2018.00089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Way, G. P., Ruhl, N., Snekser, J. L., Kiesel, A. L., and McRobert, S. P. (2015). A comparison of methodologies to test aggression in zebrafish. Zebrafish 12, 144–151. doi: 10.1089/zeb.2014.1025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, K. J., Norton, W. H., Trümbach, D., Meijer, A. H., Ninkovic, J., Topp, S., et al. (2009). Zebrafish reward mutants reveal novel transcripts mediating the behavioral effects of amphetamine. Genome Biol. 10:R81. doi: 10.1186/gb-2009-10-7-r81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wee, C. L., Kunes, S., and Song, E. (2021). Quantification of Food Intake. Available online at: https://bio-protocol.org/prep1116 (accessed March 5, 2022).

Google Scholar

Wee, C. L., Nikitchenko, M., Wang, W.-C., Luks-Morgan, S. J., Song, E., Gagnon, J. A., et al. (2019a). Zebrafish oxytocin neurons drive nocifensive behavior via brainstem premotor targets. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1477–1492. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0452-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wee, C. L., Song, E. Y., Johnson, R. E., Ailani, D., Randlett, O., Kim, J.-Y., et al. (2019b). A bidirectional network for appetite control in larval zebrafish. Elife 8:e43775. doi: 10.7554/eLife.43775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wee, C. L., Song, E., Nikitchenko, M., Wong, S., Engert, F., and Kunes, S. (2022). Social isolation modulates appetite and defensive behavior via a common oxytocinergic circuit in larval zebrafish. Nat. Commun. 13:2573. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-29765-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weidenfeld, J., and Ovadia, H. (2017). “The role of the amygdala in regulating the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis,” in The Amygdala – Where Emotions Shape Perception, Learning and Memories, ed. B. Ferry (London: IntechOpen), 173–186. doi: 10.5772/67828

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weitekamp, C. A., Kvasnicka, A., Keely, S. P., Brinkman, N. E., Howey, X. M., Gaballah, S., et al. (2021). Monoassociation with bacterial isolates reveals the role of colonization, community complexity and abundance on locomotor behavior in larval zebrafish. Anim. Microbiome 3:12. doi: 10.1186/s42523-020-00069-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilcox, J. A., and Reid Duffy, P. (2015). The syndrome of catatonia. Behav. Sci. 5, 576–588. doi: 10.3390/bs5040576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilkinson, M., Wilkinson, D. A., Khan, I., and Crim, L. W. (1983). Benzodiazepine receptors in fish brain: [3H]-flunitrazepam binding and modulatory effects of GABA in rainbow trout. Brain Res. Bull. 10, 301–303. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(83)90094-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolman, M. A., Jain, R. A., Liss, L., and Granato, M. (2011). Chemical modulation of memory formation in larval zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 15468–15473. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107156108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, K., Elegante, M., Bartels, B., Elkhayat, S., Tien, D., Roy, S., et al. (2010). Analyzing habituation responses to novelty in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 208, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, I. G., Schoppik, D., Shi, V. J., Zimmerman, S., Coleman, H. A., Greenwood, J., et al. (2014). Neuropeptidergic signaling partitions arousal behaviors in zebrafish. J. Neurosci. 34, 3142–3160. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3529-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2009). Pharmacological Treatment of Mental Disorders in Primary Health Care. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Yang, F. N., Liu, T. T., and Wang, Z. (2022). Functional connectome mediates the association between sleep disturbance and mental health in preadolescence: a longitudinal mediation study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 43, 2041–2050. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeh, C.-M., Glöck, M., and Ryu, S. (2013). An optimized whole-body cortisol quantification method for assessing stress levels in larval zebrafish. PLoS One 8:e79406. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0079406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science 149, 269–274.

Google Scholar

Zaki, H., Lushi, E., and Severi, K. E. (2021). Larval zebrafish exhibit collective circulation in confined spaces. Front. Phys. 9:678600. doi: 10.3389/fphy.2021.678600

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarrindast, M.-R., and Khakpai, F. (2015). The modulatory role of dopamine in anxiety-like behavior. Arch. Iran. Med. 18, 591–603.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Zellner, D. A., Loaiza, S., Gonzalez, Z., Pita, J., Morales, J., Pecora, D., et al. (2006). Food selection changes under stress. Physiol. Behav. 87, 789–793. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zellner, D., Padnos, B., Hunter, D. L., MacPhail, R. C., and Padilla, S. (2011). Rearing conditions differentially affect the locomotor behavior of larval zebrafish, but not their response to valproate-induced developmental neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 33, 674–679. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhdanova, I. V., Wang, S. Y., Leclair, O. U., and Danilova, N. P. (2001). Melatonin promotes sleep-like state in zebrafish. Brain Res. 903, 263–268. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02444-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziv, L., Muto, A., Schoonheim, P. J., Meijsing, S. H., Strasser, D., Ingraham, H. A., et al. (2013). An affective disorder in zebrafish with mutation of the glucocorticoid receptor. Mol. Psychiatry 18, 681–691. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: zebrafish, stress, anxiety, depression, mental health, endophenotype, neuropsychiatric disorders

Citation: Tan JXM, Ang RJW and Wee CL (2022) Larval Zebrafish as a Model for Mechanistic Discovery in Mental Health. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 15:900213. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2022.900213

Received: 20 March 2022; Accepted: 25 April 2022;
Published: 24 June 2022.

Edited by:

Claire Russell, Royal Veterinary College (RVC), United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Mahendra Wagle, University of California, San Francisco, United States
Soojin Ryu, University of Exeter, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Tan, Ang and Wee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Caroline Lei Wee, weel@imcb.a-star.edu.sg

Download