Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
Restricted access Research articles

Individual-learning ability predicts social-foraging strategy in house sparrows

Edith Katsnelson

Edith Katsnelson

Department of Zoology, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel

[email protected]

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

,
Uzi Motro

Uzi Motro

Department of Evolution, Systematics and Ecology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 91904, Israel

Department of Statistics, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 91904, Israel

The Center for Rationality, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem 91904, Israel

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

,
Marcus W. Feldman

Marcus W. Feldman

Department of Biology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

and
Arnon Lotem

Arnon Lotem

Department of Zoology, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv 69978, Israel

Google Scholar

Find this author on PubMed

Published:https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1151

    Social foragers can use either a ‘producer’ strategy, which involves searching for food, or a ‘scrounger’ strategy, which involves joining others' food discoveries. While producers rely on personal information and past experience, we may ask whether the tendency to forage as a producer is related to being a better learner. To answer this question, we hand-raised house sparrow (Passer domesticus) nestlings that upon independence were given an individual-learning task that required them to associate colour signal and food presence. Following the testing phase, all fledglings were released into a shared aviary, and their social-foraging tendencies were measured. We found a significant positive correlation between individual's performance in the individual-learning task and subsequent tendency to use searching (producing) behaviour. Individual-learning score was negatively correlated with initial fear of the test apparatus and with body weight. However, the correlation between individual learning and searching remained significant after controlling for these variables. Since it was measured before the birds entered a social group, individual-learning ability could not be the outcome of being a producer. However, the two traits may be initially associated, or individual learning could facilitate producing behaviour. To our knowledge, this is the first evidence that associates individual-learning abilities with social-foraging strategies in animal groups.

    References

    • 1
      Slater P. J. B. . 1981 Individual differences in animal behavior. Perspectives in ethology (eds , Bateson P. P. G.& Klopfer P. H. ), pp. 35–49. New York, NY: Plenum. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
    • 2
      Clark A. B.& Ehlinger T. J. . 1987 Pattern and adaptation in individual behavioral differences. Perspectives in ethology (eds , Bateson P. P. G.& Klopfer P. H. ), pp. 1–47. New York, NY: Plenum Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
    • 3
      Stamps J. A. . 1991 Why evolutionary issues are reviving interest in proximate behavioral mechanisms. Am. Zool. 31, 338–348. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
    • 4
      Wilson D. S., Clark A. B., Coleman K.& Dearstyne T. . 1994 Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends Ecol. Evol. 9, 442–446. Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 5
      Wilson D. S. . 1998 Adaptive individual differences within single populations. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 353, 199–205.doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0202 (doi:10.1098/rstb.1998.0202). Link, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 6
      Sih A., Bell A. M., Johnson J. C.& Ziemba R. E. . 2004 Behavioral syndromes: an integrative overview. Q. Rev. Biol. 79, 241–277.doi: 10.1086/422893 (doi:10.1086/422893). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 7
      Groothuis T. G. G.& Carere C. . 2005 Avian personalities: characterization and epigenesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 137–150.doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010 (doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 8
      Reale D., Reader S. M., Sol D., McDougall P. T.& Dingemanse N. J. . 2007 Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol. Rev. 82, 291–318.doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 9
      Sih A.& Bell A. M. . 2008 Insights for behavioral ecology from behavioral syndromes. Advances in the study of behavior (eds , Brockman H. J., Roper T. J., Naguib M., Wynne-Edwards K. E., Barnard C.& Mitani J. C. ), vol. 38, pp. 227–281. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press Inc. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
    • 10
      Dall S. R. X., Houston A. I.& McNamara J. M. . 2004 The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecol. Lett. 7, 734–739.doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00618.x (doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00618.x). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 11
      McElreath R.& Strimling P. . 2006 How noisy information and individual asymmetries can make ‘personality’ an adaptation: a simple model. Anim. Behav. 72, 1135–1139.doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.001 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.001). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 12
      Wolf M., van Doorn G. S., Leimar O.& Weissing F. J. . 2007 Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal personalities. Nature 447, 581–584.doi: 10.1038/nature05835 (doi:10.1038/nature05835). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 13
      Wolf M., van Doorn G. S.& Weissing F. J. . 2008 Evolutionary emergence of responsive and unresponsive personalities. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 15 825–15 830.doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805473105 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0805473105). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 14
      Bateson P. . 1979 How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for. Anim. Behav. 27, 470–486.doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90184-2 (doi:10.1016/0003-3472(79)90184-2). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 15
      Nelson D. A.& Marler P. . 1993 Innate recognition of song in white-crowned sparrows—a role in selective vocal learning. Anim. Behav. 46, 806–808.doi: 10.1006/anbe.1993.1258 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1993.1258). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 16
      Laland K. N.& Janik V. M. . 2006 The animal cultures debate. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 542–547.doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.06.005 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2006.06.005). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 17
      Laland K. N. . 2004 Social learning strategies. Learn. Behav. 32, 4–14. Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 18
      Boogert N. J., Reader S. M.& Laland K. N. . 2006 The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings. Anim. Behav. 72, 1229–1239.doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.02.021 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.02.021). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 19
      Boogert N. J., Reader S. M., Hoppitt W.& Laland K. N. . 2008 The origin and spread of innovations in starlings. Anim. Behav. 75, 1509–1518.doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.033 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.033). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 20
      Liker A.& Bokony V. . 2009 Larger groups are more successful in innovative problem solving in house sparrows. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 7893–7898.doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900042106 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0900042106). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 21
      Mery F., Belay A. T., So A. K. C., Sokolowski M. B.& Kawecki T. J. . 2007 Natural polymorphism affecting learning and memory in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 13 051–13 055.doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702923104 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0702923104). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 22
      Matzel L. D., Townsend D. A., Grossman H., Han Y. R., Hale G., Zappulla M., Light K.& Kolata S. . 2006 Exploration in outbred mice covaries with general learning abilities irrespective of stress reactivity, emotionality, and physical attributes. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 86, 228–240.doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.03.004 (doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2006.03.004). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 23
      Matzel L. D., Han Y. R., Grossman H., Karnik M. S., Patel D., Scott N., Specht S. M.& Gandhi C. C. . 2003 Individual differences in the expression of a ‘general’ learning ability in mice. J. Neurosci. 23, 6423–6433. Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 24
      Barnard C. J.& Sibly R. M. . 1981 Producers and scroungers—a general-model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows. Anim. Behav. 29, 543–550.doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80117-0 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80117-0). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 25
      Bell A. M. . 2007 Future directions in behavioural syndromes research. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 755–761.doi: 10.1098/rspb.2006.0199 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.0199). Link, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 26
      Giraldeau L. A.& Caraco T. . 2000 Social foraging theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
    • 27
      Giraldeau L. A.& Dubois F. . 2008 Social foraging and the study of exploitative behavior. Advances in the study of behavior (eds , Brockman H. J., Roper T. J., Naguib M., Wynne-Edwards K. E., Barnard C.& Mitani J. C. ), vol. 38, pp. 59–104. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press Inc. Google Scholar
    • 28
      Kurvers R. H. J. M., Prins H. H. T., van Wieren S. E., van Oers K., Nolet B. A.& Ydenberg R. C. . 2010 The effect of personality on social foraging: shy barnacle geese scrounge more. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 601–608.doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.1474 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1474). Link, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 29
      Beauchamp G. . 2006 Phenotypic correlates of scrounging behavior in zebra finches: role of foraging efficiency and dominance. Ethology 112, 873–878.doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01241.x (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01241.x). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 30
      Liker A.& Barta Z. . 2002 The effects of dominance on social foraging tactic use in house sparrows. Behaviour 139, 1061–1076.doi: 10.1163/15685390260337903 (doi:10.1163/15685390260337903). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 31
      Marchetti C.& Drent P. J. . 2000 Individual differences in the use of social information in foraging by captive great tits. Anim. Behav. 60, 131–140.doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1443 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1443). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 32
      Giraldeau L. A.& Lefebvre L. . 1986 Exchangeable producer and scrounger roles in a captive flock of feral pigeons—a case for the skill pool effect. Anim. Behav. 34, 797–803.doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80064-1 (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80064-1). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 33
      Beauchamp G. . 2001 Consistency and flexibility in the scrounging behaviour of zebra finches. Can. J. Zool. 79, 540–544.doi: 10.1139/cjz-79-3-540 (doi:10.1139/cjz-79-3-540). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 34
      Morand-Ferron J., Giraldeau L. A.& Lefebvre L. . 2007 Wild carib grackles play a producer–scrounger game. Behav. Ecol. 18, 916–921.doi: 10.1093/beheco/arm058 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arm058). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 35
      Morand-Ferron J.& Giraldeau L. A. . 2010 Learning behaviorally stable solutions to producer–scrounger games. Behav. Ecol. 21, 343–348.doi: 10.1093/beheco/arp195 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arp195). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 36
      Giraldeau L. A., Valone T. J.& Templeton J. J. . 2002 Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 357, 1559–1566.doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1065 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1065). Link, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 37
      Kameda T.& Nakanishi D. . 2002 Cost-benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment—an evolutionary simulation and an experiment with human subjects. Evol. Hum. Behav. 23, 373–393.doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00101-0 (doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00101-0). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 38
      Dall S. R. X., Giraldeau L. A., Olsson O., McNamara J. M.& Stephens D. W. . 2005 Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 187–193.doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2005.01.010). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 39
      Bicca-Marques J. C.& Garber P. A. . 2005 Use of social and ecological information in tamarin foraging decisions. Int. J. Primatol. 26, 1321–1344.doi: 10.1007/s10764-005-8855-9 (doi:10.1007/s10764-005-8855-9). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 40
      Katsnelson E., Motro U., Feldman M. W.& Lotem A. . 2008 Early experience affects producer–scrounger foraging tendencies in the house sparrow. Anim. Behav. 75, 1465–1472.doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.020 (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.020). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 41
      Belmaker A. . 2007 Learning to choose between foraging strategies in adult house sparrows. MS thesis, Tel-Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel. [in Hebrew.]. Google Scholar
    • 42
      Gionfriddo J. P.& Best L. B. . 1996 Grit color selection by house sparrows and northern bobwhites. J. Wildl. Manage. 60, 836–842. Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 43
      Coolen I. . 2002 Increasing foraging group size increases scrounger use and reduces searching efficiency in nutmeg mannikins (Lonchura punctulata). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 52, 232–238.doi: 10.1007/s00265-002-0500-4 (doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0500-4). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 44
      Sokal R. R.& Rohlf F. J. . 1981 Biometry, 2nd edn. San Francisco, CA: Freeman and Company. Google Scholar
    • 45
      Sheskin D. J. . 2004 Handbook of parametric and nonparametric statistical procedures, 3rd edn, p. 1071. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Google Scholar
    • 46
      Seferta A., Guay P. J., Marzinotto E.& Lefebvre L. . 2001 Learning differences between feral pigeons and zenaida doves: the role of neophobia and human proximity. Ethology 107, 281–293.doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00658.x (doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00658.x). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 47
      Webster S. J.& Lefebvre L. . 2001 Problem solving and neophobia in a columbiform–passeriform assemblage in Barbados. Anim. Behav. 62, 23–32.doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1725 (doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1725). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 48
      Greenberg R. . 2003 The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds. Animal innovation (eds , Reader S. M.& Laland K. N. ), pp. 175–196. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. CrossrefGoogle Scholar
    • 49
      Cassady J. C.& Johnson R. E. . 2002 Cognitive test anxiety and academic performance. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 27, 270–295.doi: 10.1006/ceps.2001.1094 (doi:10.1006/ceps.2001.1094). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 50
      Spencer K. A., Buchanan K. A., Goldsmith A. R.& Catchpole C. K. . 2003 Song as an honest signal of developmental stress in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm. Behav. 79, 241–277. Google Scholar
    • 51
      Johnson E. O.& Breslau N. . 2000 Increased risk of learning disabilities in low birth weight boys at age 11 years. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 490–500.doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00223-1 (doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00223-1). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 52
      Hack M., Flannery D. J., Schluchter M., Cartar L., Borawski E.& Klein N. . 2002 Outcomes in young adulthood for very-low-birth-weight infants. N. Engl. J. Med. 346, 149–157.doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa010856 (doi:10.1056/NEJMoa010856). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 53
      Lofdahl K. L., Holliday M.& Hirsch J. . 1992 Selection for conditionability in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Psychol. 106, 172–183.doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.106.2.172 (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.106.2.172). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 54
      Mery F.& Kawecki T. J. . 2002 Experimental evolution of learning ability in fruit flies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 14 274–14 279.doi: 10.1073/pnas.222371199 (doi:10.1073/pnas.222371199). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 55
      Dukas R. . 2004 Evolutionary biology of animal cognition. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 35, 347–374.doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.35.112202.130152 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.35.112202.130152). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 56
      Galsworthy M. J., Paya-Cano J. L., Liu L., Monleon S., Gregoryan G., Fernandes C., Schalkwyk L. C.& Plomin R. . 2005 Assessing reliability, heritability and general cognitive ability in a battery of cognitive tasks for laboratory mice. Behav. Genet. 35, 675–692.doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-3423-9 (doi:10.1007/s10519-005-3423-9). Crossref, PubMed, ISIGoogle Scholar
    • 57
      Arbilly M., Motro U., Feldman M. W.& Lotem A. Submitted. Co-evolution of learning and social strategies: I. complex and simple learning rules in the producer–scrounger game. Google Scholar
    • 58
      Laland K. N., Odling-Smee J.& Gilbert S. F. . 2008 EvoDevo and niche construction: building bridges. J. Exp. Zool. B Mol. Dev. Evol. 310B, 549–566.doi: 10.1002/jez.b.21232 (doi:10.1002/jez.b.21232). Crossref, ISIGoogle Scholar