Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x5gtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T12:55:20.364Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association between latent toxoplasmosis and cognition in adults: a cross-sectional study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 November 2014

S. D. GALE*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA The Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA
B. L. BROWN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA
L. D. ERICKSON
Affiliation:
Department of Sociology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA
A. BERRETT
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA
D. W. HEDGES
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA The Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Psychology Department and Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, 1060 SWKT, Provo, Utah 84602, USA. E-mail: shawn_gale@byu.edu

Summary

Latent infection from Toxoplasma gondii (T. gondii) is widespread worldwide and has been associated with cognitive deficits in some but not all animal models and in humans. We tested the hypothesis that latent toxoplasmosis is associated with decreased cognitive function in a large cross-sectional dataset, the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES). There were 4178 participants aged 20–59 years, of whom 19·1% had IgG antibodies against T. gondii. Two ordinary least squares (OLS) regression models adjusted for the NHANES complex sampling design and weighted to represent the US population were estimated for simple reaction time, processing speed and short-term memory or attention. The first model included only main effects of latent toxoplasmosis and demographic control variables, and the second added interaction terms between latent toxoplasmosis and the poverty-to-income ratio (PIR), educational attainment and race-ethnicity. We also used multivariate models to assess all three cognitive outcomes in the same model. Although the models evaluating main effects only demonstrated no association between latent toxoplasmosis and the cognitive outcomes, significant interactions between latent toxoplasmosis and the PIR, between latent toxoplasmosis and educational attainment, and between latent toxoplasmosis and race-ethnicity indicated that latent toxoplasmosis may adversely affect cognitive function in certain groups.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alvarado-Esquivel, C., Liesenfeld, O., Marquez-Conde, J. A., Estrada-Martinez, S. and Dubey, J. P. (2010). Seroepidemiology of infection with Toxoplasma gondii in workers occupationally exposed to water, sewage, and soil in Durango, Mexico. Journal of Parasitology 96, 847850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvarado-Esquivel, C., Torres-Castorena, A., Liesenfeld, O., Estrada-Martinez, S. and Urbina-Alvarez, J. D. (2012). High seroprevalence of Toxoplasma gondii infection in a subset of Mexican patients with work accidents and low socioeconomic status. Parasites and Vectors 5, 13.Google Scholar
Alvarado-Esquivel, C., Pacheco-Vega, S. J., Hernandez-Tinoco, J., Sanchez-Anguiano, L. F., Berumen-Segovia, L. O., Rodriguez-Acevedo, F. J., Beristain-Garcia, I., Rabago-Sanchez, E., Liesenfeld, O., Campillo-Ruiz, F. and Guereca-Garcia, O. A. (2014). Seroprevalence of Toxoplasma gondii infection and associated risk factors in Huicholes in Mexico. Parasites and Vectors 7, 301.Google Scholar
Baker, E. L., Letz, R. E., Fidler, A. T., Shalat, S., Plantamura, D. and Lyndon, M. (1985). A computer-based neurobehavioral evaluation system for occupational and environmental epidemiology: methodology and validation studies. Neurobehavioral Toxicology and Teratology 7, 369377.Google Scholar
Berenreiterova, M., Flegr, J., Kubena, A. A. and Nemec, P. (2011). The distribution of Toxoplasma gondii cysts in the brain of a mouse with latent toxoplasmosis: implications for the behavioral manipulation hypothesis. PLoS ONE 6, e28925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beste, C., Getzmann, S., Gajewski, P. D., Golka, K. and Falkenstein, M. (2014). Latent Toxoplasma gondii infection leads to deficits in goal-directed behavior in healthy elderly. Neurobiology of Aging 35, 10371044.Google Scholar
Carruthers, V. B. and Suzuki, Y. (2007). Effects of Toxoplasma gondii infection on the brain. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33, 745751.Google Scholar
Eppig, C., Fincher, C. L. and Thornhill, R. (2010). Parasite prevalence and the worldwide distribution of cognitive ability. Proceedings: Biological Sciences 277, 38013808.Google Scholar
Flegr, J., Preiss, M., Klose, J., Havlíček, J., Vitáková, M. and Kodym, P. (2003). Decreased level of psychobiological factor novelty seeking and lower intelligence in men latently infected with the protozoan parasite Toxoplasma gondii Dopamine, a missing link between schizophrenia and toxoplasmosis? Biological Psychology 63, 253268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flegr, J., Guenter, W., Bielinski, M., Deptula, A., Zalas-Wiecek, P., Piskunowicz, M., Szwed, K., Bucinski, A., Gospodarek, E. and Borkowska, A. (2012). Toxoplasma gondii infection affects cognitive function – corrigendum. Folia Parasitologica 59, 253254.Google Scholar
Fuks, J. M., Arrighi, R. B., Weidner, J. M., Kumar Mendu, S., Jin, Z., Wallin, R. P., Rethi, B., Birnir, B. and Barragan, A. (2012). GABAergic signaling is linked to a hypermigratory phenotype in dendritic cells infected by Toxoplasma gondii . PLoS Pathogens 8, e1003051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gajewski, P. D., Falkenstein, M., Hengstler, J. G. and Golka, K. (2014). Toxoplasma gondii impairs memory in infected seniors. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 36, 193199.Google Scholar
Guenter, W., Bielinski, M., Deptula, A., Zalas-Wiecek, P., Piskunowicz, M., Szwed, K., Bucinski, A., Gospodarek, E. and Borkowska, A. (2012). Does Toxoplasma gondii infection affect cognitive function? A case control study. Folia Parasitologica 59, 9398.Google Scholar
Gulinello, M., Acquarone, M., Kim, J. H., Spray, D. C., Barbosa, H. S., Sellers, R., Tanowitz, H. B. and Weiss, L. M. (2010). Acquired infection with Toxoplasma gondii in adult mice results in sensorimotor deficits but normal cognitive behavior despite widespread brain pathology. Microbes and Infection 12, 528537.Google Scholar
Havlicek, J., Gasova, Z. G., Smith, A. P., Zvara, K. and Flegr, J. (2001). Decrease of psychomotor performance in subjects with latent'asymptomatic' toxoplasmosis. Parasitology 122, 515520.Google Scholar
Hinze-Selch, D., Daubener, W., Eggert, L., Erdag, S., Stoltenberg, R. and Wilms, S. (2007). A controlled prospective study of Toxoplasma gondii infection in individuals with schizophrenia: beyond seroprevalence. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33, 782788.Google Scholar
Ingram, W. M., Goodrich, L. M., Robey, E. A. and Eisen, M. B. (2013). Mice infected with low-virulence strains of Toxoplasma gondii lose their innate aversion to cat urine, even after extensive parasite clearance. PLoS ONE 8, e75246.Google Scholar
Kannan, G. and Pletnikov, M. V. (2012). Toxoplasma gondii and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: an animal model perspective. Schizophrenia Bulletin 38, 11551161.Google Scholar
Kaplan, G. A., Turrell, G., Lynch, J. W., Everson, S. A., Helkala, E. L. and Salonen, J. T. (2001). Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood. International Journal of Epidemiology 30, 256263.Google Scholar
Krieg, E. F. Jr., Chrislip, D. W., Letz, R. E., Otto, D. A., Crespo, C. J., Brightwell, W. S. and Ehrenberg, R. L. (2001). Neurobehavioral test performance in the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Neurotoxicology and Teratology 23, 569589.Google Scholar
Kusbeci, O. Y., Miman, O., Yaman, M., Aktepe, O. C. and Yazar, S. (2011). Could Toxoplasma gondii have any role in Alzheimer disease? Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 25, 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Letz, R. E. and Baker, E. L. (1988). NES 2: Neurobehavioral Evaluation System Manual. Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Winchester, MA, USA.Google Scholar
Luby, J., Belden, A., Botteron, K., Marrus, N., Harms, M. P., Babb, C., Nishino, T. and Barch, D. (2013). The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: the mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatrics 167, 11351142.Google Scholar
McConkey, G. A., Martin, H. L., Bristow, G. C. and Webster, J. P. (2013). Toxoplasma gondii infection and behaviour – location, location, location? Journal of Experimental Biology 216, 113119.Google Scholar
Miman, O., Mutlu, E. A., Ozcan, O., Atambay, M., Karlidag, R. and Unal, S. (2010). Is there any role of Toxoplasma gondii in the etiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder? Psychiatry Research 177, 263265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mizgajska-Wiktor, H., Jarosz, W., Andrzejewska, I., Krzykala, M., Janowski, J. and Kozlowska, M. (2013). Differences in some developmental features between Toxoplasma gondii-seropositive and seronegative school children. Folia Parasitologica 60, 416424.Google Scholar
Mody, P., Gupta, A., Bikdeli, B., Lampropulos, J. F. and Dharmarajan, K. (2012). Most important articles on cardiovascular disease among racial and ethnic minorities. Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes 5, e33e41.Google Scholar
Montoya, J. G. and Liesenfeld, O. (2004). Toxoplasmosis. Lancet 363, 19651976.Google Scholar
Pearce, B. D., Kruszon-Moran, D. and Jones, J. L. (2014). The association of Toxoplasma gondii infection with neurocognitive deficits in a population-based analysis. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 49, 10011010.Google Scholar
Prandovszky, E., Gaskell, E., Martin, H., Dubey, J. P., Webster, J. P. and McConkey, G. A. (2011). The neurotropic parasite Toxoplasma gondii increases dopamine metabolism. PLoS ONE 6, e23866.Google Scholar
Rencher, A. C. and Scott, D. T. (1990). Assessing the contribution of individual variables following rejection of a multivariate hypothesis. Communications in Statistics-Simulation and Computation 19, 535553.Google Scholar
Roe, C. M., Mintun, M. A., D'Angelo, G., Xiong, C., Grant, E. A. and Morris, J. C. (2008). Alzheimer disease and cognitive reserve: variation of education effect with carbon 11-labeled Pittsburgh Compound B uptake. Archives of Neurology 65, 14671471.Google Scholar
Stibbs, H. H. (1985). Changes in brain concentrations of catecholamines and indoleamines in Toxoplasma gondii infected mice. Annals of Tropical Medicine and Parasitology 79, 153157.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R. and Fincher, C. L. (2011). Parasite stress promotes homicide and child maltreatment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 366, 34663477.Google Scholar
Turrell, G., Lynch, J. W., Kaplan, G. A., Everson, S. A., Helkala, E. L., Kauhanen, J. and Salonen, J. T. (2002). Socioeconomic position across the lifecourse and cognitive function in late middle age. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 57, S43S51.Google Scholar
Xiao, J., Kannan, G., Jones-Brando, L., Brannock, C., Krasnova, I. N., Cadet, J. L., Pletnikov, M. and Yolken, R. H. (2012). Sex-specific changes in gene expression and behavior induced by chronic Toxoplasma infection in mice. Neuroscience 206, 3948.Google Scholar
Yolken, R. H., Dickerson, F. B. and Fuller Torrey, E. (2009). Toxoplasma and schizophrenia. Parasite Immunology 31, 706715.Google Scholar