Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 20 September 2019
Sec. Eating Behavior

The Executive Functions in Overweight and Obesity: A Systematic Review of Neuropsychological Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Studies

  • 1Department of Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Background: The increasing incidence of people affected by overweight or obesity is a significant health problem. The knowledge of the factors which influences the inappropriate eating behaviors causing excessive body fat is an essential goal for the research. Overweight and obesity are significant risk factors for many health diseases, such as cardiovascular problems, diabetes. Recently, many studies have focused on the relationship between body weight and cognitive processes.

Objectives: This systematic review is aimed to investigate the existence and the nature of the relationship between excessive body weight (overweight/obesity) and executive functions, analyzing cross-sectional, and longitudinal studies in order to verify the evidence of a possible causality between these variables.

Methods: The review was carried out according to the PRISMA-Statement, through systematic searches in the scientific databases PubMed, Medline, PsychInfo, and PsycArticles. The studies selected examined performance on executive tasks by participants with overweight or obesity, aged between 5 and 70 years. Studies examining eating disorders or obesity resulting from other medical problems were excluded. Furthermore, the results of studies using a cross-sectional design and those using a longitudinal one were separately investigated.

Results: Sixty-three cross-sectional studies and twenty-eight longitudinal studies that met our inclusion and exclusion criteria were analyzed. The results confirmed the presence of a relation between executive functions and overweight/obesity, although the directionality of this relation was not clear; nor did any single executive function emerge as being more involved than others in this relation. Despite this, there was evidence of a reciprocal influence between executive functions and overweight/obesity.

Conclusions: This systematic review underlines the presence of a relationship between executive functions and overweight/obesity. Moreover, it seems to suggest a bidirectional trend in this relationship that could be the cause of the failure of interventions for weight reduction. The results of this review highlight the importance of a theoretical model able to consider all the main variables of interest, with the aim to structuring integrated approaches to solve the overweight/obesity problems.

Introduction

Rationale

Obesity and overweight, defined as the accumulation of excessive body fat, are risk factors for many chronic diseases, such as hypertension (Jiang et al., 2016) and diabetes (Hauner, 2017) as well as musculoskeletal (McPhail et al., 2014) and respiratory problems (Littleton, 2012). Prospective studies have shown an association between obesity in adulthood and cognitive impairment in old age (Sanderlin et al., 2017). Moreover, obesity appears to be connected to psychopathologies—such as anxiety disorders and depression (Carpiniello et al., 2009; De Wit et al., 2010; Gariepy et al., 2010; Luppino et al., 2010; Carey et al., 2014)—and to social difficulties—such as bullying and social isolation (Kolotkin et al., 2001). The most common risk factors associated with an increase in body weight are poor eating habits and a lack of adequate physical activity (World Health Organization, 2000; Prentice, 2001; Dubbert et al., 2002), which results in a chronic imbalance between individual's needs and energy acquisition (Yumuk et al., 2015).

Conventionally, overweight classifications are made according to the body mass index of an individual (BMI; World Health Organization, 2000). BMI takes into account the weight and height of a person, providing a quantifiable index as the measure of body mass. The WHO considers different severity of overweight: pre-clinical obesity (BMI between 25 and 29.9), obesity class I (BMI between 30 and 34.9), obesity class II (BMI between 35 and 39.9), obesity class III (BMI equal to or higher than 40). An excessive increase in BMI can lead to a higher risk of premature death and a lower quality of life (World Health Organization, 2000). However, some authors focused on other indices that appear to be more sensitive for investigating the relationship between different degrees of overweight and their effects on health, such as waist circumference, waist-to-height ratio, and the body adiposity index (Janssen et al., 2004; Ashwell et al., 2012; Lam et al., 2015). In general, an increase in body fat, in the absence of metabolic and hormonal pathologies (Bray, 1999), is strongly associated with overeating behaviors and excessive ingestion of high-calorific foods that affect the individual's metabolism (McCrory et al., 1999; Ouwens et al., 2003).

The prevalence of obesity or overweight has increased in recent years. In 2014, more than 1.9 billion adults were overweight (World Health Organization, 2015); of these, 600 million were classifiable as obese. Regarding younger people, in 2013 it was estimated that about 42 million children and adolescents between the ages of 5–18 years, and about 12.4% of children below the age of 5 years, were overweight or obese (World Health Organization, 2015; Yumuk et al., 2015). It is expected that around 60% of the world's population will reach critical BMI values by 2030 (Kelly et al., 2008).

Considering these data, it appears useful to investigate the predisposing and exacerbating factors of increases in BMI and body fat, related to overeating behavior. In line with this need, recent years have seen increasing interest in the cognitive mechanisms involved in overweight or obesity (Liang et al., 2014; Forcano et al., 2018). Furthermore, recent studies (Yang et al., 2018) have focused on the relationship between executive functions and obesity to investigate the existence and nature of this association.

Executive Functions

Executive functions (EFs) is an “umbrella term” (Damasio, 1995; Elliott, 2003; Chan et al., 2008; Diamond, 2013) that includes both complex cognitive processes—such as the resolution of new tasks, the modification of existing behaviors, the planning of new strategies for problem solving, the sequencing of complex actions (Funahashi, 2001; Elliott, 2003), the inhibition of motor or cognitive automatic responses and the control of conflicting information (Diamond, 2013)—and lower-level of cognitive processes, which allow to regulate and control thoughts and actions during goal-directed behavior and involve different cognitive dimensions such as perception and sensation, memory and motivation, attention, reasoning, and problem-solving (Pennington and Ozonoff, 1996).

Although there are various EFs, many studies have centered on three specific processes (Miyake et al., 2000; Diamond, 2013): (i) Cognitive Flexibility (or Shifting), characterized by an attentional shift between tasks or between different mental operations; (ii) Working Memory (or Updating), which includes the updating and monitoring of mental representations in order to respond appropriately to external tasks or stimuli; and (iii) Inhibition, which consists of the voluntary inhibition of dominant or automatic responses for controlling actions, thoughts and emotions, as well as attentional aspects, in order to respond appropriately to the needs of goal-directed behaviors (Miyake et al., 2000; Hofmann et al., 2012; Diamond, 2013). In general, some EFs have been studied more than others due to the presence of cognitive tasks (e.g., Stroop task, Stop Signal Task, Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), Span Task, Maze task) that seem to be more sensitive in the investigation of specific EFs, however there are some limits in their interpretations (Diamond, 2013; Vainik et al., 2013).

Some authors (Grafman and Litvan, 1999; Chan et al., 2008) distinguished between two different groups of EFs: the “cold EFs” and the “hot EFs.” The first—which include verbal reasoning, problem-solving skills, planning, attentional maintenance, cognitive flexibility, response inhibition, and control of conflicting information—are characterized by the absence of emotional processing of stimuli and do not generate emotional arousal (Chan et al., 2008). The second, the hot EFs—which include expectations of punishment-gratification, social behavior, and decision-making—are characterized by the presence of beliefs and desires, and they include a powerful emotional component (Chan et al., 2008). According to Miyake model, these executive domains would be included in cold EFs. However, in daily life, hot and cold EFs work jointly, and both are necessary to direct our behavior. It is essential to underline that EFs are characterized by individual differences, which during life undergo multiple modifications (Jacques and Marcovitch, 2010; Hall and Marteau, 2014), as the reduction of cognitive flexibility and planning with aging (see Jacques and Marcovitch, 2010), or the alterations in inhibition in psychopathology (Nigg, 2000). These differences and changes can also be traced back to the establishment of healthy behaviors, such as eating habits (Hall and Marteau, 2014).

Executive Functions in Obesity and Overweight

A recent review by Dohle et al. (2018) showed that some studies support the hypothesis that food behaviors affect executive functioning, i.e., healthy eating habits promote the preservation of cognitive functions throughout life (Morris et al., 2005; Smith and Blumenthal, 2016). Other authors are inclined to sustain the opposite point of view, in which cognitive functions are considered as the predictors of food behaviors and, consequently, of body weight changes. According to this view, EFs deficits are considered the cause of inappropriate attitudes to food and represent a trigger for both eating disorders and changes in BMI (Dohle et al., 2018). These different views on the relationship between EFs and eating habits are also observed in the studies that considered the association between obesity/overweight and EFs (Perry, 2004; Pignatti et al., 2006; Davis et al., 2007b; Gonzales et al., 2010).

The theoretical models that consider the relationship between EFs and overweight/obesity are less developed, and usually, they do not focus on specific executive processes. However, it might be interesting to extend this type of studies because they could help in identifying some aspects which are connected to the increase in obesity and related problems. To clarify the nature of the relationship between EFs and overweight/obesity could allow identifying both a causal direction in the relationship between EFs and excessive body weight and the most suitable theoretical model able to explain this relationship.

In general, it could be useful to define whether the studies investigating the relationship between EFs and excessive body weight showed a consensus about the presence of a clear link between the examined variables. In fact, some reviews tried to collect information about this relationship (Fitzpatrick et al., 2013; Vainik et al., 2013; Emery and Levine, 2017; Gettens and Gorin, 2017; Gluck et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2018), but they only confirmed the existence of a correlation between these dimensions, but they were unable to clarify the essence of this relationship, nor the causality. Identifying whether the EFs represent predictors of weight gain (Smith and Robbins, 2013; Chen et al., 2017), or consequences of the increased body weight (Perry, 2004; Sellbom and Gunstad, 2012) still represents an important goal in research.

A review analyzing the relationship between EFs and overweight aimed to examine studies with different experimental designs (cross-sectional, longitudinal) could help in identifying an eventually causal relationship between variables, as well as it could allow understanding how the interactions that emerged in cross-sectional studies, change over time in longitudinal studies. In our view, this represents an essential goal, because it can be useful to both for structuring interventions aimed at reducing risks related to excessive body weight and/or EFs impairment and contributing to the development of a theoretical model. Moreover, studies analyzing the causality between these variables could be a starting point to identify whether some executive domains are more involved than others during body weight gain. An important aspect to consider is the role of every single executive domain in the relationship with overweight/obesity. To identify whether there is a specific EF or some EFs, which influences or are influenced by the excessive body weight could be useful both for the development of a theoretical model on EFs-overweight relationship and for the definition of risk factors related to excessive body weight or impairment in executive domains. Some studies identified impairment in specific EF domains as decision-making, planning and problem solving (for a review see Fitzpatrick et al., 2013), or in inhibition (Gluck et al., 2017). However, generally, the studies identified alteration of EFs, without well-defined the relationship between the single domain of EFs and excessive body weight. This tendency could be due to the different cognitive tasks used and the high number of methodological designs considered in the studies (Vainik et al., 2013).

Objectives

This systematic review aimed to analyse longitudinal and cross-sectional studies that have investigated the association between EFs and obesity or overweight in the absence of chronic diseases or related eating disorders, trying to add knowledge about the nature of this relationship.

Specifically, the aims of this systematic review are:

(a) to document the cross-sectional evidence between EFs and overweight/obesity, trying to identify the consensus on the presence of a relationship between EFs and excessive body weight;

(b) to see if any executive domain has been associated mainly with excessive body weight, considering both positive and negative results;

(c) to analyse longitudinal studies to assess the causality between EFs and the BMI, considering EFs eventually an outcome or predictor of increase in BMI.

This review represents an attempt to systematize the studies on the relationship between EFs and overweight (Vainik et al., 2013; Emery and Levine, 2017; Yang et al., 2018). The inclusion of longitudinal studies, by also considering different interventions to reduce weight (as in Thiara et al., 2017) could help to clarify the nature of this relationship better. The final aim of this review is to understand how approaching the problems related to excess body weight.

Method

This systematic review was conducted according to the PRISMA-statement (Liberati et al., 2009; Moher et al., 2009). Online registration of the protocol has not been provided.

Research Strategies

The systematic review was conducted using PubMed, PsycINFO, PsycArticles, MedLine databases. The following keywords were used: “Executive Function,” “Inhibition,” “Cognitive Inhibition,” “Selective Attention,” “Updating,” “Working Memory,” “Shifting,” Cognitive Flexibility,” “BMI,” “Overweight,” “Obesity,” “Overeating,” “Diet.”

The scripts used for the search are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Script for the systematic research.

The starting date of the work was January 8th, 2018. All original, “full-text” papers published in international, peer-reviewed journals up to June 10th, 2018 were considered.

Eligibility Criteria

Selections were made independently by two researchers (FF; MC) and any disagreements resolved by a supervisor (GF). All the studies investigated the relationship between EFs and excessive body weight. Studies including at least one group with overweight or obesity, classified through the international criteria as BMI (World Health Organization, 2000) and BMI percentiles (Flegal et al., 2002), and investigating at least one EF were included. Furthermore, both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies were considered and analyzed separately. For the selection of the articles the following inclusion criteria were used: (a) academic articles published in international, “peer-reviewed” journals; (b) studies written in English; (c) studies on humans with overweight or obesity (BMI higher than 25) at various levels of severity; (d) studies using cognitive tasks to assess EFs; (e) studies including participants aged between 5 and 70 years; (f) cross-sectional and longitudinal studies; (g) studies including different interventions to reduce body weight (bariatric surgery, cognitive remediation therapy, weight-loss programmes that included diets, or physical activity); (h) studies including other psychological variables related to EFs and body weight.

The following exclusion criteria were applied: (a) short report: these type of articles were excluded because after a preliminary analysis of them it was observed that the information reported was too general; (b) studies examining participants with binge eating disorder or other eating disorders; (c) studies focusing on cognitive functions other than executive ones; (d) studies analyzing EFs through self-report questionnaires; (f) studies on obesity of metabolic origin or caused by other medical diseases; (g) studies considering overweight in psychopathological or psychiatric conditions (e.g., depression, schizophrenia, ADHD, etc.). Moreover, for cross-sectional studies, the absence of a normal-weight control group for comparison of executive functioning was an additional exclusion criterion. For the longitudinal researches, both observational and experimental studies were included.

Additionally, in both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, differences between groups (with normal-weight and overweight), or between different assessment times (pre, post, follow-up) were mainly commented, although regression analyses (continuous BMI) were also considered. Correlational studies were excluded if the method did not include the presence of different conditions of BMI (including both normal-weight and overweight individuals).

Data Collection Process

According to PICOS (Liberati et al., 2009), the authors extracted from the selected articles information about participants in both the control group and the groups with overweight/obesity (age, BMI, gender), methods (executive tasks used), and main results observed in the EFs tasks.

According to the aims of this review, for the cross-sectional studies, all the results concerning comparisons between overweight/obese groups and normal-weight groups on a cognitive task that assessed one or more EFs were analyzed. For the longitudinal studies, all the results concerning the analysis over time of participants with excessive body weight were examined, including also changes in weight, and executive functioning following either weight-loss programmes or cognitive training. The characteristics of the studies are shown in Tables 3, 4.

Quality Assessment

A quality assessment analyzed the eligibility of each article by detecting the quality of the studies. This process was aimed to reduce the risk of bias selection and was conducted using a six-point checklist created explicitly for the screening of the studies of this review. For each point, a maximum score of two (high-quality) could be awarded per article: a score of zero corresponded to a low-quality index, a score of 1 to a medium-quality index and a score of 2 to a high-quality index. To derive an overall quality of score of the study, the mean score of each study was multiplied by 100. Studies with a score <75% were considered with high quality, in line with other qualitative analyses (e.g., Varkevisser et al., 2019). The systematic review excluded studies with very low quality (lower than 50%). Table 2 shows the six-point quality assessment checklist. Tables 3, 4 reported the quality assessment for each selected article.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Checklist for quality assessment.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cross-sectional studies.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Longitudinal Studies.

Results

Study Selection

The initial search produced 1,817 articles. After excluding 614 duplicates, 922 articles were rejected according to an analysis of both title and abstract, leaving a final total of 281 studies to be reviewed and subjected to the quality assessment.

At the end of the review process, 88 articles remained. The flow chart (Figure 1) shows the study selection process, including the number of studies found, the assessment process and the reasons for the exclusion of the articles.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart.

The 88 selected articles were categorized according to the experimental design. Sixty-three studies used a cross-sectional design, and twenty-eight studies used a longitudinal design (see Tables 3, 4). Three studies (Deckers et al., 2017; Demos et al., 2017; Vantieghem et al., 2018) used both cross-sectional and longitudinal design. These studies considered the differences between participants with normal-weight and participants with overweight or obesity and analyzed the differences in executive performances during the time. For this reason, they were considered in both sections of the review.

Quality Assessment for Risk Bias

Seventy-nine per cent of the studies (N = 70) were of high quality, while 20% (N = 18) were of low quality. Figure 2 shows the percentage of studies per quality level for each point on the assessment tool. Overall, studies showed higher quality in their results and discussion sections. Conversely, lower scores were found for the control of psychological and physiological variables (Figure 2). The selection of the articles for the systematic review was justified by the good quality of each study, explicitly considering the results on EFs. In general, despite a large number of the selected studies, the high quality of the studies may have reduced the risks of misinterpretation of the results.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Percentage of the studies and quality levels for each point of tool assessment.

Cross-Sectional Studies

Systematic searching gave 63 cross-sectional studies that met the inclusion criteria (see Table 3). Of these studies, twenty-nine involved adult participants (aged over 30 years), twenty examined adolescents (aged 12–22 years), two studies looked at young adults (aged 23–30 years) and finally twelve studies investigated the relationship between EFs and excessive body weight in children (aged >12 years) (see Table 3).

Only nine studies had a higher proportion of males than females (Pignatti et al., 2006; Verdejo-García et al., 2010; Gentier et al., 2013; Mole et al., 2015; Qavam et al., 2015; Reyes et al., 2015; Alarcón et al., 2016; Tsai et al., 2016; Wu et al., 2017). Furthermore, Weller et al. (2008) performed two different analyses to examine samples of males and females independently.

All the studies used BMI and the related WHO classification to assign participants to different overweight groups. For children and adolescents, the guidelines for using percentiles recommended by WHO or Center for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) were employed (Flegal et al., 2002; de Onis et al., 2007), except in two studies (Reyes et al., 2015; Goldschmidt et al., 2018) where z scores for CDC classification were used (Harrington et al., 2013).

Studies focused mainly on differences in executive functioning between individuals with obesity and normal-weight; thirteen analyzed differences between participants with normal-weight and overweight; thirteen studies investigated differences in performance between participants with obesity, overweight, and normal-weight (see Table 3).

Most of the studies reported a significant difference between the groups in executive functioning, confirming the relationship between excessive body weight and executive dysfunctions. Only thirteen studies reported no differences (Gonzales et al., 2010; Ariza et al., 2012; Delgado-Rico et al., 2012; Hendrick et al., 2012; Loeber et al., 2012; Moreno-López et al., 2012; Frank et al., 2014; Voon et al., 2014; Bongers et al., 2015; Reyes et al., 2015; Schiff et al., 2016; Sweat et al., 2017; Van der Oord et al., 2018).

Executive Functions in Cross-Sectional Studies

Cognitive Flexibility

The tasks most commonly used to assess cognitive flexibility were the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Milner, 1963), the Trail Making Test (TMT, AB) (Reitan, 1958) and the Switching of Attention Task (Rogers and Monsell, 1995) (see Table 3).

Twenty-seven studies assessed the differences between groups on cognitive flexibility (see Table 3); only eight of them found no differences in cognitive flexibility between normal-weight and overweight/obese groups (Gonzales et al., 2010; Ariza et al., 2012; Delgado-Rico et al., 2012; Catoira et al., 2016; Schiff et al., 2016; Kittel et al., 2017; Sweat et al., 2017; Dassen et al., 2018a). In general, the results showed greater difficulty in performing tasks involving this function in participants with obesity compared to those with normal-weight. Furthermore, the study by Blanco-Gómez et al. (2015) highlighted a further difference: compared with participants with overweight, participants with obesity showed higher flexibility deficits.

Inhibition

The most common cognitive tasks used to measure inhibitory control were the Stroop Color-Word Task (Stroop, 1935) and the Stop-Signal Task (Lappin and Eriksen, 1966) (see Table 3).

Forty-five studies investigated the relationship between inhibitory control and excessive body weight (see Table 3). Of these, seventeen studies reported no differences between the groups (Gonzales et al., 2010; Ariza et al., 2012; Delgado-Rico et al., 2012; Hendrick et al., 2012; Loeber et al., 2012; Moreno-López et al., 2012; Stingl et al., 2012; Fields et al., 2013; Voon et al., 2014; Yau et al., 2014; Bongers et al., 2015; Schiff et al., 2016; Wu et al., 2016; Goldschmidt et al., 2018; Pearce et al., 2018; Van der Oord et al., 2018). The remaining studies reported lower inhibitory control in obese than in normal-weight participants.

Working Memory

The Digit Span Test (in particular the Backwards version) (Reynolds, 1997), and the N-Back Test (Kane et al., 2007) were used in various versions (see Table 3) to investigate differences in working memory performance.

Of the twenty-four studies that analyzed the relation between overweight/obesity and working memory (see Table 2), six observed no differences between groups (Gonzales et al., 2010; Ariza et al., 2012; Frank et al., 2014; Restivo et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2018; Van der Oord et al., 2018). The remaining studies found that participants with overweight/obesity performed worse than normal-weight participants on working memory tasks; moreover, obese participants performed worse than participants with overweight (Coppin et al., 2014; Alarcón et al., 2016).

Decision-Making, Planning, and Problem-Solving

The tasks used to assess decision-making, planning and problem-solving were the IGT (Bechara et al., 2005) and the Delay Discounting Task (Richards et al., 1999) (see Table 3).

Twenty-six studies (see Table 3) investigated differences in performances between groups on tasks involving complex EFs such as decision-making, planning and problem-solving. Among these studies, only six (Bongers et al., 2015; Mole et al., 2015; Schiff et al., 2016; Kittel et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2018; Van der Oord et al., 2018), individuals with obesity performed worse than those with normal-weight on decision making, planning and risk-taking. Furthermore, Schiff et al. (2016), despite of they observed no clear between differences in decision-making, found that the group with obesity responded differently in terms of gratification mechanisms connected with food (as measured by the Temporal Discounting Task), in fact, they showed more sensitivity to reward stimuli than normal-weight group. Weller et al. (2008), using the Delay Discounting task, found that women affected by obesity, compared to women with normal-weight, preferred an immediate reward than a major one after some time. This difference was not observed in men (Weller et al., 2008).

Discussion

The analysis of the cross-sectional studies confirmed the existence of a relationship between overweight/obesity and EFs, even if it did not indicate the direction of this relationship. Many types of cognitive tasks were used to investigate executive functioning, but, despite this heterogeneity, the results were consistent. However, the very different demands of the tasks used did not allow determining whether one single EF is more closely involved than the others in the relationship with overweight/obesity, though the most analyzed EF related to excessive body weight is Inhibition (see Table 3). The studies that failed to confirm a relationship between EFs and overweight/obesity used a small sample size (Hendrick et al., 2012; Schiff et al., 2016), or a high number of cognitive tasks (Gonzales et al., 2010)

The present systematic review included studies that take into account people with different ages considering from children to the elderly. This choice was aimed to investigate whether the relationship between EFs and overweight/obesity presents similar characteristics, regardless of the age of the participants. The results of the review confirmed the relationship between EFs and overweight both in studies examining adults and young adults (Gunstad et al., 2007; Fagundo et al., 2012; Coppin et al., 2014) and in those that looked at children (Yadava and Sharma, 2014; Bozkurt et al., 2017) and adolescents (Nederkoorn et al., 2006; Galioto Wiedemann et al., 2014). These results prevent us from making inferences about the causality of this relationship over a lifespan but highlight the existence of a negative relationship between executive performances and overweight, regardless of the age considered.

Many studies tried to control for certain variables (gender, age, and education) that might influence executive performance, by matching samples or controlling the effects of these variables through statistical analysis (Gunstad et al., 2007; Deckers et al., 2017; Kittel et al., 2017; Perpiñá et al., 2017). This methodological aspect highlighted the existence of some dimensions (e.g., demographical variables as gender or educational level) that might influence the relation between body weight and EFs; therefore, considering these variables can contribute to further strengthen the results (Kittel et al., 2017).

Generally, the analyzed studies used suitable inclusion criteria that allow excluding individuals with chronic medical conditions, psychological diseases or eating disorders, in order to avoid an effect of these dimensions on the observed results (Fagundo et al., 2012; Galioto et al., 2013; Galioto Wiedemann et al., 2014). Moreover, in some studies, physiological differences between participants with normal-weight and overweight/obesity were reported. In particular, participants with severe obesity showed worse values, in blood pressure, cholesterol levels, insulin resistance (Maayan et al., 2011; Perpiñá et al., 2017) and levels of glycolic metabolism activation although, in the absence of pathological medical conditions in line with well-known results (Heymsfield and Wadden, 2017).

Both psychopathological and physiological aspects related to obesity, and specifically with severe obesity, have an impact on the executive functioning and consequently with the performances in executive tasks; therefore these variables should be controlled in further studies.

Although the cross-sectional studies showed no clear direction in the overweight–executive functioning relationship, many of the authors have advanced various hypotheses (Gonzales et al., 2010; Galioto Wiedemann et al., 2014). For example, Kamijo et al. (2012a) hypothesized that ineffective inhibitory control of the prefrontal cortex would cause excessive consumption of calories that is directly associated with an increase in body fat. Moreover, other authors considered also the dopaminergic mechanism involved in executive processing as related to weight variations (Arnsten and Li, 2005). Neuroimaging studies of individuals with obesity have shown an association between the hypoactivation of dopaminergic D2-receptors and a decrease in neural metabolism in the areas most involved in executive functioning (Volkow et al., 2011). Furthermore, dopamine is also implicated in the reward system (Volkow et al., 2011; Smith and Robbins, 2013). This neural system resulted impaired in individuals with excessive body weight, and alterations of this system could influence the approach to food in terms of favoring the consumption of high-calorie foods to achieve higher gratification (Schiff et al., 2016). All these findings could support theoretical models on the genesis of obesity (Davis et al., 2007b; Smith and Robbins, 2013) that view changes in executive functioning as one of the leading causes of weight gain. The hypothesis of executive dysfunctions as a cause of inappropriate eating behavior could partially support the theoretical model of Food Addiction, in which the excessive consumption of food is characterized by behavioral aspects similar to those defining other substance addiction diseases (Wang et al., 2004; Smith and Robbins, 2013).

Nevertheless, other authors viewed executive deficits as a consequence of obesity, recognizing it as a cause of neurophysiological and metabolic diseases, such as changes in insulin sensitivity (Gonzales et al., 2010), inflammatory processes as a result of body fat accumulation (Lasselin et al., 2016), and changes in cerebrovascular blood flow (Verdejo-García et al., 2010; Qavam et al., 2015). These alterations could be the cause of structural changes (e.g., a reduction of the orbitofrontal cortex) (Cohen et al., 2011) or functional changes (e.g., reduced functional connectivity of executive networks) (Tsai et al., 2016) in the cerebral areas involved in executive functioning. This vision seems to be in line with the Neuroinflammation Model (Perry, 2004) in which high BMI appears to result in systemic inflammation, which negatively affects cognitive functions including executive ones (C-reactive protein and interleukin would play an essential role in this process; Bourassa and Sbarra, 2017), and with the model proposed by Sellbom and Gunstad (2012) in which the changing in blood flow and metabolism of the frontal lobes as well as the atrophy of the frontal and temporal lobes would cause an impairment in inhibitory control resulting in an increase in overeating behaviors (Sellbom and Gunstad, 2012).

The consistent results confirming the relationship between EFs and obesity suggests that even a moderate increase in body weight may be associated with a decrease in executive performances (Verdejo-García et al., 2010; Cohen et al., 2011; Sellaro and Colzato, 2017). These views are supported by results obtained comparing groups of participants with normal-weight, overweight and obesity, in which differences in performances also emerged between overweight and obesity conditions (Galioto Wiedemann et al., 2014; Wu et al., 2017).

Another aspect highlighted by the cross-sectional studies is the role of certain psychological variables related to BMI (Catoira et al., 2016; Restivo et al., 2017) that appear to modulate the relationship between EFs and excessive body weight. Indeed, the presence of high levels of anxiety and depression in individuals with obesity, even in the absence of established psychopathologies, appears to result in worse executive performances (Restivo et al., 2017). These findings could be linked to the theoretical model of Emotionally-Driven Eating (Dallman, 2010), which postulated that overeating, related to overweight, is a dysfunctional attempt to regulate emotions in people characterized by a deficit in emotion regulation.

Longitudinal Studies

Our systematic search allows selecting twenty-eight longitudinal studies investigating executive functioning in individuals with overweight or obesity (see Table 4). Of these, eighteen examined adult participants (aged more than 30 years), five looked at children (aged <12 years) and five involved adolescents (aged 12–22 years) (see Table 4).

All studies used BMI to classify overweight and obesity, although z-scores (Davis et al., 2007a, 2011; Augustijn et al., 2018), percentiles (Pauli-Pott et al., 2010), or adapted BMI scores (Verbeken et al., 2014) were used in studies involving children.

Five studies (Alosco et al., 2014a; Kulendran et al., 2017; Xie et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2017; Stinson et al., 2018) reported having a significantly higher percentage of males than females in their sample.

Twelve studies (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Davis et al., 2007a, 2011; Pauli-Pott et al., 2010; Witbracht et al., 2012; Kulendran et al., 2014; Galioto et al., 2016; Demos et al., 2017; Xie et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2017; Augustijn et al., 2018; Vantieghem et al., 2018) analyzed the effects of non-invasive programmes aimed at weight-loss on the relationship between BMI and EFs: some interventions integrated various modalities of treatment, specifically diet and physical activity (Pauli-Pott et al., 2010; Kulendran et al., 2014; Galioto et al., 2016; Demos et al., 2017; Xie et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2017; Vantieghem et al., 2018); while others focused only on diet programmes (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Witbracht et al., 2012) or physical activity (Davis et al., 2007a, 2011). Furthermore, two studies (Kulendran et al., 2014; Augustijn et al., 2018) provided residential interventions, with treatment lasting from four (Davis et al., 2011) to fifty-2 weeks (Pauli-Pott et al., 2010). In all the studies, at least two measurements were taken: one before and one after the procedure.

Ten studies examined the effects of bariatric surgery on the executive functioning in participants with severe obesity (Spitznagel et al., 2013, 2014; Alosco et al., 2014a,c,d, 2015; Galioto et al., 2015; Kulendran et al., 2017; Pearce et al., 2017). The analysis of EFs was performed before surgery and at follow-up, with time intervals ranging from 12 weeks (Spitznagel et al., 2013, 2014) to 48 months (Alosco et al., 2014d). In some cases, more than one follow-up was carried out (Spitznagel et al., 2013, 2014; Alosco et al., 2014d).

All the studies investigating weight reduction in participants with obesity reported a general improvement in EF performances. Only Pearce et al. (2017) failed to detect any significant changes in performances.

Four studies assessed the effects of cognitive interventions on EFs in obese participants, showed a general improvement in executive performances associated with a reduction in body weight. Specifically, two studies evaluated the benefits of Cognitive Remediation Therapy (Alosco et al., 2014b; Allom et al., 2018), one assessed the impact of an intervention focused on Working Memory (Galioto et al., 2015) and one focused on the effects of a treatment aimed at strengthening cognitive functions in general (Verbeken et al., 2014).

Two further studies analyzed the trend over time of body weight and executive functioning in adults with obesity (Deckers et al., 2017; Stinson et al., 2018) without introducing weight reduction programs and reported inconsistent results. Deckers et al. (2017) found no relationship between weight changes and executive performance, while Stinson et al. (2018) found evidence of the role of EFs, specifically of reduced inhibitory control, in maintaining high body weight.

Eleven studies (Pauli-Pott et al., 2010; Spitznagel et al., 2013, 2014; Kulendran et al., 2014; Galioto et al., 2015, 2016; Xu et al., 2017; Augustijn et al., 2018; Dassen et al., 2018a; Stinson et al., 2018) investigated the predictive role of performance on executive tasks on body weight changes, and observed that appropriate executive functioning predicted a reduction in body weight in participants with obesity or overweight.

Executive Functions in Longitudinal Studies

Cognitive Flexibility

The tasks most commonly used to assess cognitive flexibility were the WCST, TMT, and Switching of Attention Task (see Table 4).

Of the eleven studies that investigated the relationship between cognitive flexibility and obesity (see Table 4), six (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Alosco et al., 2014c; Spitznagel et al., 2014; Deckers et al., 2017; Augustijn et al., 2018; Stinson et al., 2018) failed to confirm this relationship. Those that found an association between obesity and executive functioning reported an improvement in performance as a result of weight reduction. Furthermore, negative performance appeared to be associated with less weight reduction over time (Spitznagel et al., 2013; Augustijn et al., 2018).

Inhibition

The Stroop Color-Word Task and Stop-Signal Task were the tests most commonly used to investigate cognitive and motor inhibition (see Table 4).

Thirteen studies reported a relationship between BMI and cognitive inhibition (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Pauli-Pott et al., 2010; Alosco et al., 2014d, 2015; Kulendran et al., 2014, 2017; Galioto et al., 2015, 2016; Xie et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2017; Augustijn et al., 2018; Stinson et al., 2018; Vantieghem et al., 2018). Some of these studies showed that inhibition control predicted a reduction of body weight considering both bariatric surgery (Kulendran et al., 2017) and weight loss programs (Pauli-Pott et al., 2010; Kulendran et al., 2014; Galioto et al., 2016; Xu et al., 2017; Augustijn et al., 2018; Stinson et al., 2018). Other studies showed an improvement in the inhibition after bariatric surgery (Alosco et al., 2014d, 2015; Galioto et al., 2015) or weight-loss programs (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Xie et al., 2017; Vantieghem et al., 2018).

Working Memory

The task most often used to investigate working memory was the Digit Span Test (Reynolds, 1997) (see Table 4).

Eight studies reported a negative relationship between working memory and body weight (Spitznagel et al., 2013, 2014; Alosco et al., 2014b,d, 2015; Galioto et al., 2015; Augustijn et al., 2018; Dassen et al., 2018b). Indeed some authors found an improvement of the performance in working memory tasks after bariatric surgery (Alosco et al., 2014b,d, 2015; Galioto et al., 2015), while other authors found a predictive role of working memory performance in the outcome of weight reduction programs (Augustijn et al., 2018; Dassen et al., 2018b) or bariatric surgery (Spitznagel et al., 2013, 2014); better performance predicted success of interventions. Conversely, three studies found no relationship between obesity and working memory (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Galioto et al., 2016; Pearce et al., 2017).

Decision-Making

Decision-making, as measured using the Iowa Gambling Task, did not appear to be directly associated with weight reduction in patients with obesity and overweight (Witbracht et al., 2012; Stinson et al., 2018). Only Demos et al. (2017) observed an improvement in decision-making following a reduction in body weight, but these authors used a task that employed food-related stimuli.

Discussion

The results of the longitudinal studies confirmed the findings reported in cross-sectional studies, highlighting a relationship between executive functioning and overweight/obesity even if the direction of this relationship remains unclear.

Studies that analyzed the effects of treatments aimed at reducing body weight showed a general improvement in executive tasks as a result. This improvement appeared to occur both in adult populations (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Witbracht et al., 2012) and in children and adolescents (Davis et al., 2011; Kulendran et al., 2014; Vantieghem et al., 2018). Moreover, studies focused on the ability of executive functioning to predict the success of weight-loss interventions found that higher executive functioning could be the cause of BMI reduction (see Table 4). Not all EFs appear to be related to obesity. Such inconsistency in the results could be due to different versions of the tasks used to evaluate EFs, as shown in the studies analyzed decision-making.

As for the cross-sectional studies, the authors interpreted the results based on two different types of theoretical models. One hypothesizes that excessive body weight is the cause of changes in executive functioning, according to results showing an improvement in executive tasks following treatment for weight loss (Davis et al., 2007a, 2011; Alosco et al., 2014a,b,c,d, 2015; Verbeken et al., 2014; Galioto et al., 2015; Demos et al., 2017; Xie et al., 2017; Vantieghem et al., 2018). In the other theoretical view, EFs are considered as predictors of eating behaviors related to excessive body weight, like overeating. Studies assessing the effects of strengthening EFs in participants with overweight or obesity (Verbeken et al., 2014; Allom et al., 2018; Dassen et al., 2018b; Raman et al., 2018) have observed both an increase in executive functioning and a reduction in BMI. This reduction may be due to improved eating behavior as a result of adequate working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control. These enforcement functions would promote healthier behaviors, reducing the risk associated with obesity, and further improve weight reduction (Allom et al., 2018). Studies that have shown the predictive role of the EFs on the success of weight-loss treatments (Pauli-Pott et al., 2010; Spitznagel et al., 2013, 2014; Kulendran et al., 2014, 2017; Galioto et al., 2016; Xu et al., 2017; Augustijn et al., 2018) confirmed the critical role of executive functioning in the occurrence of obesity.

Concerning bariatric surgery, the effects of weight-loss on executive performances resulted only at the follow-up (Spitznagel et al., 2013; Alosco et al., 2014a; Pearce et al., 2017). This result could be interpreted in two ways. On the one hand, it may suggest that a reduction in body fat favors improvement in executive functioning (Alosco et al., 2014b) as a consequence of the resolution of metabolic alterations related to excessive BMI; on the other hand, better performance at baseline could lead to an improvement in healthy eating habits (Spitznagel et al., 2013; Pearce et al., 2017), linked to a reduction of BMI over time. This last interpretation is supported by the results observed at the follow-up that showed a higher reduction in BMI in participants presenting better EFs performance at baseline (Spitznagel et al., 2013; Pearce et al., 2017). Lastly, it is interesting to note that control groups with obesity that did not benefit from the treatments (Bryan and Tiggemann, 2001; Alosco et al., 2014c; Pearce et al., 2017; Xie et al., 2017) did not show improvement in performance on cognitive tasks in the follow up assessment. These results confirm that a reduction in body fat leads to gains in executive functioning, although the groups that did not benefit from the treatment did not show a further executive decline.

Despite these findings, short-term follow-ups showed no evidence of a causal relationship of EFs on obesity. These studies did not observe significant differences between participants with obesity who have reduced their body weight and those who maintained their condition unchanged (Deckers et al., 2017). In line with these results, we can conclude that the relationship between EFs and excessive body weight appears robust even when longitudinal studies are considered. However, even considering the results of longitudinal studies appear challenging to determine the direction of this relationship, and further studies are needed.

General Discussion

Only in recent years, the studies focused their attention on the relationship between excessive body weight and EFs (Fitzpatrick et al., 2013). This relationship appears to be confirmed by most of the studies, both cross-sectional (e.g., Verdejo-García et al., 2010; Cohen et al., 2011; Maayan et al., 2011; Dassen et al., 2018a) and longitudinal (e.g., Spitznagel et al., 2013; Alosco et al., 2014d, 2015; Augustijn et al., 2018), analyzed in this systematic review, despite the heterogeneity of the tasks used and the methodological framework adopted. Functional and neuroimaging studies confirmed changes in the cortical areas involved in executive functioning in participants with obesity (Stingl et al., 2012; Alarcón et al., 2016; Tsai et al., 2016) even when cognitive tasks failed to highlight any significant differences in performance between obesity and normal-weight conditions (Hendrick et al., 2012; Frank et al., 2014; Pearce et al., 2017).

The choice to selected studies which considered different aged made us possible to highlight a similar pattern in the relationship between EFs and overweight/obesity in children (Blanco-Gómez et al., 2015; Tsai et al., 2016) and adults (Cohen et al., 2011; Deckers et al., 2017), despite the individual differences linked to age.

This systematic review allowed us to observe poor performance on executive function tasks also in people with overweight, not only in those with obesity (Verdejo-García et al., 2010; Sellaro and Colzato, 2017), although only a few studies have investigated the condition of overweight (BMI between 25 and 30) compared to normal-weight (BMI lower than 25) and obesity (BMI higher than 30). These results should be explored in further studies to verify how executive functioning is expressed at the different stages of overweight and to understand if the early intervention could prevent the worsening of the increase in adiposity.

As previously reported, the results of these studies have been interpreted according to two different theoretical models. At the conclusion of this systematic review, no single theoretical model appears to prevail. The empirical data seem to support both theoretical models: the one postulating the influence of executive system dysfunctions on obesity (Drewnowski, 1997; Goldstone et al., 2009; Smith and Robbins, 2013), the other viewing impairment of executive functioning as a consequence of the obesity (Ricca et al., 2009; Pieper and Laugero, 2013).

Other longitudinal studies are needed to disentangle the relationship between obesity and executive dysfunctions. These studies could either examine the eating behavior and BMI of people with low executive functioning over time or monitor the executive functionality of people with overweight who become obese over time. Finally, the possibility that the relationship between executive dysfunctions and overweight/obesity could be bidirectional cannot be excluded; in fact, many studies seem to suggest that the bidirectionality is the real nature of this relation (Spitznagel et al., 2013; Augustijn et al., 2018; Raman et al., 2018).

This systematic review has some strengths, such as the decision to exclude studies of children of preschool age (younger than 5 years) and those over 70 years. This decision was taken for various reasons. First, the EFs and underlying neural areas of preschool-age children are immature and still developing (Diamond, 2013); moreover, during this period children are still introjecting eating habits learned from the external environment (Guxens et al., 2009; Gregory et al., 2010). However, previous studies have shown a specific predictive value of EFs performance in preschool children concerning weight and eating behaviors (Park et al., 2014), and for this reason, it would be interesting to study this specific age group separately. The decision to not include studies of people over 70 years of age was influenced by the “obesity paradox” hypothesis (Artham et al., 2008; Park et al., 2014), which recognizes the health benefits to older people in having a higher BMI. Furthermore, impairment of EFs in older people can be associated with the aging process (Fjell et al., 2016). Although analysis of the relationship between EFs and overweight/obesity in these two age groups could be interesting, their inclusion in the present study would have led to extreme heterogeneity.

Limitations

This systematic review was not able to identify if one specific EF had a more significant role than another on the analyzed relationship. This result could represent a limitation of this study because it has not allowed us to establish whether differences in performance were due to changes in some functions rather than others. This limitation is due mainly to the heterogeneity of cognitive tasks (Yang et al., 2018). Another limit could be represented by the selection of participants from 5–70 years; in fact, also if the results are coherent, it is known that the brain continues to develop from childhood to young adulthood, and the differences related to aging could influence the relation between cognitive aspects and weight changes. These age-related differences may have covered possible results that could indicate a causal direction between the variables.

Considering the longitudinal studies, the most extended follow-up period—of 4 years—was performed by Alosco et al. (2014d), though with considerable data loss. No other study investigated the relationship between EFs and body weight following body loss treatment over such a long time. This aspect represents a further limitation of the results, i.e., it is not clear whether the improvements were sustained over time or whether a subsequent reversal of the trend occurs, which might have been the reason behind the drop-out from treatment among bariatric patients. Besides, a possible change in the trend over time could indicate that it is the executive damage that influences the success of weight-loss interventions.

Another limitation is represented by not having included in the systematic review the analysis of psychological variables that could modulate the relationship between EFs and excessive body weight. In fact, a few of the selected studies controlled the psychological dimensions, like anxiety, depression, or emotional regulation. It would be interesting to carry out an analysis of these dimensions. One of the studies that considered some emotional components showed that emotions could modulate the relationship between executive functioning and obesity. However, others found no effect of psychological variables on this relationship (Yau et al., 2014). Even so, examining these psychological variables might also lead to a better understanding of the Emotionally-Driven Eating Model (Chen et al., 2017) in individuals with no eating disorders.

Further limitations are due to the limited samples considered by both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies and by the higher prevalence of females among the participants that do not allow to generalize the results. Weller et al. (2008) found different results between males and females on EF performances, and further studies would be useful to analyse gender differences.

A significant limitation of this work concerns the lack of studies comparing participants with overweight and obesity separately. This comparison would have allowed us to examine the relationship between different severities of excessive body weight and the impairment of executive functioning, and to identify the nature of this relationship. Furthermore, a specific focus on participants with overweight would also have led to determine the cognitive characteristics that might serve as warning signs of the development of obesity.

Finally, a meta-analysis measuring the statistical power of the results obtained from the studies analyzed in this study could help to interpret better the results obtained in this systematic review.

Conclusions

The analysis of studies on the relationship between executive functioning and excessive body weight did not give us decisive responses to all the questions advanced by this systematic review but clarified a large part of the issues on this topic. A consistent relationship between executive functioning and overweight/obesity has been confirmed, but it remains unclear whether a general executive dysfunction is involved or whether one EF is more implicated than others.

Although it was not possible to confirm a specific theoretical model on the relationship between EFs and overweight/obesity, the association between these dimensions appears to be the result of a complex interaction between different factors that influence both people's attitude to food and eating and their executive functioning. Prolonged inappropriate food intake related to the maintenance of excessive body weight leads to poorer performance on executive tasks. Furthermore, executive impairment exacerbates inappropriate behaviors, leading to increased body fat (Stinson et al., 2018). Both these aspects are associated with a real risk of cognitive impairment in old age (Sanderlin et al., 2017) and difficulty in responding appropriately to external stimuli that is typical of executive dysfunctions and which would negatively affect the life of obese individuals. It is essential to intervene in both these dimensions to reduce the impact of obesity on quality of life.

It would be interesting in future to evaluate the effectiveness of long-term interventions involving weight-loss programmes. The success of weight-loss interventions may be strictly linked to an improvement in executive functioning because effective executive skills would allow healthier lifestyles. In this context, it might be useful to examine the integrated model of Sellbom and Gunstad (2012) and the Emotionally-Driven Eating model (Gianini et al., 2013; Wagner et al., 2013) in terms of the relationship between BMI and cognitive functioning variables such as mood and emotional regulation that were not often analyzed in the studies reviewed here.

The leading role of this systematic review was to underline the powerful connection between cognitive aspects, specifically EFs, and excessive body weight, highlighting the importance of considering the nature of the link between these variables in studies on overeating and obesity. A relevant suggestion that emerges by this study is the need for longitudinal studies which, starting from the analysis of EFs, monitor the BMI over time.

It could be essential for structuring intervention aimed at enhancing EFs to prevent the drop-out rate among patients with severe obesity who fail to benefit for a long time from the effects of treatments (Galioto et al., 2016; Xu et al., 2017), by favoring the long-term maintenance of the lower weight achieved. It needs to reduce the risk of further weight gain in people with overweight, thereby preventing the occurrence of severe obesity. Moreover, an integrated approach that also takes emotion regulation and mood into account could be the best strategy for countering dysfunctional eating behaviors and executive functioning; therefore it would be necessary to develop an integrated theoretical model that should jointly consider EFs, eating behavior, emotion regulation, and mood in the field of overweight and obesity.

Author Contributions

MC and FF conception of the review, the literature search, and the writing of the manuscript. GF resolved disagreements in the choice of the articles and also contributed to the discussions of the analyzed searches and the revisions of the manuscript. The authors revised, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Ph.D Founding Program, Departement of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Alarcón, G., Ray, S., and Nagel, B. J. (2016). Lower working memory performance in overweight and obese adolescents is mediated by white matter microstructure. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 22, 281–292. doi: 10.1017/S135561771500q5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allom, V., Mullan, B., Smith, E., Hay, P., and Raman, J. (2018). Breaking bad habits by improving executive function in individuals with obesity. BMC Public Health 18:505. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5392-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alosco, M. L., Galioto, R., Spitznagel, M. B., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Cohen, R., et al. (2014a). Cognitive function after bariatric surgery: evidence for improvement 3 years after surgery. Am. J. Surg. 207, 870–876. doi: 10.1016/j.amjsurg.2013.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alosco, M. L., Spitznagel, M. B., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Cohen, R., Crosby, R. D., et al. (2014c). The effects of cystatin C and alkaline phosphatase changes on cognitive function 12-months after bariatric surgery. J. Neurol. Sci. 345, 176–180. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2014.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alosco, M. L., Spitznagel, M. B., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Cohen, R., Crosby, R. D., et al. (2015). Improved serum leptin and ghrelin following bariatric surgery predict better postoperative cognitive function. J. Clin. Neurol. 11, 48–56. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2015.11.1.48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alosco, M. L., Spitznagel, M. B., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Cohen, R., Paul, R., et al. (2014b). Improved memory function two years after bariatric surgery. Obesity 22, 32–38. doi: 10.1002/oby.20494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alosco, M. L., Spitznagel, M. B., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Crosby, R. D., Mitchell, J. E., et al. (2014d). Family history of Alzheimer's disease limits improvement in cognitive function after bariatric surgery. SAGE Open Med. 2:2050312114539477. doi: 10.1177/2050312114539477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ariza, M., Garolera, M., Jurado, M. A., Garcia-Garcia, I., Hernan, I., Sánchez-Garre, C., et al. (2012). Dopamine genes (DRD2/ANKK1-TaqA1 and DRD4-7R) and executive function: their interaction with obesity. PLoS ONE 7:e41482. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arnsten, A. F., and Li, B. M. (2005). Neurobiology of executive functions: catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1377–1384. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artham, S. M., Lavie, C. J., Milani, R. V., and Ventura, H. O. (2008). The obesity paradox: impact of obesity on the prevalence and prognosis of cardiovascular diseases. Postgrad. Med. 120, 34–41. doi: 10.3810/pgm.2008.07.1788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashwell, M., Gunn, P., and Gibson, S. (2012). Waist-to-height ratio is a better screening tool than waist circumference and BMI for adult cardiometabolic risk factors: systematic review and meta-analysis. Obesity Rev. 13, 275–286. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00952.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Augustijn, M. J. C. M., D'Hondt, E., Van Acker, L., De Guchtenaere, A., Lenoir, M., Caeyenberghs, K., et al. (2018). Role of motor competence and executive functioning in weight loss: a study in children with obesity. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 39, 642–651. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, L. O., and Manning, K. J. (2016). Challenges in the detection of working memory and attention decrements among overweight adolescent girls. Neuropsychobiology 73, 43–51. doi: 10.1159/000442670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (2005). The iowa gambling task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 159–162. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanco-Gómez, A., Ferré, N., Luque, V., Cardona, M., Gispert-Llauradó, M., Escribano, J., et al. (2015). Being overweight or obese is associated with inhibition control in children from six to ten years of age. Acta Paediatr. 104, 619–625. doi: 10.1111/apa.12976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bongers, P., van de Giessen, E., Roefs, A., Nederkoorn, C., Booij, J., van den Brink, W., et al. (2015). Being impulsive and obese increases susceptibility to speeded detection of high-calorie foods. Health Psychol. 34, 677–685. doi: 10.1037/hea0000167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourassa, K., and Sbarra, D. A. (2017). Body mass and cognitive decline are indirectly associated via inflammation among aging adults. Brain Behav. Immun. 60, 63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozkurt, H., Özer, S., Yilmaz, R., Sönmezgöz, E., Kazanci, Ö., Erbaş, O., et al. (2017). Assessment of neurocognitive functions in children and adolescents with obesity. Appl. Neuropsychol. 6, 262–268. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2016.1150184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bray, G. A. (1999). Etiology and pathogenesis of obesity. Clin. Cornerstone 2, 1–15. doi: 10.1016/S1098-3597(99)90001-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brogan, A., Hevey, D., O'Callaghan, G., Yoder, R., and O'Shea, D. (2011). Impaired decision making among morbidly obese adults. J. Psychosom. Res. 70, 189–196. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryan, J., and Tiggemann, M. (2001). The effect of weight-loss dieting on cognitive performance and psychological well-being in overweight women. Appetite 36, 147–156. doi: 10.1006/appe.2000.0389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo, D., Galioto, R., Gunstad, J., and Spitznagel, M. B. (2014). Uncontrolled eating is associated with reduced executive functioning. Clin. Obes. 4, 172–179. doi: 10.1111/cob.12058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, M., Small, H., Yoong, S. L., Boyes, A., Bisquera, A., and Sanson-Fisher, R. (2014). Prevalence of comorbid depression and obesity in general practice: a cross-sectional survey. Br. J. Generic Pract. 64, e122–e127. doi: 10.3399/bjgp14X677482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpiniello, B., Pinna, F., Pillai, G., Nonnoi, V., Pisano, E., Corrias, S., et al. (2009). Obesity and psychopathology. A study of psychiatric comorbidity among patients attending a specialist obesity unit. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. 18, 119–127. doi: 10.1017/S1121189X00001007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catoira, N. P., Tapajóz, F., Allegri, R. F., Lajfer, J., Rodríguez Cámara, M., Iturry, M. L., et al. (2016). Obesity, metabolic profile, and inhibition failure: young women under scrutiny. Physiol. Behav. 157, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.01.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, R. C., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., and Chen, E. Y. (2008). Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, P. A., Chavez, R. S., and Heatherton, T. F. (2017). Structural integrity between executive control and reward regions of the brain predicts body fat percentage in chronic dieters. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 162–166. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2016.1235556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. I., Yates, K. F., Duong, M., and Convit, A. (2011). Obesity, orbitofrontal structure and function are associated with food choice: a cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 1:e000175. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2011-000175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppin, G., Nolan-Poupart, S., Jones-Gotman, M., and Small, D. M. (2014). Working memory and reward association learning impairments in obesity. Neuropsychologia 65, 146–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallman, M. F. (2010). Stress-induced obesity and the emotional nervous system. Trends Endocrinol. Metabol. 21, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R. (1995). On some functions of the human prefrontal cortex. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 769, 241–252. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1995.tb38142.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danner, U. N., Ouwehand, C., van Haastert, N. L., Hornsveld, H., and de Ridder, D. T. (2012). Decision-making impairments in women with binge eating disorder in comparison with obese and normal weight women. Eur. Eating Disord. Rev. 20, e56–e62. doi: 10.1002/erv.1098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dassen, F. C., Houben, K., Van Breukelen, G. J., and Jansen, A. (2018b). Gamified working memory training in overweight individuals reduces food intake but not body weight. Appetite 124, 89–98. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dassen, F. C. M., Houben, K., Allom, V., and Jansen, A. (2018a). Self-regulation and obesity: the role of executive function and delay discounting in the prediction of weight loss. J. Behav. Med. 41, 806–818. doi: 10.1007/s10865-018-9940-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C., Patte, K., Levitan, R., Reid, C., Tweed, S., and Curtis, C. (2007b). From motivation to behaviour: a model of reward sensitivity, overeating, and food preferences in the risk profile for obesity. Appetite 48, 12–19. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., Boyle, C. A., Waller, J. L., Miller, P. H., Naglieri, J. A., et al. (2007a). Effects of aerobic exercise on overweight children's cognitive functioning: a randomized controlled trial. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 78, 510–519. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2007.10599450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 30, 91–98. doi: 10.1037/a0021766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Onis, M. D., Onyango, A. W., Borghi, E., Siyam, A., Nishida, C., and Siekmann, J. (2007). Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bull. World Health Organ. 85, 660–667. doi: 10.2471/BLT.07.043497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Wit, L., Luppino, F., van Straten, A., Penninx, B., Zitman, F., and Cuijpers, P. (2010). Depression and obesity: a meta-analysis of community-based studies. Psychiatr Res. 178, 230–235. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deckers, K., van Boxtel, M. P. J., Verhey, F. R. J., and Köhler, S. (2017). Obesity and cognitive decline in adults: effect of methodological choices and confounding by age in a longitudinal study. J. Nutri. Health Aging 21, 546–553. doi: 10.1007/s12603-016-0757-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delgado-Rico, E., Río-Valle, J. S., González-Jiménez, E., Campoy, C., and Verdejo-García, A. (2012). BMI predicts emotion-driven impulsivity and cognitive inflexibility in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity 20, 1604–1610. doi: 10.1038/oby.2012.47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demos, K. E., McCaffery, J. M., Thomas, J. G., Mailloux, K. A., Hare, T. A., and Wing, R. R. (2017). Identifying the mechanisms through which behavioral weight-loss treatment improves food decision-making in obesity. Appetite 114, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dohle, S., Diel, K., and Hofmann, W. (2018). Executive functions and the self-regulation of eating behavior: a review. Appetite 124, 4–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drewnowski, A. (1997). Taste preferences and food intake. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 17, 237–253. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.17.1.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubbert, P. M., Carithers, T., Hall, J. E., Barbour, K. A., Clark, B. L., Sumner, A. E., et al. (2002). Obesity, physical inactivity, and risk for cardiovascular disease. Am. J. Med. Sci. 324, 116–126. doi: 10.1097/00000441-200209000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliott, R. (2003). Executive functions and their disorders: Imaging in clinical neuroscience. Br. Med. Bull. 65, 49–59. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.49

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, R. L., and Levine, M. D. (2017). Questionnaire and behavioral task measures of impulsivity are differentially associated with body mass index: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 143, 868–902. doi: 10.1037/bul0000105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagundo, A. B., De la Torre, R., Jiménez-Murcia, S., Agüera, Z., Granero, R., Tárrega, S., et al. (2012). Executive functions profile in extreme eating/weight conditions: from anorexia nervosa to obesity. PLoS ONE 7:e43382. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fields, S. A., Sabet, M., and Reynolds, B. (2013). Dimensions of impulsive behavior in obese, overweight, and healthy-weight adolescents. Appetite 70, 60–66. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzpatrick, S., Gilbert, S., and Serpell, L. (2013). Systematic review: are overweight and obese individuals impaired on behavioural tasks of executive functioning? Neuropsychol. Rev. 23, 138–156. doi: 10.1007/s11065-013-9224-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., Sneve, M. H., Grydeland, H., Storsve, A. B., and Walhovd, K. B. (2016). The disconnected brain and executive function decline in aging. Cereb. Cortex 27, 2303–2317. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flegal, K. M., Wei, R., and Ogden, C. (2002). Weight-for-stature compared with body mass index–for-age growth charts for the United States from the Centers for Disease control and prevention. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 75, 761–766. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/75.4.761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forcano, L., Mata, F., de la Torre, R., and Verdejo-Garcia, A. (2018). Cognitive and neuromodulation strategies for unhealthy eating and obesity: systematic review and discussion of neurocognitive mechanisms. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 87, 161–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frank, S., Wilms, B., Veit, R., Ernst, B., Thurnheer, M., Kullmann, S., et al. (2014). Altered brain activity in severely obese women may recover after Roux-en Y gastric bypass surgery. Int. J. Obes. 38, 341–348. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funahashi, S. (2001). Neuronal mechanisms of executive control by the prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Res. 39, 147–165. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(00)00224-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galioto Wiedemann, R., Calvo, D., Meister, J., and Spitznagel, M. B. (2014). Self-reported physical activity is associated with cognitive function in lean, but not obese individuals. Clin. Obes. 4, 309–315. doi: 10.1111/cob.12071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galioto, R., Alosco, M. L., Spitznagel, M. B., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Cohen, R., et al. (2015). Glucose regulation and cognitive function after bariatric surgery. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 37, 402–413. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1023264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galioto, R., Bond, D., Gunstad, J., Pera, V., Rathier, L., and Tremont, G. (2016). Executive functions predict weight loss in a medically supervised weight loss programme. Obesity Sci. Pract. 2, 334–340. doi: 10.1002/osp4.70

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galioto, R. M., Alosco, M. L., Spitznagel, M. B., Stanek, K. M., and Gunstad, J. (2013). Cognitive reserve preserves cognitive function in obese individuals. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 20, 684–699. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2012.762972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gameiro, F., Perea, M. V., Ladera, V., Rosa, B., and García, R. (2017). Executive functioning in obese individuals waiting for clinical treatment. Psicothema 29, 61–66. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gariepy, G., Nitka, D., and Schmitz, N. (2010). The association between obesity and anxiety disorders in the population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Obes. 34, 407–419. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentier, I., Augustijn, M., Deforche, B., Tanghe, A., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., Lenoir, M., et al. (2013). A comparative study of performance in simple and choice reaction time tasks between obese and healthy-weight children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 2635–2641. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gettens, K. M., and Gorin, A. A. (2017). Executive function in weight loss and weight loss maintenance: a conceptual review and novel neuropsychological model of weight control. J. Behav. Med. 40, 687–701. doi: 10.1007/s10865-017-9831-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gianini, L. M., White, M. A., and Masheb, R. M. (2013). Eating pathology, emotion regulation, and emotional overeating in obese adults with binge eating disorder. Eat. Behav. 14, 309–313. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluck, M. E., Viswanath, P., and Stinson, E. J. (2017). Obesity, appetite, and the prefrontal cortex. Curr. Obes. Rep. 6, 380–388. doi: 10.1007/s13679-017-0289-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldschmidt, A. B., O'Brien, S., Lavender, J. M., Pearson, C. M., Le Grange, D., and Hunter, S. J. (2018). Executive functioning in a racially diverse sample of children who are overweight and at risk for eating disorders. Appetite 124, 43–49. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstone, A. P., Prechtl de Hernandez, C. G., Beaver, J. D., Muhammed, K., Croese, C., Bell, G., et al. (2009). Fasting biases brain reward systems towards high-calorie foods. Eur. J. Neurosci. 30, 1625–1635. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06949.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzales, M. M., Tarumi, T., Miles, S. C., Tanaka, H., Shah, F., and Haley, A. P. (2010). Insulin sensitivity as a mediator of the relationship between BMI and working memory-related brain activation. Obesity 18, 2131–2137. doi: 10.1038/oby.2010.183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grafman, J., and Litvan, I. (1999). Importance of deficits in executive functions. Lancet 354, 1921–1923. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)90438-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregory, J. E., Paxton, S. J., and Brozovic, A. M. (2010). Maternal feeding practices, child eating behaviour and body mass index in preschool-aged children: a prospective analysis. Int. J. Behav. Nutri. Phys. Activity 7:55. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunstad, J., Paul, R. H., Cohen, R. A., Tate, D. F., Spitznagel, M. B., and Gordon, E. (2007). Elevated body mass index is associated with executive dysfunction in otherwise healthy adults. Compr. Psychiatr. 48, 57–61. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guxens, M., Mendez, M. A., Julvez, J., Plana, E., Forns, J., Basagaña, X., et al. (2009). Cognitive function and overweight in preschool children. Am. J. Epidemiol. 170, 438–446. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, P. A., and Marteau, T. M. (2014). Executive function in the context of chronic disease prevention: theory, research and practice. Prevent. Med. 68, 44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrington, D. M., Staiano, A. E., Broyles, S. T., Gupta, A. K., and Katzmarzyk, P. T. (2013). BMI percentiles for the identification of abdominal obesity and metabolic risk in children and adolescents: evidence in support of the CDC 95th percentile. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 67, 218–222. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2012.203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauner, H. (2017). “Obesity and diabetes,” in Textbook of Diabetes, eds R. I. G. Holt, C. Cockram, A. Flyvbjerg, and B. J. Goldstein, 215–228.

Google Scholar

Hendrick, O. M., Luo, X., Zhang, S., and Li, C. S. (2012). Saliency processing and obesity: a preliminary imaging study of the stop signal task. Obesity 20, 1796–1802. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heymsfield, S. B., and Wadden, T. A. (2017). Mechanisms, pathophysiology, and management of obesity. N. England J. Med. 376, 254–266. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1514009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jacques, S., and Marcovitch, S. (2010). “Development of executive function across the life span,” in The Handbook of Life-Span Development, Vol. 1: Cognition, Biology, and Methods, eds W. F. Overton and R. M. Lerner (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc.), 431–466. doi: 10.1002/9780470880166.hlsd001013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, I., Katzmarzyk, P. T., and Ross, R. (2004). Waist circumference and not body mass index explains obesity-related health risk. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 79, 379–384. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/79.3.379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, S. Z., Lu, W., Zong, X. F., Ruan, H. Y., and Liu, Y. (2016). Obesity and hypertension. Exp. Ther. Med. 12, 2395–2399. doi: 10.3892/etm.2016.3667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamijo, K., Khan, N. A., Pontifex, M. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., Raine, L. B., et al. (2012a). The relation of adiposity to cognitive control and scholastic achievement in preadolescent children. Obesity 20, 2406–2411. doi: 10.1038/oby.2012.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., et al. (2012b). The association of childhood obesity to neuroelectric indices of inhibition. Psychophysiology 49, 1361–1371. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01459.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., et al. (2012c). The negative association of childhood obesity to cognitive control of action monitoring. Cereb. Cortex 24, 654–662. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Miura, T. K., and Colflesh, G. J. (2007). Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity. J. Exp. Psychol. 33:615. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, T., Yang, W., Chen, C. S., Reynolds, K., and He, J. (2008). Global burden of obesity in 2005 and projections to 2030. Int. J. Obes. 32, 1431–1437. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kittel, R., Schmidt, R., and Hilbert, A. (2017). Executive functions in adolescents with binge-eating disorder and obesity. Int. J. Eating Disord. 50, 933–941. doi: 10.1002/eat.22714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolotkin, R. L., Meter, K., and Williams, G. R. (2001). Quality of life and obesity. Obesity Rev. 2, 219–229. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-789X.2001.00040.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulendran, M., Borovoi, L., Purkayastha, S., Darzi, A., and Vlaev, I. (2017). Impulsivity predicts weight loss after obesity surgery. Surg. Obesity Related Dis. 13, 1033–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2016.12.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulendran, M., Vlaev, I., Sugden, C., King, D., Ashrafian, H., Gately, P., et al. (2014). Neuropsychological assessment as a predictor of weight loss in obese adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 38, 507–512. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, B. C. C., Koh, G. C. H., Chen, C., Wong, M. T. K., and Fallows, S. J. (2015). Comparison of body mass index (BMI), body adiposity index (BAI), waist circumference (WC), waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) and waist-to-height ratio (WHtR) as predictors of cardiovascular disease risk factors in an adult population in Singapore. PLoS ONE 10:e0122985. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lappin, J. S., and Eriksen, C. W. (1966). Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. J. Exp. Psychol. 72, 805–811. doi: 10.1037/h0021266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasselin, J., Magne, E., Beau, C., Aubert, A., Dexpert, S., Carrez, J., et al. (2016). Low-grade inflammation is a major contributor of impaired attentional set shifting in obese subjects. Brain Behav. Immun. 58, 63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, J., Matheson, B. E., Kaye, W. H., and Boutelle, K. N. (2014). Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 38, 494–506. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P., et al. (2009). The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med. 6:e1000100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Littleton, S. W. (2012). Impact of obesity on respiratory function. Respirology 17, 43–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1843.2011.02096.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loeber, S., Grosshans, M., Korucuoglu, O., Vollmert, C., Vollstädt-Klein, S., Schneider, S., et al. (2012). Impairment of inhibitory control in response to food-associated cues and attentional bias of obese participants and normal-weight controls. Int. J. Obes. 36, 1334–1339. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luppino, F. S., de Wit, L. M., Bouvy, P. F., Stijnen, T., Cuijpers, P., Penninx, B. W., et al. (2010). Overweight, obesity, and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 67, 220–229. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maayan, L., Hoogendoorn, C., Sweat, V., and Convit, A. (2011). Disinhibited eating in obese adolescents is associated with orbitofrontal volume reductions and executive dysfunction. Obesity 19, 1382–1387. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrory, M. A., Fuss, P. J., Hays, N. P., Vinken, A. G., Greenberg, A. S., and Roberts, S. B. (1999). Overeating in America: association between restaurant food consumption and body fatness in healthy adult men and women ages 19 to 80. Obes. Res. 7, 564–571. doi: 10.1002/j.1550-8528.1999.tb00715.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPhail, S. M., Schippers, M., Marshall, A. L., Waite, M., and Kuipers, P. (2014). Perceived barriers and facilitators to increasing physical activity among people with musculoskeletal disorders: a qualitative investigation to inform intervention development. Clin. Interv. Aging 9, 2113–2122. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S72731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting: the role of the frontal lobes. Arch. Neurol. 9, 90–100. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1963.00460070100010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 151, 264–269. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mole, T. B., Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Collins, P., Mitchell, S. P., Bolton, S., et al. (2015). Impulsivity in disorders of food and drug misuse. Psychol. Med. 45, 771–782. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714001834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno-López, L., Soriano-Mas, C., Delgado-Rico, E., Rio-Valle, J. S., and Verdejo-García, A. (2012). Brain structural correlates of reward sensitivity and impulsivity in adolescents with normal and excess weight. PLoS ONE 7:e49185. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, M. C., Evans, D. A., Tangney, C. C., Bienias, J. L., and Wilson, R. S. (2005). Fish consumption and cognitive decline with age in a large community study. Arch. Neurol. 62:1849–1853. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.12.noc50161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navas, J. F., Vilar-López, R., Perales, J. C., Steward, T., Fernández-Aranda, F., and Verdejo-García, A. (2016). Altered decision-making under risk in obesity. PLoS ONE 11:e0155600. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0155600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nederkoorn, C., Braet, C., Van Eijs, Y., Tanghe, A., and Jansen, A. (2006). Why obese children cannot resist food: the role of impulsivity. Eat. Behav. 7, 315–322. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 126:220. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouwens, M. A., van Strien, T., and van der Staak, C. P. (2003). Tendency toward overeating and restraint as predictors of food consumption. Appetite 40, 291–298. doi: 10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00006-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, J., Ahmadi, S. F., Streja, E., Molnar, M. Z., Flegal, K. M., Gillen, D., et al. (2014). Obesity paradox in end-stage kidney disease patients. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 56, 415–425. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2013.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauli-Pott, U., Albayrak, O., Hebebrand, J., and Pott, W. (2010). Does inhibitory control capacity in overweight and obese children and adolescents predict success in a weight-reduction program? Eur. Child Adolescent Psychiatry 19, 135–141. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0049-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, A. L., Mackey, E., Cherry, J. B. C., Olson, A., You, X., Magge, S. N., et al. (2017). Effect of adolescent bariatric surgery on the brain and cognition: a pilot study. Obesity 25, 1852–1860. doi: 10.1002/oby.22013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearce, A. L., Mackey, E., Nadler, E. P., and Vaidya, C. J. (2018). Sleep health and psychopathology mediate executive deficits in pediatric obesity. Childhood Obesity 14, 189–196. doi: 10.1089/chi.2017.0281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, B. F., and Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 37, 51–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perpiñá, C., Segura, M., and Sánchez-Reales, S. (2017). Cognitive flexibility and decision-making in eating disorders and obesity. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia. Bulimia Obesity 22, 435–444. doi: 10.1007/s40519-016-0331-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, V. H. (2004). The influence of systemic inflammation on inflammation in the brain: implications for chronic neurodegenerative disease. Brain Behav. Immun. 18, 407–413. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2004.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pieper, J. R., and Laugero, K. D. (2013). Preschool children with lower executive function may be more vulnerable to emotional-based eating in the absence of hunger. Appetite 62, 103–109. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pignatti, R., Bertella, L., Albani, G., Mauro, A., Molinari, E., and Semenza, C. (2006). Decision-making in obesity: a study using the Gambling Task. Eat. Weight Disord. Stud. Anorexia Bulimia Obesity 11, 126–132. doi: 10.1007/BF03327557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prentice, A. M. (2001). Overeating: the health risks. Obesity Res. 9, 234S−238S. doi: 10.1038/oby.2001.124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qavam, S. E., Anisan, A., Fathi, M., and Pourabbasi, A. (2015). Study of relationship between obesity and executive functions among high school students in Bushehr, Iran. J. Diabetes Metab. Disord. 14:79. doi: 10.1186/s40200-015-0211-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raman, J., Hay, P., Tchanturia, K., and Smith, E. (2018). A randomised controlled trial of manualized cognitive remediation therapy in adult obesity. Appetite 123, 269–279. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restivo, M. R., McKinnon, M. C., Frey, B. N., Hall, G. B., Syed, W., and Taylor, V. H. (2017). The impact of obesity on neuropsychological functioning in adults with and without major depressive disorder. PLoS ONE 12:e0176898. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyes, S., Peirano, P., Peigneux, P., Lozoff, B., and Algarin, C. (2015). Inhibitory control in otherwise healthy overweight 10-year-old children. Int. J. Obes. 39, 1230–1235. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reynolds, C. R. (1997). Forward and backward memory span should not be combined for clinical analysis. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 12, 29–40. doi: 10.1093/arclin/12.1.29

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricca, V., Castellini, G., Lo Sauro, C., Ravaldi, C., Lapi, F., Mannucci, E., et al. (2009). Correlations between binge eating and emotional eating in a sample of overweight subjects. Appetite 53, 418–421. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, J. B., Zhang, L., Mitchell, S. H., and De Wit, H. (1999). Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: effect of alcohol. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 71, 121–143. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1999.71-121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, R. D., and Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. 124:207. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanderlin, A. H., Todem, D., and Bozoki, A. C. (2017). Obesity and co-morbid conditions are associated with specific neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:164. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiff, S., Amodio, P., Testa, G., Nardi, M., Montagnese, S., Caregaro, L., et al. (2016). Impulsivity toward food reward is related to BMI: evidence from intertemporal choice in obese and normal-weight individuals. Brain Cogn. 110, 112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2015.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sellaro, R., and Colzato, L. S. (2017). High body mass index is associated with impaired cognitive control. Appetite 113, 301–309. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sellbom, K. S., and Gunstad, J. (2012). Cognitive function and decline in obesity. J. Alzheimer Dis. 30, S89–S95. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skoranski, A. M., Most, S. B., Lutz-Stehl, M., Hoffman, J. E., Hassink, S. G., and Simons, R. F. (2013). Response monitoring and cognitive control in childhood obesity. Biol. Psychol. 92, 199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. G., and Robbins, T. W. (2013). The neurobiological underpinnings of obesity and binge eating: a rationale for adopting the food addiction model. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 804–810. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, P. J., and Blumenthal, J. A. (2016). Dietary factors and cognitive decline. J. Prevent. Alzheimer Dis. 3, 53–64. doi: 10.14283/jpad.2015.71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitoni, G. F., Ottaviani, C., Petta, A. M., Zingaretti, P., Aragona, M., Sarnicola, A., et al. (2017). Obesity is associated with lack of inhibitory control and impaired heart rate variability reactivity and recovery in response to food stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 116, 77–84. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitznagel, M. B., Alosco, M., Galioto, R., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Sysko, R., et al. (2014). The role of cognitive function in postoperative weight loss outcomes: 36-month follow-up. Obesity Surg. 24, 1078–1084. doi: 10.1007/s11695-014-1205-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitznagel, M. B., Garcia, S., Miller, L. A., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Wing, R., et al. (2013). Cognitive function predicts weight loss after bariatric surgery. Surg. Obesity Related Dis. 9, 453–459. doi: 10.1016/j.soard.2011.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanek, K. M., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Cohen, R., Paul, R., Crosby, R. D., et al. (2013). Body mass index and neurocognitive functioning across the adult lifespan. Neuropsychology 27, 141–151. doi: 10.1037/a0031988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steenbergen, L., and Colzato, L. S. (2017). Overweight and cognitive performance: high body mass index is associated with impairment in reactive control during task switching. Front. Nutri. 4:51. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2017.00051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stingl, K. T., Kullmann, S., Ketterer, C., Heni, M., Häring, H. U., Fritsche, A., et al. (2012). Neuronal correlates of reduced memory performance in overweight subjects. Neuroimage 60, 362–369. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stinson, E. J., Krakoff, J., and Gluck, M. E. (2018). Depressive symptoms and poorer performance on the Stroop Task are associated with weight gain. Physiol. Behav. 186, 25–30. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweat, V., Yates, K. F., Migliaccio, R., and Convit, A. (2017). Obese adolescents show reduced cognitive processing speed compared with healthy weight peers. Childhood Obesity 13, 190–196. doi: 10.1089/chi.2016.0255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiara, G., Cigliobianco, M., Muravsky, A., Paoli, R. A., Mansur, R., Hawa, R., et al. (2017). Evidence for neurocognitive improvement after bariatric surgery: a systematic review. Psychosomatics 58, 217–227. doi: 10.1016/j.psym.2017.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C. L., Chen, F. C., Pan, C. Y., and Tseng, Y. T. (2016). The neurocognitive performance of visuospatial attention in children with obesity. Front. Psychol. 7:1033. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vainik, U., Dagher, A., Dub,é, L., and Fellows, L. K. (2013). Neurobehavioural correlates of body mass index and eating behaviours in adults: a systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 279–299. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Oord, S., Braet, C., Cortese, S., and Claes, L. (2018). Testing the dual pathway model of ADHD in obesity: a pilot study. Eating Weight Disord. Stud. Anorexia Bulimia Obesity 23, 507–512. doi: 10.1007/s40519-017-0375-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vantieghem, S., Bautmans, I., De Guchtenaere, A., Tanghe, A., and Provyn, S. (2018). Improved cognitive functioning in obese adolescents after a 30-week inpatient weight loss program. Pediatr. Res. 1, 267–271. doi: 10.1038/s41390-018-0047-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varkevisser, R. D. M., van Stralen, M. M., Kroeze, W., Ket, J. C. F., and Steenhuis, I. H. M. (2019). Determinants of weight loss maintenance: a systematic review. Obesity Rev. 20, 171–211. doi: 10.1111/obr.12772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verbeken, S., Braet, C., Bosmans, G., and Goossens, L. (2014). Comparing decision making in average and overweight children and adolescents. Int. J. Obes. 38, 547–551. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verbeken, S., Braet, C., Goossens, L., and Van der Oord, S. (2013). Executive function training with game elements for obese children: a novel treatment to enhance self-regulatory abilities for weight-control. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 290–299. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verdejo-García, A., Pérez-Expósito, M., Schmidt-Río-Valle, J., Fernández-Serrano, M. J., Cruz, F., Pérez-García, M., et al. (2010). Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity 18, 1572–1578. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., and Baler, R. D. (2011). Reward, dopamine and the control of food intake: implications for obesity. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Irvine, M. A., Derbyshire, K., Worbe, Y., Lange, I., Abbott, S., et al. (2014). Measuring “waiting” impulsivity in substance addictions and binge eating disorder in a novel analogue of rodent serial reaction time task. Biol. Psychiatr. 75, 148–155. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, D. D., Altman, M., Boswell, R. G., Kelley, W. M., and Heatherton, T. F. (2013). Self-regulatory depletion enhances neural responses to rewards and impairs top-down control. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2262–2271. doi: 10.1177/0956797613492985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G. J., Volkow, N. D., Telang, F., Jayne, M., Ma, J., Rao, M., et al. (2004). Exposure to appetitive food stimuli markedly activates the human brain. Neuroimage 21, 1790–1797. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weller, R. E., Cook, E. W. III., Avsar, K. B., and Cox, J. E. (2008). Obese women show greater delay discounting than healthy-weight women. Appetite 51, 563–569. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witbracht, M. G., Laugero, K. D., Van Loan, M. D., Adams, S. H., and Keim, N. L. (2012). Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task is related to magnitude of weight loss and salivary cortisol in a diet-induced weight loss intervention in overweight women. Physiol. Behav. 106, 291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2000). Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic (No. 894). World Health Organization; 894: i–xii, 1–253.

Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2015). World Health Statistics 2015. Geneva, WHO, 2015, pp 101–111. Available online at: www.who.int/gho/publications/world_health_statistics/2015/en/

Google Scholar

Wu, N., Chen, Y., Yang, J., and Li, F. (2017). Childhood obesity and academic performance: the role of working memory. Front. Psychol. 8:611. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, X., Nussbaum, M. A., and Madigan, M. L. (2016). Executive function and measures of fall risk among people with obesity. Percept. Mot. Skills 122, 825–839. doi: 10.1177/0031512516646158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, C., Wang, X., Zhou, C., Xu, C., and Chang, Y. K. (2017). Exercise and dietary program-induced weight reduction is associated with cognitive function among obese adolescents: a longitudinal study. Peer J 5:e3286. doi: 10.7717/peerj.3286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., Deng, Z. Y., Huang, Q., Zhang, W. X., Qi, C. Z., and Huang, J. A. (2017). Prefrontal cortex-mediated executive function as assessed by Stroop task performance associates with weight loss among overweight and obese adolescents and young adults. Behav. Brain Res. 321, 240–248. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.12.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yadava, A., and Sharma, N. R. (2014). Cognitive functioning in relation to body mass index. J. Indian Acad. Appl. Psychol. 40, 203–210. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Shields, G. S., Guo, C., and Liu, Y. (2018). Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: a meta-analysis and review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 225–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yau, P. L., Kang, E. H., Javier, D. C., and Convit, A. (2014). Preliminary evidence of cognitive and brain abnormalities in uncomplicated adolescent obesity. Obesity 22, 1865–1871. doi: 10.1002/oby.20801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yumuk, V., Tsigos, C., Fried, M., Schindler, K., Busetto, L., and Micic, D. (2015). European guidelines for obesity management in adults. Obesity Facts 8, 402–424. doi: 10.1159/000442721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: executive functions, obesity, overweight, cross-sectional studies, longitudinal studies

Citation: Favieri F, Forte G and Casagrande M (2019) The Executive Functions in Overweight and Obesity: A Systematic Review of Neuropsychological Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Studies. Front. Psychol. 10:2126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02126

Received: 08 May 2019; Accepted: 02 September 2019;
Published: 20 September 2019.

Edited by:

Antonios Dakanalis, University of Milano Bicocca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Nicholas T. Bello, Rutgers University, The State University of New Jersey, United States
Uku Vainik, Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital, McGill University, Canada
Roger A. Dixon, University of Alberta, Canada

Copyright © 2019 Favieri, Forte and Casagrande. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Francesca Favieri, francesca.favieri@uniroma1.it; Maria Casagrande, maria.casagrande@uniroma1.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.