Skip to main content

Longitudinal assessment of the relationship between frailty and social relationships among Japanese older adults: a random intercept cross-lagged panel model

Abstract

Objectives

This study aimed to explore the bidirectional association between frailty and social relationships in older adults while distinguishing between interpersonal and intrapersonal effects.

Methods

A prospective cohort study of community-dwelling older adults was conducted in Japan in three waves spanning six years with follow-ups in every three years. Random intercept cross-lagged panel model was used to explore temporal associations between frailty and social relationships.

Results

Data for 520 participants (mean age 73.02 [SD 6.38] years, 56.7% women) were analyzed. Across individuals, frailty was associated with social relationships (β = -0.514, p < 0.001). At the interpersonal level, frailty was cross-sectionally associated with social relationships separately at T1(β = -0.389, p < 0.01), T2 (β = -0.343, p < 0.001) and T3 (β = -0.273, p < 0.05). Moreover, social relationships were associated with subsequent increases in symptoms of frailty in all measurement waves (β = -0.332, p < 0.001; β = -0.169, p < 0.01) and vice versa (β = -0.149, p < 0.05; β = -0.292, p < 0.001).

Conclusions

The results suggest that frailty was associated with lower levels of social relationships. Frailty improvement programs can be combined with interventions to enhance social relationships, which will be beneficial in preventing frailty. The results emphasize the importance of combining clinical treatments of frailty with interventions to improve social relationships.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Global aging is increasing, and frailty is putting more pressure on health care systems. Frailty in older adults is a common condition that has been gaining attention in recent years, affecting more than 25% of people over the age of 85 [1]. Frailty is an evolving concept, normally describes an altered health status following a stressful event, that leaves one vulnerable to the effects of poorly balanced resolutions in the body [2, 3], and frequently leads to negative health consequences such as decreased functional status, hospitalization, disability, and death [2, 4, 5]. Research have shown that frailty is reversible and can be improved by interventions [2, 6]. Therefore, prevention of frailty in older people is an important issue in healthy aging.

Previous studies have established an association between frailty and a number of factors, including older age [7], poor economic status [8], low educational attainment [9], depression [10] and schizophrenia [11].

In addition to these, social factors, e.g., relationships, are also important determinants of frailty. Social relationships are the associations between individuals and their social environment [12] and have a direct impact on health outcomes regardless of the individual’s state of stress [13]. In recent years, a growing number of studies have begun to explore the connection between social relationships and frailty using population-based samples, however, the direction and mechanism of this connection is not fully understood [14]. For example, poor social relationships may be a risk factor for frailty; conversely, deterioration in social relationships may also lead to deterioration in health status, thereby exacerbating symptoms of frailty. It is perhaps even more likely that there is a vicious cycle in which deteriorating frailty weakens social relationships and health, which in turn exacerbates symptoms of frailty. Regardless of the cause-and-effect relationship, clarifying this relationship is critical to the clinical management of frailty patients as well as to interventions.

Previous studies revealed that social relationships predicted subsequent frailty. In a study of older Korean, frailty is more likely to occur among people who have less contact with others, as the frequency of contact with friends is most associated with frailty [15]. Similarly, in a one-year follow-up of community-dwelling older adults in China, the enrichment of social relationships positively influenced frailty by affecting the incidence of depression and physical activity [16]. However, few studies have focused on associations in the opposite direction, i.e., impact of frailty on social relationships. A small number of relevant studies exist but produce varied results. A longitudinal study in Japan showed that in the subfield of frailty, lower stepping speed and body strength were essential risk factors for subsequent declines in social relationships [17]. Whereas, a study in Amsterdam showed that the small social network of frail older adults existed at baseline and did not show an improvement across time; it was the increase of loneliness which changed the status of frailty [18]. Thus, we assumed the existence of a possible bidirectional association between social relationships and frailty which required further examination. Exploration of this relationship could help future research determine whether social relationship deficits and frailty precede each other or fall into a vicious cycle. This could help establish effective interventions for frail or socially unconnected older adults.

Cross-lagged model was used to explore the bidirectional association. Further, our study distinguished between within-person and between-person differences. The traditional cross-lagged panel model (CLPM) studies the longitudinal interrelationships between variables, and it assumes that the variables have no stable internal differences and only result in interpersonal effects, which is often unrealistic. Frailty and social relations are trait-like variables and show stable interpersonal variability over time [19, 20]. Studies have shown that conflating interpersonal and intrapersonal effects may lead to overestimation, underestimation, or even reversal of cross-lagged effects [21], and the conclusions drawn from CLPM at the interpersonal level cannot be inferred to the intrapersonal level [22]. For example, older adults with poor social relationships reporting frailty symptoms does not necessarily mean that improvement of social relationship will make them less frail [23]. The random intercept cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM) decomposes within-person and between-person effects in longitudinal associations between variables by including a random intercept to explain time-invariant individual differences, with less error than the traditional CLPM for within-person effects [21].

Against this background, we aimed to elucidate the longitudinal bidirectional relationship between frailty and social relationships in older adults using the RI-CLPM, taking into account the between-person effects.

Materials and methods

Design

This longitudinal study, conducted from 2011 to 2017, is a part of a project which started in 1991 in a suburban area of central Japan. This project aimed to identify the factors contributing to health, longevity, and well-being of the local residents. Data were collected using a self-administrated questionnaire. Initially, questionnaires were distributed to all residents and collected after 2 weeks. The survey was conducted every three years.

Participants

In the present study, older people aged 65 years and above were selected for the baseline year. Applying a longitudinal methodology, data from three waves were collected in 2011 (T1), 2014 (T2), and 2017 (T3). Initially, we selected 1085 participants aged ≥ 65 years at the baseline year; after eliminating 78 people needed support and care, 70 participants with complete missing data on frailty and social relationships and 22 adults with Parkinson’s disease and dementia, we included 915 participants and collected data regarding their social relationships and frailty in 2014 and 2017. Of this number, 166 participants were unable to complete follow-up in 2014 and 229 participants in 2017 due to death, hospitalization, moving, etc. Finally, 520 participants were included in this study (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of the participants in the present study

Measures

Frailty

Frailty indicators were extracted from the Kihon Checklist (KCL), a popular frailty assessment tool [23]. The scale has been widely used in studies of frailty in the Japanese older adults and has shown sufficient reliability and validity [24, 25]. The lifestyle domain with 20 items was utilized which included activities of daily life, physical strength, nutritional status, oral function, the condition of being housebound, and cognitive function to predict frailty. Items were scored as either 0 for “good” or 1 for “poor”. The total score ranged from 0 to 20. A higher score indicated more symptoms of frailty.

Social relationships

The Index of Social Interaction (ISI) was used to evaluate social relationships, which measures various aspects of social relationships in daily settings [26]. There are 18 items in this scale, classified into five domains. The independence domain explores the motivation to live and maintain a healthy and active life. The social curiosity domain includes habits of reading, using new equipment, having hobbies, and a feeling of importance in society. The interaction domain assesses how well family and non-family people communicate. The domain of participation in society assesses involvement in social and neighborhood groups, as well as assuming an active social role. Finally, the feeling of safety domain explores if participants have someone offering counselling and providing support during emergencies. For the items, positive answers (always, often, sometimes) and negative answers (rare) were given 1 and 0 points, respectively. The total score ranged from 0 to 18. The higher score the participants got, the higher level of social relationships they had.

Frailty and social relationships showed appropriate internal consistency at three time points (frailty: α = 0.83, α = 0.88, α = 0.90; social relationships: α = 0.78, α = 0.83, α = 0.84).

Covariates

Based on previous studies, age, gender, exercise, alcohol consumption/smoking and chronic diseases were considered as covariates at baseline [27, 28]. Age and gender were considered as continuous and categorical variables, respectively. Regarding exercise, the respondents were asked “Do you exercise?” Responses of “always”, “frequently”, and “sometimes” were coded as “performing exercise” whereas responses of “no” were coded as “not exercising”. Regarding alcohol consumption, respondents were asked “Do you drink alcohol?” Responses of “always”, “everyday”, and “sometimes” were coded as “consumes alcohol” whereas responses of “hardly ever” and “do not consume alcohol” were coded as “non-consumers of alcohol”. For smoking, the respondents were asked “are you an active smoker?” Responses of “everyday” and “sometimes” were coded as “actively smoking”, responses of “I previously smoked but have now stopped” was coded as “former smoker”, whereas “I do not smoke” was coded as “non-smoking behavior”. Chronic diseases was treated as a categorical variable. Participants were classified as having at least one chronic disease or none based on the presence of the following diseases: hypertension, stroke, heart disease, diabetes, hyperlipidemia, lung disease, stomach/liver/gallbladder disorders, kidney disorders, musculoskeletal disorders, cancer, immune disease, depression, and eye and ear disorders.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among all variables were reported using SPSS. RI-CLPM was used to explore the bidirectional relation between frailty and social relationships [20]. First, we evaluated intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) using SPSS for frailty and social relationships to understand how much of variance was explained by the differences of participants. Second, measurement invariance was used to check the equivalence of these constructs across time to ensure that effects were attributed to real changes in variables. A three-step procedure was used for measurement invariance, including configural, metric, and scalar invariances [29]. Finally, the RI-CLPM was fit to identify bidirectional and time-ordered relations between frailty and social relationships. The covariates were controlled at the level of the random intercept. The comparative fit indices (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) were used to estimate model fit [30, 31]. Acceptable model fit (CFI > 0.90, SRMR/RMSEA < 0.10) and good model fit (CFI/TLI > 0.95, SRMR/RMSEA < 0.08) were defined using standard benchmark values [32]. The results were presented as standardized coefficients. All analyses were performed using SPSS version 27 (Armonk, NY, USA) and Mplus version 8.6 (Muthén and Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, USA). The dataset had missing values due to the longitudinal nature. Full information maximum likelihood (FIML) was used to process missing data. FIML is a maximum likelihood method for handling missing data in a single step and is extensively used in structural equation models [33].

Results

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for the demographic characteristics of participants, age at baseline, and frailty and social relationships scores. The mean and standard deviations of age at baseline were 73.02 ± 6.38 years. Over half of the participants were women, not living alone, had high economic status, and at least one chronic disease. The mean frailty scores were 2.37 ± 2.67 in 2011, 3.69 ± 3.33 in 2014, and 5.28 ± 3.88 in 2017. The average scores of social relationships were 16.42 ± 1.51, 16.02 ± 2.05, and 16.26 ± 1.83 in 2011, 2014, and 2017, respectively. Table 2 shows the correlation matrix for frailty and ISI. The bivariate correlations among frailty and ISI were in the expected direction. At each measurement wave, frailty was negatively correlated with ISI. The ICC for frailty was 0.64, indicating that 64% of the variance was due to between-person differences and 36% due to within-person fluctuations. For social relationships, the ICC was 0.55, indicating that 55% of the variation was due to the differences among older adults, and 45% due to intrapersonal fluctuations.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the study sample (n = 520)
Table 2 Bivariate correlations (n = 520)

Measurement invariance

To compare factors longitudinally, we investigated measurement invariance in longitudinal measurement models of frailty and social relationships, for which the factor loadings must be time-invariant, at least with metric invariance across time [34, 35]. According to previous studies [36, 37], metric invariance can be established when compared with the configuration model, ΔCFI < 0.010, ΔRMSEA < 0.015, and ΔSRMR < 0.030, and scalar invariance, when, ΔCFI < 0.010, ΔRMSEA < 0.015, and ΔSRMR < 0.010. As shown in Table 3, both constructs demonstrated acceptable model fit and achieved metric invariance. Additionally, χ^2 difference tests did not yield significant result over time, indicating that frailty and social relationships can be compared across three time points.

Table 3 Longitudinal Measurement Invariance

RI-CLPM

The RI-CLPM model incorporating all covariates is depicted in Fig. 2 which indicates an acceptable model fit, χ2 = 106.991, df = 25, CFI = 0.962, TLI = 0.922, RMSEA = 0.078, SRMR = 0.066. The association between frailty and social relationships was divided into interpersonal and intrapersonal effects. At the interpersonal level, there was a strong negative association between frailty and social relationships (β = -0.514). This indicated that participants having more symptoms of frailty across the measurement waves showed lower level of social relationships and vice versa. At the individual level, social relationships and frailty had a negative cross-sectional association separately at T1 (β = -0.389, p < 0.01), T2 (β = -0.343, p < 0.001) and T3 (β = -0.273, p < 0.05). This meant that the intrapersonal changes of frailty and social relationships were correlated. The autoregressive path of frailty and social relationships were significant, indicating the severity of frailty and social relationships were carried over within-individuals to the next measurement wave. Intrapersonal cross-lagged paths from social relationships to frailty were significant at T1-T2 (β = -0.149, p < 0.05) and T2-T3 (β = -0.292, p < 0.001). In the opposite direction, the paths from frailty to social relationships were also significant at T1-T2 (β = -0.332, p < 0.001) and T2-T3 (β = -0.169, p < 0.01).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Random-intercept cross-lagged panel model results. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. RI means random intercepts. Completely standardized parameter estimates with standard errors are reported in this model and controlled for all covariates. CFI = 0.962, TLI = 0.922, RMSEA = 0.078 (95% CI, 0.064–0.095), SRMR = 0.066, χ2 = 106.991, df = 25

Discussion

We explored the bidirectional association between frailty and social relationships in older people through RI-CLPM, while distinguishing interpersonal and intrapersonal effects. Our results showed a significant negative correlation at within- and between-person levels. Additionally, each measurement wave revealed consistent cross-sectional correlations. To our knowledge, this is one of the first studies to examine the within-person temporal dynamics of the relation between frailty and social relationships in older adults while controlling for between-person effects. Therefore, this study will provide the basis for future intervention research.

Regarding autoregressive influences, past social relationships always have a positive impact on future social relationships. Past research has shown that active social relationships can create positive feedback, making older adults more willing to participate in social activities and having a positive effect on the development of future social relationships [38]. Likewise, the effect of frailty symptoms on their own autoregression was consistent. As previous studies have shown, frailty accumulated with the increase of chronic diseases and the deterioration of body functions, and the accumulation rate accelerated with age [39]. The ability of older people who were already in a frail state to resist external disturbances further deteriorated, which in turn would aggravate the frail symptoms [40]. This result emphasizes the importance of early intervention.

In the present study, between-person relationship revealed a robust trait effect indicating a negative association between social relationships and frailty across all three datasets. Individuals characterized by diminished social relationships exhibited a higher propensity for experiencing frailty symptoms compared to their counterparts. Consequently, these between-person findings delineate populations warranting targeted interventions. Our results offer additional elucidation on the interplay between social relationships and frailty symptoms among older adults, highlighting the heightened vulnerability of individuals with attenuated social networks to increased frailty manifestations.

After controlling for these trait effects, the within-person autoregressive path showed a vicious cycle of frailty and social relationships. Social relationships always predicted the subsequent variations of frailty. Although there were methodological differences in the assessment, similar results could be found in previous studies. A longitudinal study in the United Kingdom suggests that an increased risk of frailty is associated with reduced social relationships. Individuals with poor social relationships may experience prevalence of cardiovascular disease and exhibit worse health behaviors [3]. Because of infrequent contact with others, people with lower social relationships have reduced need for health, which may contribute to the risk of frailty. Longitudinal studies linking social relationships with walking speed, activities of daily living [41], and mobility and upper extremity strength [42] suggested that weakened social relationships might increase the risk of sarcopenia, a major factor in frailty [43], and some measures of frailty, including upper extremity strength and walking ability, were direct measures of sarcopenia. Moreover, one of the etiologies of sarcopenia is lack of physical activity [44], which could be one possible link between lack of social relationships and development of frailty. People with poor social relationships tend to be less physically active [45] which leads to an increased risk of frailty [46]. Therefore, it is advisable to include promotion of social relationships in the intervention kit for preventing or decreasing frailty.

In the opposite direction, the predictive effect of frailty on social relationships was also reflected in all measured waves, which was consistent with a few previous studies. A longitudinal study in Singapore showed association between increased social engagement and decreased frailty [47]. Symptoms of frailty increase loneliness in older people and make them inactive to participate in social activities, thus affecting social relationships. Another longitudinal study of older adults demonstrated that frailty led to reduced social activity and contact with neighbors [17]. The lack of adequate physical activity in physically-limited older adults restricts mobility in the living space, which in turn limits social relationships. Although there is not enough research on the effects of frailty on social relationships, there are many studies that report the predictive effects of subdomains of frailty on social relationships. For example, functional decline [48], poorer cognitive function [49], poor oral health [50], and cognitive decline [51] were shown to be predictors of worse social relationships. As levels of these subdomains decrease, older adults become frailer [52, 53].

In each measurement wave, frailty and social relationships had a significant cross-sectional association. Thus, better social relationships may be associated with milder frailty symptoms and vice versa. This is consistent with the findings of Lestari et al., showing that frail older adults preferred less-active types of social relationships [4], and Tsutsumimoto et al., showing that frailty and social relationships in community-dwelling older adults were negatively correlated [54]. In a short-term Austrian study, the prevalence of frailty significantly reduced within 12 weeks after a social support intervention [6], suggesting the relatively stronger association between frailty and social relationships in the short term than in the long term. It is worth mentioning that our study also found that the lagging effect of frailty on social relationships became progressively weaker, probably due to the fact that long periods of frailty allowed older adults to adapt to physical changes and thus had the ability to maintain certain social relationships [55]. This may be related to the positive attitude of older adults towards life. In contrast, the lagged effect of social relationships on frailty became stronger. Previous studies suggested that there may be some potential mediating variables between social relationships and frailty, for example, higher levels of social relationships may be effective in reducing the risk of loneliness and depression, and thus further reducing the risk of frailty [56]. These findings may provide directions for future research.

Due to the reversible state of frailty in the older adults, the results of our study have practical implications. The importance of improving social relationships should be considered at the same time when providing care and interventions for frail older adults. Helping them to establish positive social connections such as family and friends, social networks, and some volunteer activities can be effective in improving frailty [57]. Moreover, intervention programs should be gradual, from weak to strong, because frail older adults tend to have less social participation. Some social activities, such as volunteering or physical exercise in salons often requires a higher level of functioning [58].

This study’s key strength is the utilization of RI-CLPM to examine the longitudinal bidirectional relation between frailty and social relationships and detection of within-person relations while controlling the between-person effect. Another advantage is that this study confirmed the vicious circle between frailty and social relationships, so it may be more effective to consider the importance of social relationships when developing intervention programs for frail older adults.

This study has some limitations. First, the relatively small size of the study may affect the stability of the results. Second, the study’s coverage was less extensive as it did not include a psychological dimension to measure frailty. Third, this study can only generalize the association between overall scores of frailty and social relationships, and cannot derive the specific effects between the sub-domains. Therefore, it is recommended that future studies subdivide frailty or social relationships to explore more in-depth mechanisms. Finally, the long span between time points and the small total number of waves in this study did not detect a more stable lag effect, which could lead to serendipity, and therefore future studies are recommended to perform more number of measurements to enhance the stability of this type of study.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that frailty and social relations in Japanese older adults are strongly correlated between people. Within people, changes in frailty and social relationships are consistently correlated. It further suggests that there is a vicious cycle between frailty and social relationships, which means the deterioration of social relationships in older adults also increases the risk of becoming more frail and vice versa. Our results emphasized that providing the earliest possible intervention for frail older populations while maintaining certain social relationships was necessary both to improve the frailty itself and to avoid a vicious cycle of further frailty in the future.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the local government but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study, and so are not publicly available. Data are however available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of the local government.

References

  1. Collard RM, Boter H, Schoevers RA, et al. Prevalence of frailty in community-dwelling older persons: a systematic review[J]. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60:1487–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Etman A, Burdorf A, Van der Cammen TJ, Mackenbach JP, Van Lenthe FJ. Socio-demographic determinants of worsening in Frailty among Community-Dwelling Older people in 11 European Countries. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2012;66:1116–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Davies K, Maharani A, Chandola T, Todd C, Pendleton N. The longitudinal relationship between loneliness, social isolation, and Frailty in older adults in England: a prospective analysis. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2021;2:e70–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Lestari SK, Eriksson M, de Luna X, Malmberg G, Ng N. Frailty and types of Social relationships among older adults in 17 European countries: a latent class analysis. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2022;101:104705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Song X, Mitnitski A, Rockwood K. Prevalence and 10-Year outcomes of Frailty in older adults in relation to Deficit Accumulation. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58:681–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Luger E, Dorner TE, Haider S, Kapan A, Lackinger C, Schindler K. Effects of a home-based and Volunteer-Administered Physical Training, Nutritional, and Social Support Program on Malnutrition and Frailty in older persons: a Randomized Controlled Trial. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2016;17:671–e9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Romero-Ortuno R, Kenny RA. The Frailty Index in europeans: Association with Age and Mortality. Age Ageing. 2012;41:684–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Avila-Funes JA, Helmer C, Amieva H, Barberger-Gateau P, Goff ML, Ritchie K, Portet F, Carrière I, Tavernier B, Gutiérrez-Robledo LM. Frailty among Community-Dwelling Elderly people in France: the three-city study. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63:1089–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Fhon JRS, Rodrigues RAP, Santos JLF, Diniz MA, Santos EB.; Almeida, V.C.; Giacomini, S.B.L. factors Associated with Frailty in older adults: a longitudinal study. Rev Saude Publica 2018, 52.

  10. Deng MG, Liu F, Liang Y, et al. Association between frailty and depression: a bidirectional mendelian randomization study[J]. Sci Adv. 2023;9(38):eadi3902.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Deng MG, Wang K, Liu F et al. The Relationship Between Frailty and Schizophrenia: A Genetic Association and Mendelian Randomization Study[J]. 2023.

  12. Kuiper JS, Zuidersma M, Zuidema SU, et al. Social relationships and cognitive decline: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies[J]. Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45:1169–206.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review[J]. PLoS Med, 2010, 7(7), e1000316.

  14. Fried L, Prohaska T, Burholt V, Burns A, Golden J, Hawkley L, Lawlor B, Leavey G, Lubben J, O’Sullivan. R. A Unified Approach to Loneliness. 2020.

  15. Chon D, Lee Y, Kim J, Lee K. The Association between Frequency of Social Contact and Frailty in Older people: Korean Frailty and Aging Cohort Study (KFACS). J Korean Med Sci 2018, 33.

  16. Jin Y, Yu R, Si H, Bian Y, Qiao X, Ji L, Liu Q, Wang W, Yu J, Li Y. Effects of Social Support on Frailty trajectory classes among Community-Dwelling older adults: the mediating role of depressive symptoms and physical activity. Geriatr Nur (Lond. 2022;45:39–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Nagai K, Tamaki K, Kusunoki H, Wada Y, Tsuji S, Itoh M, Sano K, Amano M, Hayashitani S, Yokoyama R. Physical Frailty predicts the development of Social Frailty: a prospective cohort study. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20:1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Hoogendijk EO, Suanet B, Dent E, Deeg DJ, Aartsen MJ. Adverse effects of Frailty on Social Functioning in older adults: results from the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. Maturitas. 2016;83:45–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hoogendijk EO, Heymans MW, Deeg DJ, Huisman M. Socioeconomic inequalities in Frailty among older adults: results from a 10-Year longitudinal study in the Netherlands. Gerontology. 2018;64:157–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Azevedo RS, de Sousa JR, Araujo MT, Martins Filho AJ, de Alcantara BN, Araujo FM, Queiroz MG, Cruz AC, Vasconcelos BHB, Chiang JO. In situ Immune response and mechanisms of cell damage in Central Nervous System of fatal cases Microcephaly by Zika Virus. Sci Rep. 2018;8:1.

    Article  CAS  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Hamaker EL, Kuiper RM, Grasman RP. A critique of the Cross-lagged Panel Model. Psychol Methods. 2015;20:102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Hamaker EL. Why Researchers Should Think within-Person: A Paradigmatic Rationale. 2012.

  23. Fukutomi E, Okumiya K, Wada T, Sakamoto R, Ishimoto Y, Kimura Y, Kasahara Y, Chen W-L, Imai H, Fujisawa M. Importance of Cognitive Assessment as Part of the K Ihon C Hecklist developed by the J apanese M inistry of H ealth, L Abor and W Elfare for Prediction of Frailty at a 2-Year follow up. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2013;13:654–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Sewo Sampaio PY, Sampaio RAC, Yamada M, et al. Systematic review of the Kihon Checklist: is it a reliable assessment of frailty?[J]. Volume 16. Geriatrics & gerontology international; 2016. pp. 893–902. 8.

  25. Satake S, Senda K, Hong YJ, et al. Validity of the K ihon Checklist for assessing frailty status[J]. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2016;16(6):709–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Anme T, Shimada C. Social Interaction and Mortality in a five year longitudinal study of the Elderly. Nihon Koshu Eisei Zasshi Jpn. J Public Health. 2000;47:127–33.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Tsutsumimoto K, Doi T, Makizako H, Hotta R, Nakakubo S, Makino K, Suzuki T, Shimada H. Cognitive Frailty is Associated with fall-related fracture among older people. J Nutr Health Aging. 2018;22:1216–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Tomioka K, Kurumatani N, Saeki K. The Differential effects of type and frequency of Social Participation on IADL declines of older people. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0207426.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Putnick DL, Bornstein MH. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research[J]. Dev Rev. 2016;41:71–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling 2nd Ed. N Y Guilford 2005, 3.

  31. MacCallum RC, Austin JT. Applications of structural equation modeling in Psychological Research. Annu Rev Psychol. 2000;51:201–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Marsh HW, Hau K-T, Wen Z. Search of Golden rules: comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Struct Equ Model. 2004;11:320–41.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  33. Allison PD. Missing data techniques for structural equation modeling. J Abnorm Psychol. 2003;112:545–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Mulder JD, Hamaker EL. Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 2021;28:638–48.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  35. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford; 2015.

  36. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 2007;14:464–504.

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  37. Putnick DL, Bornstein MH. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev Rev. 2016;41:71–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Curtis RG, Huxhold O, Windsor TD. Perceived control and social activity in midlife and older age: a reciprocal association? Findings from the German ageing Survey[J]. Journals Gerontology: Ser B 2018, 807–15.

  39. Rogers NT, Marshall A, Roberts CH, et al. Physical activity and trajectories of frailty among older adults: evidence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing[J]. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(2):e0170878.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Hoogendijk EO, Afilalo J, Ensrud KE, Kowal. Paul and Onder, Graziano and Fried, Linda P. Frailty: implications for clinical practice and public health[J]. Lancet. 2019;394:1365–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Shankar A, McMunn A, Demakakos P, Hamer M, Steptoe A. Social isolation and loneliness: prospective associations with functional status in older adults. Health Psychol. 2017;36:179–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Perissinotto CM, Stijacic Cenzer IS, Covinsky KE. Loneliness in older persons: a predictor of functional decline and death. Arch Intern Med. 2012;172:1078–83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Reginster JY, Cooper C, Rizzoli R, et al. Recommendations for the conduct of clinical trials for drugs to treat or prevent Sarcopenia. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2016;28:47–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Walston JD. Sarcopenia in older adults. Curr Opin Rheumatol. 2012;24:623–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Shankar A, McMunn A, Banks J, Steptoe A. Loneliness, social isolation, and behavioral and biological health indicators in older adults. Health Psychol. 2011;30:377–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Song J, Lindquist LA, Chang RW, et al. Sedentary behavior as a risk factor for physical frailty independent of moderate activity: results from the osteoarthritis initiative. Am J Public Health. 2015;105:1439–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Zhao Y, Liao L, Feng H, Chen H, Ning H. Correction to: enablers and barriers to implementing care quality improvement program in nursing homes in China. BMC Geriatric. 2022;22:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Hajek A, Brettschneider C, Eisele M, et al. Social support and functional decline in the oldest old. Gerontology. 2022;68:200–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Lara E, Caballero FF, Rico-Uribe LA, et al. Are loneliness and social isolation associated with cognitive decline? Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2019;34:1613–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Kossioni AE, Hajto-Bryk J, Janssens B, et al. Practical guidelines for physicians in promoting oral health in frail older adults. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2018;19:1039–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Du C, Dong X, Katz B, et al. Source of perceived social support and cognitive change: an 8-year prospective cohort study[J]. Aging Ment Health. 2023;27(8):1496–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. He B, Ma Y, Wang C, et al. Prevalence and risk factors for frailty among community-dwelling older people in China: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Nutr Health Aging. 2019;23:442–50.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Feng Z, Lugtenberg M, Franse C, et al. Risk factors and protective factors associated with incident or increase of frailty among community-dwelling older adults: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0178383.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Tsutsumimoto K, Doi T, Makizako H, et al. Association of social frailty with both cognitive and physical deficits among older people. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2017;18:603–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. AAndrew MK, Dupuis-Blanchard S, Maxwell C, Giguere A, Keefe J, Rockwood K. St. John, P and Canadian Frailty Network. Social and societal implications of frailty, including impact on Canadian healthcare systems[J]. J Frailty Aging. 2018;7:217–23.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Wu F, Sheng Y. Social support network, social support, self-efficacy, health-promoting behavior and healthy aging among older adults: a pathway analysis[J]. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2019;85:103934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Berkman LF, Krishna A, Kawachi I et al. 7. Social network epidemiology. Soc Epidemiol 2014, 234–89.

  58. Makizako H, Shimada H, Doi T, et al. Social frailty leads to the development of physical frailty among physically non-frail adults: a four-year follow-up longitudinal cohort study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15:490.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We owe a debt of gratitude to all study participants and Tobishima employees.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Number JP21K18449 and by JST SPRING, Grant Number JPMJSP2124.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Mingyu Cui: Conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, original draft. Dandan Jiao, Yang Liu, Yantong Zhu, Xiang Li, Zhu Zhu, Jinrui Zhang, Afsari Banu Alpona, Yanlin Wang and Meiling Qian: Review and editing. Yuko Sawada, Kumi Watanabe Miura, Taeko Watanabe, Emiko Tanaka: Investigation and data curation. Tokie Anme: Conceptualization, methodology, investigation, reviewing and editing, project administration, funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tokie Anme.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All experimental protocols in this study have been approved by the Ethics Committee of The University of Tsukuba (approval number 1331-4, June 24th, 2022). The need for informed consent was waived by the Ethics Committee of The University of Tsukuba. All methods were carried out in accordance with declaration of Helsinki.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cui, M., Jiao, D., Liu, Y. et al. Longitudinal assessment of the relationship between frailty and social relationships among Japanese older adults: a random intercept cross-lagged panel model. BMC Public Health 24, 706 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18234-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-024-18234-1

Keywords