Skip to main content
Intended for healthcare professionals

Abstract

David Graeber’s ‘bullshit jobs theory’ has generated a great deal of academic and public interest. This theory holds that a large and rapidly increasing number of workers are undertaking jobs that they themselves recognise as being useless and of no social value. Despite generating clear testable hypotheses, this theory is not based on robust empirical research. We, therefore, use representative data from the EU to test five of its core hypotheses. Although we find that the perception of doing useless work is strongly associated with poor wellbeing, our findings contradict the main propositions of Graeber’s theory. The proportion of employees describing their jobs as useless is low and declining and bears little relationship to Graeber’s predictions. Marx’s concept of alienation and a ‘Work Relations’ approach provide inspiration for an alternative account that highlights poor management and toxic workplace environments in explaining why workers perceive paid work as useless.

Introduction

The meaningfulness of work has long been a central concern of sociology (Bailey and Madden, 2017) and other disciplines (Steger et al., 2012). Indeed, meaningful work relates to both Marx’s (1964[1844]) theory of alienation and Weber’s (1978[1922]) theory of bureaucratisation (Kalleberg, 2011). The meaningfulness of work has been investigated both directly (Bailey and Madden, 2016, 2017; Budd, 2011) and indirectly as a component of ‘intrinsic job quality’ (Kalleberg, 2011). However, Bailey and Madden (2019) point out that few studies have directly investigated meaningless work. The publication of David Graeber’s (2013, 2018) ‘bullshit jobs theory’ has generated a heightened interest among academics, the public and policymakers regarding useless jobs. In fact, Graeber’s 2013 essay and 2018 book on bullshit jobs (BS jobs) have been cited more than 900 times, according to Google Scholar, and discussed widely in the popular media. While such bibliometrics may not provide a complete picture with regards to the extent of academic influence, it is important to take seriously claims contained within non-academic publications, such as Graeber’s, and subject them to rigorous empirical examination. In fact, sociology of work has frequently been influenced by popular works published by trade publishers. In particular, few would deny the influence of the likes of Harry Braverman (1974), Naomi Klein (1999, 2007), or Guy Standing (2011, 2014). Moreover, Graeber’s (2013, 2018) arguments have been taken up uncritically by some scholars such as Frayne (2015) and Spicer (2017) whose own books have gone on to be well received both within and beyond academia.
The BS jobs theory suggests that many workers experience their jobs as being comprised of meaningless tasks in which they have to appear productive. As a result some academics writing on the future of work, and the post-work and anti-work traditions, have suggested that if, as Graeber claims, 30–60% of work is ‘bullshit’, radical reductions in the length of the working week could be easily achieved (e.g. Frayne, 2019; Susskind, 2020). It is important to recognise that Graeber is not simply stating that some people have useless jobs but is instead proposing a theory that seeks to explain why these jobs exist. This theory is premised on the existence of an economic system, that Graeber (2018) terms ‘managerial feudalism’, that produces a large and increasing number of workers with BS jobs, especially those with student debt in the finance, law and administration professions.
However, the evidence presented by Graeber (2018) in support of his ‘bullshit jobs’ thesis is largely based on qualitative data from employees who approached the author to praise him for his earlier speculative essay on the subject and to share anecdotes with him. Not only does reliance on this data source provide little empirical support for Graeber’s generalisations but it is also likely plagued by self-selection bias. Fortunately, Graeber’s book offers several clear predictions that are straightforward to test quantitatively. This article, therefore, seeks to empirically test several of Graeber’s (2018) main propositions:
1.
that the number of employees doing useless jobs is high (i.e. 20–50%);
2.
that useless jobs have been increasing rapidly over time;
3.
that some occupations have very high rates of BS jobs (e.g. financial services, marketing, administration) and others very low (e.g. refuse collectors, cleaners, farmers);
4.
that young workers with higher education qualifications are more likely to be doing BS jobs due to student debts;
5.
that useless jobs cause ‘spiritual violence’ and poor mental health.
Our findings demonstrate that while Graeber’s (2018) specific account of BS jobs and managerial feudalism cannot be empirically sustained, his work has uncovered an important and largely unresearched social ill. The scale of the problem is far from that predicted by Graeber’s theory. Nevertheless, millions of European workers suffer from work which they feel is not useful. Moreover, this experience is strongly associated with poor wellbeing. We, therefore, finish our analyses with our own tentative explanation, inspired by Marx’s writings on alienation, for why people think their job is useless.
Since Graeber is quite emphatic that employees themselves are the most credible source of information about the true value (or otherwise) of their jobs, survey data provide an obvious opportunity to test this theory. We use the 2005–2015 European Working Conditions Surveys (EWCS) to understand the drivers of respondents answering ‘rarely’ or ‘never’ to the statement: ‘I have the feeling of doing useful work’. However, our findings offer little support for any of these specific hypotheses generated from Graeber’s theory.

Meaningless work and BS jobs

What, then, differentiates Graeber’s BS jobs theory from the established literature on the meaningfulness of work? The key difference is that the meaningfulness of work has tended to be seen as a subjective phenomenon (Bailey and Madden, 2016, 2017; Kalleberg, 2011; Rosso et al., 2010). For instance, Bailey and Madden (2016, 2017) find that the meaningfulness or otherwise of work is not static but episodic and transitory; with the meaningfulness of work altering in relation to completion of tasks and goals, and participation in the rituals that reflect on these successes. In contrast, Graeber (2018) maintains that BS jobs are not just subjectively experienced as meaningless, pointless or useless, but that this subjective assessment is actually an accurate appraisal of the job’s lack of social value. That is to say, that it does not provide a ‘good or service [that] answers a demand or otherwise improves people’s lives’ (Graeber, 2018: 201). Specifically, Graeber (2018: 2) defines a BS job as:
a form of paid employment that is so completely pointless, unnecessary, or pernicious that even the employee cannot justify its existence even though, as part of the conditions of employment, the employee feels obliged to pretend that this is not the case.
This direct link between subjective assessments of usefulness and objective social value makes it possible for Graeber to distinguish between jobs which are ‘bullshit’ and those which are bad quality or (in his terminology) ‘shit’; ‘shit jobs’ being those that may be socially valuable but are poorly rewarded, whereas BS jobs provide little social value but are often highly remunerated.

The rise of ‘managerial feudalism’

Graeber’s (2018) approach further diverges from established approaches to meaningful work, in that these tend to focus on the individual and organisational characteristics that influence whether a job is meaningful (Bailey and Madden, 2016, 2017; Kalleberg, 2011; Rosso et al., 2010). Graeber posits a theory that seeks to relate the claimed existence and growth of BS jobs to macro-political economy developments: namely financialisation and managerialisation.
Graeber’s (2013) original BS jobs article took a conspiratorial tone; however, his book-length exposition (Graeber, 2018) constructs a more developed theory of ‘managerial feudalism’. Graeber’s theory holds that financialisation entails the appropriation of goods rather than making, moving and maintaining goods. The economic system which has emerged from the 1970s, therefore, more closely resembles feudalism than competitive capitalism. As in feudalism, financialisation does not require significant investment in production by the ruling class and, as their wealth is based on appropriation, they are incentivised to instead invest in an entourage of ‘lackeys’, ‘goons’ and others who can maintain their power and prestige. Moreover, these lackeys and goons are themselves argued to be rewarded for their service with retainers of their own. The result, according to Graeber (2018), is ‘managerial feudalism’, an elaborate economic hierarchy in which millions hold pointless jobs. As is illustrated in Figure 1, this transformation of industrial capitalism into managerial feudalism is held to have been made possible by two further factors: automation freeing large numbers of workers from the need to engage in useful labour and the growth of student debt ensuring an extensive supply of young workers willing to take BS jobs.
Figure 1. Bullshit jobs theory.
Source: Graeber (2018).
The BS jobs thesis gives rise to many clear testable outcomes, five of which we can readily examine with the EWCS:
1.
Graeber (2018) states that the development of his theory was premised on the expectation that a high proportion, between 20% and 50%, of jobs were BS and therefore we ‘need to ask ourselves . . . how did such a large proportion of our workforce find themselves labouring at tasks which they consider pointless’ (Graeber, 2018: xxiv). At other points in the book, Graeber suggests the proportion might be much higher, but certainly not lower. Therefore, if in reality there are only relatively small numbers of workers who see their jobs as useless, then this would undermine Graeber’s thesis.
2.
Graeber (2018: 146) argues that as a result of financialisation and managerial feudalism, the number of BS jobs has been ‘increasing rapidly in recent years’. Therefore, if the number of BS jobs is not increasing or is even declining, this would again undermine the theory.
3.
BS jobs should be concentrated in particular occupations and industries while being largely absent in others. As Graeber (2018: 6) explains: ‘many service workers hate their jobs; but even those who do are aware that what they do does make some sort of meaningful difference in the world . . . [Whereas] we can only assume that any office worker who one might suspect secretly believes themselves to have a bullshit job does, indeed, believe this.’ Graeber (2018) provides many examples of BS jobs and those which are not (summarised in Table 1). In general, the theory asserts that BS jobs should be concentrated in industries connected to finance, among professionals, administrators and managers. In contrast, they should be generally absent in low-paying, blue-collar, hourly-paid ‘shit jobs’, as Graeber maintains that there tends to be an inverse relationship between social value and economic reward – although, as Table 1 shows, Graeber also believes that there are some low-paying BS jobs.
4.
Since Graeber (2018) argues that the growth of student debt has been an important supply-side cause for the growth of BS jobs, these jobs should be more prevalent among young workers with higher education qualifications. If BS jobs are not, in fact, concentrated in the predicted educational and age groups, then this would invalidate this aspect of Graeber’s theory and suggest that we should look for other explanations.
5.
Graeber (2018: 102) argues that BS jobs are a form of ‘spiritual violence’ that inflict negative states of mind on individuals, including anxiety, depression and misery. If this is the case, we should detect a clear relationship between the perception of one’s job as useless and a validated measure of psychological wellbeing.
Table 1. Examples of bullshit and non-bullshit occupations.
Bullshit occupations (low social value):
Doormen, receptionists and bailiffs
Human resource profession
Lobbyists, political consultants and public relations specialists
Sales, marketing and advertising profession
Administration profession
Engineering (including IT) profession
Management consultants
Legal profession (esp. corporate lawyers and legal consultants)
Non-bullshit occupations (high social value):
Retail, restaurant and other front-line service work
Cleaners
Manual labourers
Agriculture
Plumbers, repairmen and mechanics
Public services (esp. health, firefighting, teachers, bus drivers, refuse collectors)
Creative sector (authors, artists, musicians, journalists, set designers, etc.)
Source: Graeber (2018).

From social value to the value of work relations

As highlighted above, Graeber (2018) believes that when individuals report their jobs as being useless, this is an accurate appraisal by the individual of their job’s social value. This, Graeber goes on to argue, is harmful to an individuals’ wellbeing as it provides little identity or purpose1 and, moreover, requires workers to pretend this is not the case. Graeber (2018: 102) states this is a source of ‘spiritual violence’ that ‘makes clear the degree to which you are entirely under another person’s power’. This is held to be a psychologically damaging experience, because humans need to conceive of ‘themselves as capable of acting on the world and others in predictable ways. Deny humans this sense of agency, and they are nothing’ (Graeber, 2018: 101).
This view of paid work as constraining essential human agency is striking in its resemblance to alienation, yet this concept is absent from Graeber’s account. Similarly, Marx argued that labour under capitalism is inherently alienating as it blocks individuals’ essential need for self-realisation. However, for Marx this was not the result of individuals being engaged in activity that was of no social value but rather because capitalist social relations frustrate the free development of human abilities in spontaneous activity.2 The alienation of labour then focuses our attention upon the social relations under which work takes place, generally (Braverman, 1974), and the process of control that exists over work relations, specifically (Hyman, 1975). As Wood (2020) argues, it is necessary to ‘recognise that paid work is not just a simple matter of market exchange but also of power relations’. Although Marx wrote of alienation as a fairly homogenous phenomenon, more contemporary writers have highlighted the degree to which work relations ‘differ in the relative power of employers and employees to control tasks, negotiate the conditions of employment, and terminate a job’ (Kalleberg, 2011: 83). Therefore, that work relations frustrate the potential for self-realisation may offer an alternative explanation for why people experience their job as useless. Moreover, Elster (1985, 1986) argues that self-realisation entails self-externalisation in addition to self-actualisation, as self-realisation is dependent on abilities being observable to others who confirm or deny them. This understanding of alienation highlights that workers might see their work as useless and suffer reduced wellbeing when work does not provide them with the means to use and develop their skills, abilities and capacities, or because their abilities are denied by others in the workplace.
Unlike the BS jobs theory, alienation is not premised on the view that the work being undertaken is inherently pointless and of no value. Instead, it highlights the importance of the social relations under which work is undertaken and the degree to which they constrain the ability of workers to affirm their sense of self through the development and recognition of skills and abilities. Bailey and Madden (2019: 1) point out that few studies have looked to investigate ‘under what circumstances work is rendered meaningless’. However, support for the work relations approach over BS jobs is provided by Dur and Lent (2019), who analysed the International Social Survey Program data from 37 countries in 2015 and found that workers are more likely to report being in a socially useless job in occupations that are generally seen to offer workers limited opportunities for intrinsic skill use and autonomy. These industries included ‘stationary plant and machine operators’, ‘assemblers’, ‘labourers in mining, construction, manufacturing and transport performing simple and routine manual tasks’, ‘food preparation assistants’, ‘garment and related trades workers’ and ‘cleaners and helpers’.
The importance of social relations and work organisation for the meaningfulness of work is also attested to by Bailey and Madden (2016), who find managers can destroy the sense of meaningful work in a number of ways such as: disconnecting workers from their values; taking them for granted; treating them unfairly; overriding their judgement; disconnecting people from supportive relationships; and putting people at risk of physical or emotional harm. However, in their cross-national study, Dur and Lent (2019) find that the feeling that one’s job is socially useless is not associated at the national-level with quality of management. Following the empirical testing of the BS jobs theory, we move on to consider the degree to which work relations, such as the quality of management, level of participation and social support in the workplace, provide an alternative means for understanding why workers feel their jobs are not useful.

Evidence of BS jobs

Graeber (2018: xxii) states that his theory has ‘been overwhelmingly confirmed by statistical research’. This statistical research consists of a YouGov poll of UK workers (YouGov, 2015) and a similar one by human resource firm Schouten & Nelissen of Dutch workers (Graeber, 2018: 6). Graeber (2018: 10) argues that, as it is not possible to calculate social value, we must take the workers’ ‘word for it’ because ‘it’s safe to assume the worker knows best’ as to how useful their job actually is. Therefore, the YouGov and Schouten & Nelissen surveys respectively asked respondents if their job is making a meaningful contribution to the world and if their job is meaningful. The YouGov survey found that 37% of UK workers say their job does not make a meaningful contribution to the world, while the Schouten & Nelissen survey finds that 40% of Dutch workers do not experience their job as meaningful.
However, Graeber (2018) suggests that the problem of BS jobs is even greater. For instance, he combines the YouGov ‘don’t know’ (13%) responses with the ‘is not’ responses to create a new category of those ‘who are not entirely sure their job made any sort of meaningful contribution to the world’ (Graeber, 2018: 6). Graeber goes on to refer to a State of Enterprise Work Report which reports that, in 2016, US office workers spent 46% of their time dealing with emails, administrative tasks, interruptions for non-essential tasks and in wasteful meetings. These tasks are then classed as bullshit and used to argue that around 50% of non-BS jobs are actually bullshit and that, therefore, ‘we can probably conclude that at least half of all work done in our society could be eliminated without making any real difference’ (Graeber, 2018: 26). At another point, Graeber (2018: xx) introduces the concept of ‘second-order BS jobs’, which are non-BS jobs done in support of BS jobs. He then claims that ‘if 37 percent of jobs are bullshit, and 37 percent of the remaining 63 percent are in the support of bullshit, then slightly over 50 percent of all labour falls into the bullshit sector in the broadest sense of the term’. However, these calculations violate the methodological principle that workers are best placed to judge the social value of their work. Reporting that one deals with emails, administrative tasks, interruptions for non-essential tasks and wasteful meetings is not the same as saying these tasks are pointless, unnecessary or pernicious. Moreover, second-order BS jobs must by definition already be included in the 37% reporting their job as bullshit.
Therefore, Graeber’s (2018: 267) conclusion that ‘upward of 50 percent to 60 percent of the population has . . . been thrown out of [useful] work’ cannot be sustained. But the lower estimate of 37–40% that he provides is also questionable; as Thompson and Pitts (2018) point out, the YouGov survey item is loaded in that it sets a very high bar for non-BS jobs. Clearly, saying your job does not make a meaningful contribution to the world is not the same as saying one’s job is completely pointless, unnecessary or pernicious, as it could quite conceivably be felt to be useful to one’s nation, community, neighbourhood, customers or family.
We can find very little information of the methodology followed by Schouten & Nelissen but the 37–40% figure is contradicted by Dur and Lent’s (2019) findings, according to which only 8% of workers disagreed that their job is useful to society. For the UK, the figure was 10% – only half of what Graeber assumed while developing his theory. Moreover, these researchers find no BS occupations, that is, occupations where a ‘preponderance of workers feel their jobs are pointless’ (Graeber, 2018: 64). In fact, even the YouGov survey which Graeber (2018) relies on contradicts his theory, as the number who feel their job does not contribute to the world is higher among social groups C2DE than ABC1. In other words, the survey which Graeber uses to support his theory actually finds that BS jobs are more likely among lowest grade, semi-skilled and skilled manual workers than they are among supervisory, clerical, managerial, administrative and professional workers. Finally, Dur and Lent (2019) also demonstrate that the number of people who feel their job is useful to society has not increased since 1989. Despite the weak empirical support marshalled by Graeber (2018) in its favour, his BS jobs theory has been taken seriously by academics, journalists and policymakers. It therefore deserves rigorous empirical testing and below we outline how we propose to do so.

Methodology

We use data from the EWCS (available at the UK Data Archive), which gathers measures on the usefulness of the job, workers’ wellbeing and objective data on the quality of work. The EWCS is a high-quality cross-sectional representative study of the working population. It uses interviewer-administered questionnaires completed at respondents’ homes.
The data reported in this article focus mainly on the latest available EWCS (2015), but we also use the waves from 2005 and 2010 to examine trends in the rate of BS jobs over time. Our analyses are based on the subsample of EU28 countries, excluding self-employed workers. The number of respondents who satisfied the inclusion criteria were 21,536 in 2005, 29,716 in 2010 and 29,784 in 2015.
As a measure of whether workers experience their job as useless, we use the responses given to the question ‘please select the response which best describes your work situation [. . .] you have the feeling of doing useful work’. The question is answered using a five-point scale from always to never.3 For some of the crosstabulation presented here, the five-point scale is dichotomised: if respondents answer ‘always’, ‘most of the time’ or ‘sometimes’, we define the job as useful; if they answer ‘rarely’ or ‘never’, we define the job as useless, or ‘bullshit’ in Graeber’s terminology. ‘Don’t know’ responses were set to missing. Although we don’t have any data to specifically test the validity or reliability of this survey item, it has face validity in that it is difficult to think what else it might be measuring apart from Graeber’s notion of a BS job. This question sets the bar at the same height as Graeber who clearly articulates that, not to be BS a job must do more than simply provide a profit or other benefit to the employer; it needs to provide a good or service that is sought after, but it does not need to make the world a better place. ‘Useful’ seems to hit exactly that note, and it is indeed a word that Graeber uses extensively in his writings when describing the difference between BS jobs and other jobs. Indeed, Graeber (2018) states that the question that motivated the writing of the original BS essay was whether there were as many useless jobs as he assumed, and, whether those that hold them were aware of it. This statement further underlines the appropriateness of using the above EWCS subjective measure of usefulness to identify BS jobs. Moreover, Graeber (2018: xiv) goes on to raise the question of whether our society is riddled with useless jobs.
EWCS survey items undergo cognitive testing before inclusion in the survey, and problematic or ambiguous items are noted in the interviewer post-survey debriefs. As different survey questions would lead to marginally different levels of BS jobs, our findings also investigate the patterns of BS jobs across occupations and time (while question wording is held constant). Therefore, our findings cannot be dismissed as artefacts of the wording of the question.
We start by comparing the 2015 rates of BS jobs across the EU28 countries. We compare rates across countries, as it is unclear whether Graeber’s (2018) theory is intended to be applied to all advanced capitalist economies or is accounting for a phenomenon specific to heavily financialised Anglo-Saxon economies (i.e. the UK and Ireland in our sample). Secondly, we examine the evolution in the rate of BS jobs from 2005 to 2015, assessing the magnitude of these changes across national, industrial, occupational, educational, age and gender categories. Thus, countries are classified into six groups: ‘Nordic’, ‘Anglo-Saxon’, ‘Continental’, ‘Mediterranean’, ‘Transition-A (Anglo-Saxon model)’ and ‘Transition-B (continental model)’ (Neesham and Tache, 2010).
Thirdly, Graeber’s assertion that BS jobs are concentrated in particular occupations, we compare the 2015 rate of BS jobs across occupations, measured at the two-digit level of the International Standard Classification of Occupations (ISCO).
Fourthly, Graeber’s theory that student loans force graduates into BS jobs in order to pay back their debts was tested by comparing rates of BS jobs among UK graduates who were aged 29 or younger to non-graduates in the same age bracket (UK university students graduate with the highest levels of student debt in Europe).
Fifthly, to examine the idea that experiencing one’s work as useless is indeed associated with workers’ mental health and wellbeing, we use mean scores of the WHO-5 index. The WHO-5 is a reliable instrument constructed by adding the scores of five items: (a) ‘I have felt cheerful and in good spirits’, (b) ‘I have felt calm and relaxed’, (c) ‘I have felt active and vigorous’, (d) ‘I woke up feeling fresh and rested’ and (e) ‘my daily life has been filled with things that interest me’. Each statement is answered in a six-point scale from ‘at no time’ through to ‘all of the time’. The final additive scale is reversed to make it a positively oriented indicator.
Finally, we attempt to derive a new (albeit tentative) explanation as to why employees describe their jobs as useless. Based on previous studies analysing the associations between job quality and wellbeing (see Eurofound, 2012, 2017; Muñoz de Bustillo et al., 2011), we extract nearly 60 indicators measuring different characteristics of the job and conduct a bivariate analysis with the five-point scale measure of work usefulness. The various job quality items selected cover aspects of wage level, job prospects, skills use, decision latitude, organisational participation, training, quantitative demands, pace determinants, emotional demands, management quality, social support, friendly or safe work environment and quality of working time, among others. The magnitude of the correlations between work usefulness and the various aspects of job quality are reported as Spearman’s rho.
The use of cross-sectional survey data does not lend itself to a formulation of a new theory as to why some individuals describe their jobs as useless, but the exploratory analyses give some pointers to insightful explanations of the phenomenon. We cannot unravel causal direction and we do not attempt model-building, but, in the spirit of EDA (Exploratory Data Analysis – see, for instance, Marsh and Elliott, 2008), we develop some fertile directions for future research. As pointed out above, few studies have looked to investigate the circumstances under which work is rendered meaningless (Bailey and Madden, 2019), meaning there are few readily available formal models that could be tested. All the estimates here reported are based on weighted data to account for varying sampling probabilities, relative country sizes and response rate differentials. Data analyses are performed using RStudio software (RStudio, PBC, Boston, MA, USA).

Findings and discussion

Hypothesis One: 20% to 50% of workers believe their jobs are useless

Graeber’s (2018) theory is premised on a very large number, if not a majority, of people being employed in BS jobs. He states that in developing the theory he assumed between 20% and 50% of jobs were bullshit but maintains that the actual figure is between 37% and 60% of all paid work. As shown in Figure 2, we find that in 2015 only 4.8% of EU28 workers responded that they did not have the feeling of doing useful work. It is unclear from Graeber’s writings whether he is positing a general theory or one that is specific to particular national types of capitalism (i.e. the UK and US). Despite his repeated reference to a Dutch survey, it might be that Graeber (2018) is particularly referring to the more financialised ‘Anglo-Saxon’ economies, such as those of the UK and Ireland, but here too only 5.6% of workers do not have the feeling of doing useful work (a slightly greater proportion than in Nordic (3%) and Continental (3.9%) countries). An advantage of our empirical investigation is that we can identify differences between national contexts. However, regardless of country, it seems that Graeber’s starting premise for the development of his theory was incorrect and the prevalence of BS jobs is only a small fraction of the proportion that Graeber claims.
Figure 2. EU28, 2015: proportion of workers who reported they ‘rarely’ or ‘never’ have the feeling of doing useful work over total workers, by country (% with standard errors).
Note: whiskers represent the standard errors of these proportions.
Source: EWCS, 2015.

Hypothesis Two: The number of workers who believe their job is useless is increasing rapidly

While our findings suggest that the prevalence of BS jobs is relatively confined, Graeber could be correct to posit that it is, nevertheless, rapidly growing due to managerial feudalism. Although Graeber (2018) provides no empirical evidence that the number of BS jobs has increased over time, he nevertheless states ‘there is every reason to believe that the overall number of BS jobs, and, even more, the overall percentage of jobs considered bullshit by those who hold them, has been increasing rapidly in recent years’ (Graeber, 2018: 145). However, again we find no evidence to support this conjecture. The EWCS has included our measure of BS jobs in 2005, 2010 and 2015, so a rapid rise should be clearly visible. However, as Table 2 and Figure 3 demonstrate, we find in EU28 countries the percentage of BS jobs has fallen from 7.8% in 2005 to 5.5% in 2010 and to only 4.8% in 2015 – exactly the opposite of Graeber’s prediction of a rapid rise. Again, Graeber may have been referring to just the Anglo-Saxon countries but here the drop in the percentage of BS jobs has been even greater – from 8.7% in 2005 down monotonically to 5.5% in 2015.
Table 2. EU28, 2005, 2010, 2015: trends in the prevalence of ‘bullshit jobs’ (number and % over total jobs), by demographic and labour market categories.
    2005 – 4th EWCS 2010 – 5th EWCS 2015 – 6th EWCS
    Total Useless jobs
N (%)
Total Useless jobs
N (%)
Total Useless jobs
N (%)
Gender                  
  Men 11,530 826 (7.2) 15,767 913 (5.8) 15,003 794 (5.3)
  Women 10,006 715 (7.1) 13,949 734 (5.3) 14,770 647 (4.4)
  χ2; df = 13.313; 1, p<.001 | φ = 0.02
Age                  
  29 and younger 5135 601 (11.7) 6505 582 (8.9) 5728 413 (7.2)
  30–39 5777 396 (6.9) 7915 368 (4.6) 7211 366 (5.1)
  40–49 5870 330 (5.6) 7917 351 (4.4) 7810 299 (3.8)
  50–59 3995 186 (4.7) 6099 311 (5.1) 7028 300 (4.3)
  60+ 674 21 (3.2) 1155 28 (2.4) 1864 55 (2.9)
  χ2; df = 107.97; 4, p<.001 | Cramer’s V = 0.06
Education                  
  Early childhood or primary 1136 112 (9.8) 1046 83 (7.9) 731 65 (8.9)
  Lower secondary 3356 313 (9.3) 4757 398 (8.4) 3941 254 (6.4)
  Upper secondary 9780 755 (7.7) 9600 572 (6.0) 13,486 702 (5.2)
  Post-secondary 7183 345 (4.8) 8625 303 (3.5) 11,478 413 (3.6)
  χ2; df = 91.384; 3, p<.001 | Cramer’s V = 0.06
Occupation                  
  Managers & professionals 4433 146 (3.3) 6077 90 (1.5) 6607 137 (2.1)
  Technicians & associate professionals 3128 98 (3.1) 5257 190 (3.6) 4434 128 (2.9)
  Clerical support workers 2961 220 (7.4) 3753 246 (6.6) 3862 184 (4.8)
  Service & sales workers 2819 293 (10.4) 4458 342 (7.7) 4975 275 (5.5)
  Craft & related trade workers 2985 223 (7.5) 3851 182 (4.7) 3325 146 (4.4)
  Plant & machine op, and assemblers 1963 207 (10.5) 2744 237 (8.6) 2690 192 (7.1)
  Elementary occup. and skilled agric. workers 2918 317 (10.9) 3356 345 (10.3) 3511 350 (10.0)
  χ2; df = 417.84; 6, p<.001 | Cramer’s V = 0.12
Sector of economic activity                  
  Agriculture (A) 506 28 (5.6) 637 23 (3.7) 619 42 (6.8)
  Industry (B,C,D,E) 4962 481 (9.7) 5492 412 (7.5) 5186 283 (5.5)
  Construction (F) 1496 83 (5.6) 2206 79 (3.6) 1705 65 (3.8)
  Commerce (G) 3172 324 (10.2) 4164 356 (8.6) 4299 317 (7.4)
  Hospitality (H) 731 110 (15.1) 1153 130 (11.3) 1374 106 (7.7)
  Transport (I) 1355 70 (5.2) 2089 126 (6.0) 2151 128 (6.0)
  Financial services (J,K) 2147 129 (6.0) 3426 201 (5.9) 4155 202 (4.9)
  Public administration (L) 1723 105 (6.1) 2165 100 (4.6) 2024 60 (3.0)
  Education (M) 1792 55 (3.1) 2699 39 (1.5) 2626 69 (2.6)
  Health and social work (N) 1631 46 (2.8) 3311 59 (1.8) 3729 61 (1.6)
  Other services (O,P,Q) 1730 89 (5.1) 2036 103 (5.1) 1662 72 (4.3)
  χ2; df = 276.77; 12, p<.001 | Cramer’s V = 0.10
Country group                  
  Nordic 1994 72 (3.6) 2593 76 (2.9) 2607 79 (3.0)
  Anglo-Saxon 3211 280 (8.7) 4290 336 (7.8) 4490 253 (5.6)
  Continental 7489 510 (6.8) 10,342 560 (5.4) 10,837 420 (3.9)
  Mediterranean 4724 416 (8.8) 6814 381 (5.6) 5871 328 (5.6)
  Transition-A 2493 169 (6.8) 3422 191 (5.6) 3690 218 (5.9)
  Transition-B 1626 95 (5.9) 2255 103 (4.6) 2289 143 (6.2)
  χ2; df = 72.415; 5, p<.001 | Cramer’s V = 0.05
All employees in EU28 21,536 1541 (7.2) 29,716 1647 (5.5) 29,784 1441 (4.8)
Notes: Values are presented as number (percentage). The chi-square, degrees of freedom, p-value and Cramer’s V or phi (φ) statistics correspond to Pearson’s chi-squared test of independence between the feeling of doing useful work and workers’ characteristics, as of 2015. Regressions done with complete cases only, therefore, not all categories add up 29,784.
Source: EWCS 2005, 2010 and 2015.
Figure 3. EU28: differences in the prevalence of ‘bullshit’ jobs between 2005 and 2015, by occupational and demographic groups, with 95% confidence intervals.
Notes: The points represent the proportion of BS jobs in 2005 minus the proportion of BS jobs in 2015, for each worker category. The lower and upper caps represent the 95% confidence interval of the difference in proportions. If the hinges do not cross over the horizontal axis at value = 0, it means that the difference between 2005 and 2015 is statistically significant.
Source: EWCS 2005, 2010 and 2015.

Hypothesis Three: BS jobs are concentrated in particular professions, such as finance, law, administration and marketing, and largely absent in others, such as those linked to public services and manual labour

It is possible that by looking at the labour market as a whole our analysis has hidden the growth of particular BS occupations. Indeed, the most entertaining and creative part of Graeber’s work is his identification of large swathes of the labour market that are characterised by BS jobs. Based on his own intuition about what is useful and what is bullshit, and backed up by some of the anecdotes sent to him, he identifies occupations where a ‘preponderance of workers feel their jobs are pointless’ (Graeber, 2018: 64). In particular, he identifies some professional jobs as the ones that are most obviously BS jobs. For instance, he points out that ‘hedge fund managers, political consultants, marketing gurus, lobbyists, corporate lawyers’ (Graeber, 2018: 209) would not be missed at all if they disappeared, though if elementary school teachers and nurses, garbage collectors, and mechanics, bus drivers, grocery store workers, firefighters, or short-order chefs were to vanish, the effects would be devastating (Graeber, 2018: 209). As explained above, BS jobs being concentrated in particular industries is of analytical importance as the theory asserts that they will be concentrated in industries connected to finance where the powerful are incentivised to invest in maintaining an entourage of lackeys as their wealth is appropriated rather than created.
Table 3 shows a list of occupations ranked by the proportion of incumbents who rate their job as rarely or never useful. This shows there is no evidence for the existence of BS occupations in which the majority of workers feel their work is not useful. However, our findings do suggest that Graeber is correct that workers in some of the occupations, such as teachers (1.7% BS jobs) and nurses (health associate professionals) (1.3%), generally see themselves as doing useful jobs, while sales workers are above average in the proportion rating their job as not useful (7.7% BS jobs). Nevertheless, most of the results contradict this aspect of Graeber’s assertion. For instance, legal professionals and associate professionals, senior officials, business and administration professionals and associate professionals are all low on this ranking, and jobs that Graeber rates as being examples of essential non-BS jobs, such as refuse collectors (9.7%) and cleaners and helpers (8.1%), are high on this scale.
Table 3. EU28, 2015: two-digit ISCO occupations ranked by percentage of workers reporting they ‘rarely’ or ‘never’ have the feeling of doing useful work.
Rank Occupation (2008 ISCO, 2-digit) Total respondents (N) Percentage feeling their job is not useful (%)
1 Legal, social, cultural and related associate professionals 471 0.2%
2 Chief executives, senior officials and legislators 93 0.8%
3 Health professionals 923 1.0%
4 Health associate professionals 1054 1.3%
5 Legal, social and cultural professionals 528 1.3%
6 Science and engineering professionals 809 1.6%
7 Teaching professionals 2094 1.7%
8 Business and administration professionals 867 1.9%
9 Personal care workers 1316 1.9%
10 Production and specialised services managers 447 2.0%
11 Administrative and commercial managers 477 2.5%
12 General and keyboard clerks 1453 2.8%
13 Information and communications technology professionals 432 3.2%
14 Business and administration associate professionals 1846 3.6%
15 Electrical and electronic trades workers 440 3.8%
16 Building and related trades workers, excluding electricians 1109 4.1%
17 Science and engineering associate professionals 860 4.3%
18 Hospitality, retail and other services managers 242 4.3%
19 Metal, machinery and related trades workers 1108 4.5%
20 Protective services workers 551 5.1%
21 Food processing, wood working, garment and other craft and related trades workers 579 5.3%
22 Market-oriented skilled agricultural workers 245 6.0%
23 Handicraft and printing workers 154 6.0%
24 Information and communications technicians 237 6.1%
25 Customer services clerks 681 6.1%
26 Other clerical support workers 414 6.3%
27 Drivers and mobile plant operators 1357 6.3%
28 Personal service workers 1370 6.5%
29 Numerical and material recording clerks 919 7.2%
30 Stationary plant and machine operators 842 7.5%
31 Sales workers 2487 7.7%
32 Food preparation assistants 241 7.7%
33 Cleaners and helpers 1223 8.1%
34 Refuse workers and other elementary workers 434 9.7%
35 Agricultural, forestry and fishery labourers 169 11.2%
36 Assemblers 199 11.8%
37 Labourers in mining, construction, manufacturing and transport 807 15.2%
We conclude from the above that there is little validity in Graeber’s prediction of BS occupations. None of the occupations approached anywhere near the 20–50% levels Graeber’s theory is premised upon; moreover, the occupations where workers were most likely to feel their work was not useful were those Graeber maintains are not BS (i.e. refuse collectors and cleaners and helpers). Indeed, when we move from occupations to industrial sectors (shown in Table 2), we find that in 2015, financial services is only just above the average (4.9%).

Hypothesis Four: Young workers with student debt are more likely to be doing BS jobs

Since Graeber (2018) maintains that student debt is an important supply-side factor leading to the growth of BS jobs, BS jobs should be concentrated among younger workers with higher education qualifications. However, we find a strong inverse relationship with education level (i.e. the more educated a young worker is the less likely they are to have a BS job), declining from 8.9% of those with only primary education to 3.6% of those with post-secondary education (Table 2). To test the role of student debt we focused on the rate of BS jobs among young UK graduates (up to age 29), who have the highest levels of student debt in Europe. We find that BS jobs are twice as prevalent among non-graduates (14.6%) compared to graduates (7.2%) – the opposite of Graeber’s assertion.4 In summary, we find no evidence to support these four central propositions of Graeber’s BS jobs thesis.

Hypothesis Five: BS jobs are associated with low levels of psychological wellbeing

Despite the prevalence of BS jobs being much lower than predicted by this theory, there is strong evidence that feeling that one’s job is useless is associated with poor psychological wellbeing. In the UK in 2015, workers who felt their job was not useful had WHO-5 wellbeing scores (M = 49.3, SD = 28.3) significantly lower than those who felt they were doing useful work (M = 64.5, SD = 21.7), and a similar gap was found in the EU28 sample. These results suggest that the ‘spiritual violence’ that Graeber describes as an outcome of BS jobs may indeed make workers more depressed and anxious. However, given the cross-sectional nature of the dataset, we cannot rule out the other plausible pathway – namely, that people who have poor mental health are more prone to underestimating the usefulness of their jobs. This finding is consistent with other research showing that meaningful work is one of the dimensions of job quality that is most strongly associated with wellbeing (Eurofound, 2017). Therefore, it is important to try and understand why workers might experience their job as useless work.

Work relations as an alternative explanation of useless jobs

If Graeber’s characterisation of the phenomenon of BS jobs is inadequate, then is there an alternative perspective that can provide a more plausible account? The popular acclaim that Graeber’s work on BS jobs has received strongly suggests that the sense of doing something useful is of fundamental importance to people’s working life – very much in line with eudaimonic accounts of wellbeing (Ryan and Deci, 2001). Indeed, our findings above highlight that Graeber was correct to highlight the damaging effects of doing work that one feels to be useless. Therefore, simply refuting Graeber’s explanation is insufficient and it is necessary to also suggest alternative explanations that are potentially more satisfactory. As highlighted in our literature review, the far older concept of alienation might yield an alternative way of understanding why some workers feel that their job is useless. This concept focuses our attention on the social relations under which paid work is undertaken. Most of those variables are, at best, weakly related to the ‘useful work’ variable, but Table 4 lists 13 of those variables that are correlated at rho = 0.2 or higher. Interestingly, of the selected variables, six are to do with the quality of management, three relate to participation, two concern social support, one is about using one’s own ideas at work, and one concerns having enough time to get the job done well.
Table 4. EU28, 2015: rank correlations measures (Spearman’s rho) between the feeling of doing useful work and selected job quality indicators.
Spearman’s rho Job quality measures
Dimension   Item
−0.324 Management 1 Your immediate boss respects you as a person
−0.319 Cognitive skills 2 You are able to apply your own ideas in your work
−0.298 Management 3 Your immediate boss encourages and supports your development
−0.294 Quant demands 4 You have enough time to get the job done
−0.286 Management 5 Your immediate boss is successful in getting people to work together
−0.284 Social support 6 Your manager helps and supports you
−0.276 Social support 7 Your colleagues help and support you
−0.275 Management 8 Your immediate boss provides useful feedback on your work
−0.265 Management 9 Your immediate boss gives you praise and recognition when you do a good job
−0.255 Participation 10 You are involved in improving the work organisation or work processes
−0.252 Participation 11 You can influence decisions that are important for your work
−0.228 Participation 12 You are consulted before objectives are set
−0.225 Management 13 Your immediate boss is helpful in getting the job done
Note: the table includes only correlation estimates greater than 0.2.
The strongest correlation was with the extent to which the employee felt respected by management (rho = .324) and there were also moderate correlations with other aspects of management style and quality (i.e. feeling encouraged and supported by management, thinking that management was successful at getting people to work together, providing useful feedback and being helpful). At the other end of the spectrum, when employees experience management that is disrespectful, inefficient and poor at giving feedback, it is more difficult to perceive the usefulness of the work. That this finding contradicts those of Dur and Lent (2019) suggests that their national level proxies for quality of management are poor substitutes for individual-level data on the quality of management.
The second strongest correlation (.319) was with an item about ability to use one’s own ideas at work – an important element for feeling that your job provides you with the ability to realise your human capacities. Moreover, there was a clear relationship between the extent to which people felt that they had enough time to do their job well and their rating of the usefulness of their job, suggesting that one source of feeling a job to be useless is the pace at which one is working, affecting the ability to realise one’s potential and capabilities. There were two measures of social support: one concerning colleagues and one concerning managers giving ‘help and support’. Both were correlated with the feeling of doing useful work. Finally, there were three items that measured participation at work (in influencing important decisions, in improving work organisation, and in being consulted about objectives), all of which predict feelings of usefulness.
These findings point to the fact that feelings of usefulness at work are not a direct indication of the social value of that work but are tied to the degree to which the social relations under which that job is undertaken enable individuals to realise their human potential. In particular, if managers are respectful, supportive and listen to workers, and if workers have the opportunities for participation, to use their own ideas and have time to do a good job, they are less likely to feel that their work is useless. Our findings, therefore, suggest that workers feeling that their job is not useful, is not due to the job itself being ‘bullshit’ and the result of managerial feudalism but rather is a symptom of bad management and toxic workplace cultures leading to alienation.
Alienation and its long-term trends were hotly debated topics in sociology during the 1960s, 1970s and 1980s (Vallas, 1988; Vallas and Yarrow, 1987). Theories surrounding the growth of the so-called ‘knowledge economy’ suggested that alienation would decline as workers were empowered by both a shift in the economy away from low-discretion routine manual jobs and towards technical and professional jobs that entail greater intrinsic skill-use and the delayering of traditional hierarchies in work organisations and the growth of teamwork (Frenkle et al., 1995; Green, 2003). In line with these more optimistic accounts, we find some good news, as suggested by writings on the knowledge economy, that this form of alienation is declining. This is seemingly due to alienation being most prevalent in declining blue-collar occupations which have traditionally been marked by hierarchical work organisation and a lack of discretion, participation and intrinsic skill-use.

Limitations

While we conclude that we provide a far more plausible account for the data than Graeber, we must recognise the limitations of our study. Here we are analysing cross-sectional data, so we need to be tentative in drawing causal conclusions. Unfortunately, we are not aware of any existing panel datasets with the range of variables that would facilitate a more rigorous testing of the causes and consequences of employees reporting that their jobs are of no social value. However, these results are sufficient to cast serious doubts on Graeber’s claim that we can take employees’ descriptions of their jobs being useless at face value. Taking inspiration from Marx’s writings on alienation, we suggest, rather, that the meaning of work is socially constructed in the workplace, and poor management and toxic workplace environments can blind employees doing valuable jobs from their true contribution.

Conclusion

This article makes three important theoretical and empirical contributions to our understanding of the world of work. Firstly, we use the EWCS to investigate four hypotheses derived from Graeber’s (2018) BS jobs theory. It is unclear whether Graeber (2018) is positing a general theory or one which relates to specific national types of capitalism (i.e. the financialised Anglo-Saxon economies); however, our approach enabled us to make an empirical contribution by identifying differing levels of useless jobs across European national contexts. Nevertheless, the empirical data do not support any of Graeber’s hypotheses. Therefore, the BS jobs theory must be rejected. Not only do our findings offer no support to this theory, they are often the exact opposite of what Graeber predicts. In particular, the proportion of workers who believe their paid work is not useful is declining rather than growing rapidly, and workers in professions connected to finance and with university degrees are less likely to feel their work is useless than many manual workers.
Secondly, although Graeber’s (2018) account of BS jobs and managerial feudalism cannot be empirically sustained, we do find that millions of European workers suffer from work which they feel is not useful. Moreover, this experience is strongly associated with poor wellbeing. Therefore, in rejecting its social scientific value we, nevertheless, welcome the fact that Graeber’s provocative style and use of amusing anecdotes has raised awareness of this serious social ill. Graeber’s contribution is especially welcome given the limited research on useless jobs and the accounts provided by Graeber (2018) are a valuable and interesting source of data on people’s critical reflections on the value of their work. Moreover, the fact that many people have worked in such jobs at some point and that they have serious consequences for wellbeing may explain why Graeber’s work resonates with so many people who can relate to the accounts he gives. Studies of job quality have tended not to include direct perceptions of useless jobs and instead focused on elements of intrinsic work quality such as skill use and discretion (see, for example, Eurofound, 2012; Muñoz de Bustillo et al., 2011), which are seen as being linked to the meaningfulness of work (Kalleberg, 2011). However, our findings suggest that whether a job is perceived as useless or useful should be included in future studies of job quality. The suffering associated with jobs that are experienced as useless also highlights the importance of policies to eradicate the conditions that give rise to such feelings – a point we return to below.
This article highlights that alternative theories grounded in empirical research are required to understand the social suffering caused by the feelings of useless work that Graeber observes. Therefore, our third major contribution is to demonstrate the value of Marx’s writings on alienation. We take inspiration from Marx’s understanding of alienation to investigate whether the social relations of work can explain why millions of workers do not feel that their work is useful. In particular, we focus on the ways in which the development of workers’ human capacities may be fettered by social relations at work. In doing so, we find that several factors related to this concept are indeed strongly associated with the degree to which an individual feels their job is useful. These factors include whether one’s managers are respectful, supportive, listen to workers and provide them with enough time to do a good job along with opportunities for participation and using their own ideas. This finding is not only theoretically consistent with Marx’s theory of alienation but also has important policy implications, as it suggests that, intrinsically, BS jobs are a rare phenomenon. Therefore, this article debunks Graeber’s (2018) claim that millions of workers are engaged in BS jobs of no social value and that the solution is that they be set free by a universal basic income. Instead, it suggests the need for unions that are willing and able to engage in what Umney and Coderre-LaPalme (2017) term ‘meaning of work conflicts’ so as to overturn those social relations in which people’s work is devalued by toxic workplace cultures that leave workers feeling their labour is pointless and of no use.

Acknowledgments

This article was inspired by the insightful and imaginative work of David Graeber and we are indebted to him for raising awareness of the harms of useless jobs. We are sure that David would have had strong and compelling views about this article and it is very sad that he passed away before we had the opportunity to discuss it with him. His legacy as an academic and an activist goes far beyond the body of work discussed in this article. He will be sadly missed, both within academia and by all those who strive for a better world. We would also like to thank Dr Ian Roper and Dr Elizabeth Cotton in their role as editors along with the three anonymous reviewers for their numerous comments and suggestions. Thanks are also due to Lina Dencik for helpful comments on an early draft of this article and Dr Mathijn Wilkens from Eurofound for his support and assistance.

Funding

The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

ORCID iDs

Footnotes

1. See Wood and Burchell (2018) for a discussion of why work-based identity and purpose are important for wellbeing.
2. Seeman (1959) combined insights from Marx with others from classical and post-war sociology to posit a highly influential social-psychological definition of work alienation consisting of five dimensions: powerlessness, meaninglessness, normlessness, isolation and self-estrangement. This definition inspired a large number of studies into the 1980s, most notably that of Blauner (1964) (Chiaburu et al., 2014; Edgell and Granter, 2019; Mottaz, 1981; Nair and Vohra, 2009; Shantz et al., 2014). However, this multidimensional approach has been criticised with the meaninglessness of work increasingly being seen as an antecedent or a consequence of a unidimensional definition of alienation restricted either to self-estrangement at work, or a disconnected passive and instrumental relationship with work (Mottaz, 1981; Nair and Vohra, 2009; Shantz et al., 2014; Vallas, 1988; Vallas and Yarrow, 1987).
3. Dekker (2018) also carries out an analysis using EWCS finding that 9% of jobs in the UK are bullshit. However, Dekker takes the item ‘I doubt the importance of my work’, which is slightly different from thinking your job is completely pointless, unnecessary or pernicious, since asking about ‘doubts’ brings up issues of confidence and self-esteem while ‘importance’ brings up issues of status. For instance, someone could think their role is useful while doubting they are very important relative to other team members.
4. This is based on the pooled sample across all three waves to increase the sample size. However, the same result also exists for the 2015 UK wave and for the EU. Therefore, we can be confident that student debt plays no role in predicting levels of BS jobs.

References

Bailey C, Madden A (2016) What makes work meaningful – or meaningless. MIT Sloan Management Review 57(4): 53–61.
Bailey C, Madden A (2017) Time reclaimed: temporality and the experience of meaningful work. Work, Employment and Society 31(1): 3–18.
Bailey C, Madden A (2019) ‘We’re not scum, we’re human’: agential responses in the face of meaningless work. Scandinavian Journal of Management 35(4): 1–11.
Blauner R (1964) Alienation and Freedom. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Braverman H (1974) Labor and Monopoly Capital: The Degradation of Work in the Twentieth Century. London: Monthly Review Press.
Budd JW (2011) The Thought of Work. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Chiaburu D, Thundiyil T, Wang J (2014) Alienation and its correlates: a meta-analysis. European Management Journal 32(1): 24–36.
Dekker F (2018) In Nederland heeft bijna niemand een bullshit job [In the Netherlands hardly anybody has a bullshit job]. ESB 103(4758): 90–91. Available at: https://esb-binary-external-prod.imgix.net/A7deQfFrl_WSuJCzDq1_1tLwM10.pdf?dl=Dekker%20 (accessed 2 October 2019).
Dur R, Lent MV (2019) Socially useless jobs. Industrial Relations: A Journal of Economy and Society 58(1): 3–16.
Edgell SE, Granter E (2019) The Sociology of Work: Continuity and Change in Paid and Unpaid Work. London: SAGE.
Elster J (1985) Making Sense of Marx. Studies in Marxism and Social Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Elster J (1986) An Introduction to Karl Marx. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press.
Eurofound (2012) Trends in Job Quality in Europe. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union.
Eurofound (2017) European Working Conditions Survey, 2015 [data collection], 4th Edition. UK Data Service. SN: 8098. Available at: https://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-8098-4 (accessed 4 October 2018).
Frayne D (2015) The Refusal of Work: The Theory and Practice of Resistance to Work. London: Zed Books.
Frayne D (2019) Introduction: putting therapy to work. In: Frayne D (ed.) The Work Cure: Critical Essays on Work and Wellness. Monmouth: PCCS Books, 1–28.
Frenkle S, Korczynski M, Donoghue L, et al. (1995) Re-constituting work: trends towards knowledge work and info-normative control. Work, Employment and Society 9(4): 773–796.
Graeber D (2013) On the phenomenon of bullshit jobs: a work rant. Strike! Magazine 3: 10–11. Available at: https://www.strike.coop/bullshit-jobs/ (accessed 2 October 2019).
Graeber D (2018) Bullshit Jobs: A Theory. London: Penguin Random House UK.
Green F (2003) Demanding Work: The Paradox of Job Quality in the Affluent Economy. Woodstock, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Hyman R (1975) Industrial Relations – A Marxist Introduction. London: Palgrave.
Kalleberg AL (2011) Good Jobs, Bad Jobs: The Rise of Polarized and Precarious Employment Systems in the United States, 1970s–2000s. New York: Russell Sage.
Klein N (1999) No Logo: Taking Aim at the Brand Bullies. New York: Picador.
Klein N (2007) The Shock Doctrine. New York: Random House.
Marsh C, Elliott J (2008) Exploring Data: An Introduction to Data Analysis for Social Scientists. Cambridge: Polity Press.
Marx K (1964[1844]) The Economic & Philosophic Manuscripts of 1844. New York: International Publishers.
Mottaz CJ (1981) Some determinants of work alienation. The Sociological Quarterly 22: 515–529.
Muñoz de, Bustillo R, Fernández-Mercias E, Antón JI, et al. (2011) Measuring More Than Money. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.
Nair N, Vohra N (2009) Developing a new measure of work alienation. Journal of Individual Employment Rights 14(4): 293–309.
Neesham C, Tache I (2010) Is there an East-European social model? International Journal of Social Economics 37(5): 344–360.
Rosso BD, Kathryn DH, Wrzesniewski A (2010) On the meaning of work: a theoretical integration and review. Research in Organizational Behavior 30: 91–127.
Ryan RM, Deci EL (2001) On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology 52: 141–166.
Seeman M (1959) On the personal consequence of alienation in work. American Sociological Review 24: 783–791.
Shantz A, Alfes K, Truss C (2014) Alienation from work: Marxist ideologies and twenty-first-century practice. International Journal of Human Resource Management 25(18): 2529–2550.
Spicer A (2017) Business Bullshit. Abingdon: Routledge.
Standing G (2011) The Precariat: The New Dangerous Class. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.
Standing G (2014) A Precariat Charter: From Denizens to Citizens. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.
Steger MF, Dik BJ, Duffy RD (2012) Measuring meaningful work: the Work and Meaning Inventory (WAMI). Journal of Career Assessment 20(3): 322–337.
Susskind D (2020) A World without Work: Technology, Automation, and How We Should Respond. London: Allen Lane.
Thompson P, Pitts H (2018) Bullshit about Jobs. The RSA (Royal Society of Arts, Manufactures and Commerce). Available at: https://www.thersa.org/discover/publications-and-articles/rsa-blogs/2018/07/bullshit-about-jobs (accessed 2 October 2019).
Umney C, Coderre-LaPalme G (2017) Blocked and new frontiers for trade unions: contesting ‘the meaning of work’ in the creative and caring sectors. British Journal of Industrial Relations 55(4): 859–878.
Vallas SP (1988) New technology, job content, and worker alienation: a test of two rival perspectives. Work and Occupations 15(2): 148–178.
Vallas SP, Yarrow M (1987) Advanced technology and worker alienation: comments on the Blauner/Marxism debate. Work and Occupations 14(1): 126–142.
Weber M (1978[1922]) Economy and Society: An Outline of Interpretive Sociology. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Wood AJ (2020) Despotism on Demand: How Power Operates in the Flexible Workplace. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Wood AJ, Burchell BJ (2018) Unemployment and well-being. In: Lewis A (ed.) The Cambridge Handbook of Psychology and Economic Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 234–259.

Biographies

Magdalena Soffia is the lead analyst on job quality and wellbeing at the What Works Centre for Wellbeing. She is also an affiliated scholar with the Individual in the Labour Market Research Group at the Sociology Department of the University of Cambridge, where she obtained her PhD and worked as a Research Associate. She has worked as a consultant at the Population Division of ECLAC–United Nations researching and providing technical assistance to governments around working conditions of international migrant populations. Her current research interests centre on the measurement of job quality and wellbeing across occupational settings.
Alex J Wood is Lecturer in the Sociology of Work at the University of Birmingham and a Research Associate at the Oxford Internet Institute. His most recent research investigates worker voice, organisation and collective action in the gig economy. His publications include the book Despotism on Demand (Cornell University Press, 2020) and articles for leading academic journals, such as: Sociology, Work, Employment and Society, Human Relations and Socio-Economic Review. He is PI of the British Academy funded ‘Gig Rights Project’, which uses an innovative quantitative methodology to explore labour rights and collective politics in the UK gig economy.
Brendan Burchell is Professor in the Social Sciences at the Department of Sociology, University of Cambridge and a Fellow of Magdalene College, Cambridge. He is a researcher in DIGIT, the ESRC Digital Futures at Work Research Centre. His main research interests centre on the effects of labour market conditions on wellbeing. Recent publications have focused on reducing working hours and the future of work, unemployment, job insecurity, work intensity, part-time work, zero-hours contracts, debt, occupational gender segregation job quality and self-employment. He works in interdisciplinary environments with psychologists, sociologists, economists, lawyers and human geographers.

Cite article

Cite article

Cite article

OR

Download to reference manager

If you have citation software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice

Share options

Share

Share this article

Share with email
EMAIL ARTICLE LINK
Share on social media

Share access to this article

Sharing links are not relevant where the article is open access and not available if you do not have a subscription.

For more information view the Sage Journals article sharing page.

Information, rights and permissions

Information

Published In

Article first published online: June 2, 2021
Issue published: October 2022

Keywords

  1. alienation
  2. bullshit jobs
  3. Graeber
  4. knowledge economy
  5. meaningful work

Rights and permissions

© The Author(s) 2021.
Creative Commons License (CC BY 4.0)
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).

Authors

Affiliations

Magdalena Soffia
Brendan Burchell

Notes

Alex J Wood, University House, University of Birmingham, Birmingham B15 2TY, UK. Email: [email protected]

Metrics and citations

Metrics

Journals metrics

This article was published in Work, Employment and Society.

VIEW ALL JOURNAL METRICS

Article usage*

Total views and downloads: 61696

*Article usage tracking started in December 2016


Altmetric

See the impact this article is making through the number of times it’s been read, and the Altmetric Score.
Learn more about the Altmetric Scores



Articles citing this one

Receive email alerts when this article is cited

Web of Science: 20 view articles Opens in new tab

Crossref: 12

  1. The search for meaningful work under neo-bureaucracy: Work precarity i...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  2. To Be Involved or Not to Be Involved: Testing Prison Staff Job Involve...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  3. ‘Bullshit’ After All? Why People Consider Their Jobs Socially Useless
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  4. Getting in the Loops: Using Adaptive Design Methods for Fall Armyworm ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  5. Grudging Acts
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  6. Crisis and Utopia: André Gorz and the end of work
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  7. The Normative Value of Making a Positive Contribution–Benefiting Other...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  8. Coercion, Consent, and Class Consciousness: How Workers Respond to Ama...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  9. Explaining differences in entrepreneurial activity between immigrants ...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  10. Social stratification in meaningful work: Occupational class dispariti...
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  11. The Policy Road to Socialism
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar
  12. In Favor: UBI Pays for Itself
    Go to citation Crossref Google Scholar

Figures and tables

Figures & Media

Tables

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get access

Access options

If you have access to journal content via a personal subscription, university, library, employer or society, select from the options below:

BSA members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.

BSA members can access this journal content using society membership credentials.


Alternatively, view purchase options below:

Purchase 24 hour online access to view and download content.

Access journal content via a DeepDyve subscription or find out more about this option.