Advertisement

Abstract

Human crowds provide paradigmatic examples of collective behavior emerging through self-organization. Understanding their dynamics is crucial to help manage mass events and daily pedestrian transportation. Although recent findings emphasized that pedestrians’ interactions are fundamentally anticipatory in nature, whether and how individual anticipation functionally benefits the group is not well understood. Here, we show the link between individual anticipation and emergent pattern formation through our experiments of lane formation, where unidirectional lanes are spontaneously formed in bidirectional pedestrian flows. Manipulating the anticipatory abilities of some of the pedestrians by distracting them visually delayed the collective pattern formation. Moreover, both the distracted pedestrians and the nondistracted ones had difficulties avoiding collisions while navigating. These results imply that avoidance maneuvers are normally a cooperative process and that mutual anticipation between pedestrians facilitates efficient pattern formation. Our findings may influence various fields, including traffic management, decision-making research, and swarm dynamics.

INTRODUCTION

Human crowds engage in a rich variety of self-organizing behaviors, as do other animals, in phenomena such as birds flocking and fish schooling (1, 2). They often exhibit fascinating “global” pattern formations that spread throughout a group over a range of inter-individual interactions. Human crowd behavior and pedestrian flows have become crucial research fields to help manage mass events and pedestrian transportation safely and comfortably. Although some collective patterns of organization in human crowds such as stop-and-go waves and crowd turbulence may lead to serious disasters, such as deadly trampling accidents (3, 4), there are other phenomena that provide functional benefits to the group even if there is no conductor or external control. Lane formation, where unidirectional lanes are spontaneously formed in bidirectional pedestrian flows, is the most notable example of effective self-organization to reduce the risk of collisions between pedestrians and enhance the efficiency of traffic flow (57). To accomplish efficient crowd management and minimize risks, it is therefore important to understand individual interactions underlying collective human behavior.
Computational models often describe pedestrian dynamics with an interaction potential, or a “social force” inspired from the repulsive potential among physical particles (8, 9). Although these physics-inspired models have remarkably improved our understanding of pedestrian crowd dynamics, they do not completely explain the empirical observations. In particular, recent empirical and experimental findings have stressed that inter-individual interaction is essentially anticipatory in nature, rather than a distance-dependent physical force (7, 1016). Pedestrians’ motions are influenced by anticipated future positions rather than the current positions of their neighbors. This suggests that pedestrians in a crowd are not just passively repelled by other pedestrians but actively find a passage through a crowd by anticipating and negotiating with neighbors to avoid collisions in advance. Social force models thus have been extended to capture the anticipatory nature of pedestrian behavior with velocity-dependent interactions rather than distance-dependent ones; in addition, models that are more vision-based have been developed (1014).
The anticipatory nature in human interactions has been reported in a wide range of pedestrian situations from bottlenecks to hallways, regardless of whether the environment is indoor or outdoor, and seems to be fundamental for collective human behavior (11, 13, 15). Understanding the relationship between individual anticipatory behaviors and collective pattern formation, therefore, is an important issue. Anticipating neighbors’ motions may attenuate head-on collisions between individuals in advance, help to smooth a pedestrian’s pathway through a crowd, and facilitate an efficient transition to emergent pattern formation. However, despite unprecedented growth in experimental crowd research (17), the effects of anticipatory behaviors on self-organization in human crowds remain poorly understood. In particular, there has been no attempt to experimentally intervene in the cognitive ability of pedestrians’ anticipation.
In this study, we tested the hypothesis that collective pattern formation in human crowds is facilitated through anticipatory navigation behavior. We conducted an intervention experiment of lane formation where some participants were asked to walk while using mobile phones because the use of a mobile phone while walking was thought to interfere with their ability to anticipate neighbors’ motions. Previous experiments on gait characteristics of single pedestrians reported that mobile phone tasks made pedestrians’ perceptual visual fields narrow when they looked down and focused on the phone, and they distracted visual attention such as scanning frequency of the surrounding environment (e.g., oncoming traffic) (18, 19). Therefore, distracting pedestrians via a mobile phone task should make it more difficult for them to anticipate their neighbors’ motions because pedestrian motion is strongly influenced by other oncoming pedestrians even when they are well separated (11). In addition, it may also be difficult for pedestrians walking toward distracted pedestrians to anticipate the actions of distracted walkers. Thus, self-organization in human crowds facilitated by anticipation may be inferred when this type of distraction causes a deterioration not only of local collision risks but also of global pattern formation.
We demonstrated that the distraction slows overall walking speed and delays the onset of global pattern formation. Moreover, we observed that both the distracted pedestrians and the nondistracted ones had difficulties navigating, suggesting that coordinated avoidance maneuvers that successfully avoid collisions benefit from the anticipation of mutually interacting pedestrians. These results are consistent with the concept of self-organization in human crowds facilitated by mutual anticipation. Our findings provide insights into understanding the important role of individual cognitive abilities of anticipation in self-organizing systems.

RESULTS

Influence of visual distraction on the emergence of lane formation

We conducted experiments where two groups (27 pedestrians each) participated in bidirectional flows in a straight mock corridor (fig. S1, see Materials and Methods). We imposed an additional task on three participants in one of the two groups. They were visually distracted to potentially disrupt their anticipatory interactions with the others. As the distraction, we asked each of these three people to use a mobile phone while walking (Fig. 1A). We hypothesized that distracted pedestrians located in the front of the group, which would directly face the oncoming crowd, would have the most influence on overall crowd dynamics in this bidirectional flow experiment. To test this, we set three experimental conditions (front, middle, and rear) in addition to a baseline condition in which nobody was distracted. Under the front condition, three participants in either group were randomly selected to perform the additional task while walking and assigned to the frontmost three positions of their group. The mid- and rear conditions were the same as the front condition except that the distracted participants were assigned to the middle or rear positions, respectively. The experiments were replicated 12 times under each condition.
Fig. 1 Bidirectional flow experiments with distracted pedestrians.
(A) Snapshot from an experiment under the front condition (the distracted pedestrians located in the front of a group), where locations of the distracted pedestrians are marked by blue circles. (B) Representative examples of reconstructed pedestrian trajectories under (i) front, (ii) middle, (iii) rear, and (iv) baseline conditions. Yellow (red) lines represent pedestrians moving from left (right) to right (left). Photo credit: Hisashi Murakami, Kyoto Institute of Technology; The University of Tokyo.
Figure 1B shows the development over time of the traffic organization in bidirectional pedestrian flow under each condition. Pedestrians from the two groups initially walked in opposite directions along the corridor, deviating from the direct path to the destination as necessary to seek their passages through a crowd and avoid oncoming pedestrians. Subsequently, they self-organized into two or more unidirectional lanes and then they followed straight courses along the generated lanes. The distracted pedestrians under the front condition seemed to disturb overall behavior more than they did in the other conditions.
To account for the above observation quantitatively, we first calculated each pedestrian’s walking speed defined as traveled distance in the measurement area divided by crossing time, where the crossing time is the time that a pedestrian took from entering the measurement area to leaving it (Fig. 2A). Pedestrians under the front condition were significantly slower than those under the baseline condition (versus front: W = 259,770, P < 0.001, N = 1296; versus middle: W = 221,949, P = 0.06, N = 1296; versus rear: W = 218,982, P = 0.11, N = 1296), suggesting that distracted pedestrians under the front condition influenced overall flow as we expected.
Fig. 2 Walking speed and onset of lane formation.
(A) Pedestrians’ walking speeds under each condition. Each data point represents a pedestrian. (B) Onset of lane formation under each condition. Each data point represents a trial. Asterisks indicate the statistical significance (***P < 0.001; *P < 0.05; ns, P > 0.05). Box-and-whisker plots represent the median of the data (central thick line), data between the first and third quartiles (box), data within 1.5× the interquartile range of the median (whiskers), and outliers (unfilled circles). BL, baseline condition.
To investigate the self-organization process of lane formation, we calculated the order parameter, which is widely used for measuring the stratification degree of bidirectional flow (6, 7, 20). The order parameter was defined by discretizing the measurement area into several rows along the corridor and averaging the proportion of individuals moving in the same direction in each row (for details, see Materials and Methods). Its value takes 0 when the same number of pedestrians in a row move in different directions or when no individuals occupy a row, and it approaches 1 (which suggests clear lane formation) if most pedestrians in a row move in the same direction. Under the front condition in particular, we observed that lanes developed more slowly than under the other conditions (see fig. S2 for plots of the order parameter as a function of time). To verify this, we investigated the onset of lane formation by calculating the time at which the order parameter exceeded 0.8 for the first time after the beginning of a trial. This procedure was introduced and justified in our previous study (7) (also see Materials and Methods). We found that the onset of lane formation under the front condition was significantly later than under the baseline condition but not in the other conditions (versus front: W = 33.5, P = 0.04, N = 24; versus middle: W = 72, P = 0.76, N = 24; versus rear: W = 96.5, P = 0.92, N = 24; Fig. 2B). This suggests that only the distracted pedestrians under the front condition disturbed the facilitation of self-organization.
In summary, mobile phone distractions significantly influenced overall walking speeds and the onset of lane formation, especially when the distracted pedestrians were in the front of the group. The following question thus arises: How is individual behavior, such as local collision avoidance in particular, influenced by the distraction? We answer this question in the next section.

Local collision avoidance under the front condition

In this section, we focus on pedestrians’ behaviors until the onset of lane formation, where anticipatory collision avoidance behavior most likely happens (7), under the baseline condition and front condition. We assumed that the mobile phone distraction would increase the risk of local collision among pedestrians and that pedestrians would need to perform sudden large turns or steps to avoid collision. In other words, if pedestrians anticipated the motions of oncoming neighbors, then they could avoid each other in advance, and therefore they would not take sudden large steps. To verify this assumption, we analyzed pedestrians’ behaviors in more detail through a stratified analysis where we divided the front-condition data into three subgroups: (i) distracted, distracted pedestrians; (ii) same-directed, pedestrians walking in the same direction as the distracted ones; and (iii) opposite-directed, pedestrians walking in the opposite direction to the distracted ones.
We first investigated the temporal features of avoidance behaviors. If pedestrians performed collision avoidances locally and suddenly, then they would slow down to avoid a collision, make turns with a relatively large turn angle, and then increase their speed to catch up with the preceding pedestrians. As an indicator, we determined changes in an individual’s walking speed by calculating differences between two consecutive speed values at time t and at time tdt, where dt was the time period and dt = 1 (s). As another indicator, we determined angular deviations of the velocity vector from the desired direction (i.e., the direct path to their destination: the x axis along the corridor length in this case) by calculating the angle that the velocity vector made with the x axis at time t. These indicators in the baseline condition were distributed in a narrower range, with smaller changes in speed and deviation than in the others (see the two-dimensional frequency distribution of the indicators at a turn in fig. S3, middle row), suggesting that pedestrians under the baseline condition did not perform sudden turns thanks to mutual anticipation. If we consider the absolute value of the product of the above indicators as an index of a sudden turn, the index in the baseline condition was significantly smaller than in the others (Fig. 3A). Moreover, we compared the angular deviation at time t with changes in walking speed before a turn calculated from the velocities at time tdt and t − 2dt; we also compared those after a turn calculated from the velocities at time t + dt and t (see the distribution changes before and after a turn in fig. S3, top and bottom rows, respectively). In these comparisons, we observed that distracted pedestrians and opposite-directed ones tended to slow down before and speed up after large deviations. These results imply that they performed collision avoidances locally and suddenly.
Fig. 3 Suddenness of turns and turn intensity.
(A) Suddenness of turns, calculated as the absolute value of the product of angular deviation and change in speed. (B) Turn intensity, calculated as the product of step length and angle. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. When compared to the baseline, the values in the other groups are larger, suggesting that pedestrians under the baseline condition did not perform sudden large steps, but members of the other groups were forced to perform them.
The above analysis focused on temporal features of pedestrians’ behaviors; we also investigated various spatial features (Fig. 3B and fig. S4). We focused on the y components of movements (perpendicular to the corridor length; fig. S4A). Turning was defined as direction changes in the y component of an individual’s trajectory, and movements between consecutive turning points were regarded as “steps.” We defined “step length” as the y component of a step and “step angle” as the absolute value of angle deviation of a step from the desired direction (the x axis in this case). Considering the results of our analysis of the temporal features of pedestrian behavior, we assumed that step lengths and step angles would take large values at one point in time if pedestrians performed sudden large steps. Pedestrians under the front condition, regardless of group, demonstrated steps with large lengths and angles as compared to those in the baseline group (see fig. S4B). This suggests that pedestrians under the front condition took large steps to avoid collisions immediately before meeting oncoming neighbors. We then calculated the product of step length and angle as an index of turn intensity (Fig. 3B). As compared to the baseline values, the values in the other groups are larger, suggesting that pedestrians under the baseline condition did not perform sudden large steps along the y axis, but members of the other groups did (Fig. 3B). In summary, we observed that both distracted pedestrians and nondistracted pedestrians performed sudden large turns or steps to avoid imminent collisions, implying that they had difficulties navigating.

Additional experiments with slow-walking pedestrians

To verify the possibility that the decrease in walking speed induced by mobile phone distraction influenced the lane formation process rather than anticipatory ability, we conducted additional experiments. The experimental procedure was the same as the main experiment except for one instruction: The three selected pedestrians were asked to walk slowly instead of performing mobile phone tasks. As with the main experiment, we considered three conditions regarding where these three slow-walking pedestrians were located in the group: front, middle, and rear. The experiments were replicated three times under each condition.
We first observed that overall walking speed under the front condition was less than that of the baseline, which is similar to our main results (versus front: W = 72,233, P < 0.001, N = 810; versus middle: W = 56,255, P = 0.08, N = 810; versus rear: W = 57198, P = 0.06, N = 810; Fig. 4A). Overall walking speed under the front condition in this additional experiment was less than that in the main experiment (9.8 and 4.3% less than baseline in terms of median value, respectively). Nevertheless, instructing pedestrians to walk slowly did not affect the onset of lane formation (versus front: W = 6, P = 0.12, N = 15; versus middle: W = 26, P = 0.89, N = 15; versus rear: W = 7.5, P = 0.12, N = 15; Fig. 4B). Moreover, we did not observe the intense collision avoidance behavior that was observed in the main experiments (fig. S5). We therefore considered that slow walking per se did not influence the formation of lanes.
Fig. 4 Results of additional experiments with slow-walking pedestrians.
(A) Walking speed. (B) Onset of lane formation. Asterisks indicate the statistical significance (***P < 0.001; ns, P > 0.05) of differences between the baseline condition and each of the other conditions. See Fig. 2 for a description of the box-and-whisker plots.

Additional analysis on how pedestrians walk before lane formation

When pedestrians anticipate their neighbors’ motions to seek their paths in dynamic pedestrian flow, their path-seeking behavior involves them deviating from their desired directions, resulting in exploratory movements (7). It is possible that the distraction introduced in our experiments influenced these exploratory movements. We analyzed the diffusive properties along the y axis before lane formation using the mean-squared displacement (section S1). According to this analysis, the diffusive characteristics of distracted pedestrians’ movements varied by trial, even when including the normal diffusive properties that indicate almost straight paths along the corridor with only small lateral deviations (fig. S6). The nondistracted walkers and baseline group demonstrated super-diffusive characteristics of movement patterns in all trials (fig. S6), in accordance with the results of a previous study (7). Therefore, the distracted pedestrians sometimes did not perform their usual exploratory walking in the crowd.

DISCUSSION

In the present study, we addressed whether self-organization in human crowds can be promoted by anticipatory interaction among individuals. To this end, we conducted bidirectional flow experiments, where some participants were asked to walk while typing into mobile phones. Distracting pedestrians at the front of a group slowed down overall crowd movement and delayed onset of global lane formation. In addition, the increase of local collision avoidance demonstrated the indirect effects of the experimental manipulation on nondistracted pedestrians as well as the direct effects on distracted ones. The distraction thus locally reduced individual anticipation ability and globally disturbed the self-organization process through the propagation of reduced levels of anticipation in the crowd. These results are consistent with the idea that self-organization in human crowds is facilitated by mutual anticipation.
The mobile phone distraction altered behaviors of distracted pedestrians. We observed that they intensely performed local collision avoidance behavior compared to normal pedestrians under the baseline condition. This is consistent with the assumption that pedestrians who are able to anticipate neighbors’ motions can avoid collisions in advance. Resources of visual attention and/or other cognitive processing are limited (21); hence, pedestrians focusing on their phones would be unable to dedicate their visual resources to their walking task. According to a previous study, single walkers fixed their eyes approximately 75% of the time on the mobile phone while typing (18). Thus, it would not be unusual if distracted pedestrians performed emergency avoidance movements because they were unaware of oncoming people. Moreover, it is known that humans are notoriously bad at walking in a straight line in the absence of visual cues (22). In our experiments, distracted pedestrians rarely seemed to access the visual cues around them. Therefore, they might fail to execute collision avoidance behaviors in advance even if they were aware of oncoming people.
Somewhat unexpectedly, nondistracted pedestrians, especially those moving toward distracted ones, also intensely performed local collision avoidance behavior as compared to pedestrians under the baseline condition. This suggests that their anticipatory behavior toward oncoming pedestrians sometimes did not work well, even though they were able to dedicate their visual resources to their walking task. We speculate that this local collision avoidance of opposite-directed pedestrians was caused by their uncertainty of the motions of distracted walkers. The opposite-directed pedestrians most likely obtained less information from the gaze direction of the distracted ones, from which humans usually infer other people’s movement trajectories (23). Because distracted pedestrians looked down and focused their eyes on the phone, opposite-directed ones would be uncertain about the motions of distracted ones. Then, how do opposite-directed pedestrians deal with the uncertain motions?
In behavioral neuroscience studies, human decision-making has been considered in a serial “decision-then-action” framework where sensory evidence is collected and then action is taken. However, recent studies have stressed a “decision-during-action” framework because the decision-making may involve deadlines and time constraints for action (16, 24). In this framework, action plans can be prepared and even launched before information from the sensory cues is completely accumulated. We applied these ideas to the local collision avoidance behaviors we observed. In the baseline situation with no particular problems, pedestrians moving toward a destination should balance the benefits of making the correct decision (e.g., safely avoiding collisions) with its costs (e.g., the time constraint to reach the destination) and react accordingly. However, if uncertainty of motion is introduced in the form of oncoming distracted people, pedestrians will have to continue to seek rare information. Consequently, they will sometimes reach the situation where a collision is imminent and turn immediately before colliding with the distracted walkers. As an extended effect of the irregular behaviors of the opposite-directed pedestrians, same-directed pedestrians also might intensely perform local collision avoidance behavior.
In this way, we consider that the distraction in our experiment not only directly diminished the anticipatory ability of the distracted walkers but also indirectly influenced that of the nondistracted ones. This then induced collision avoidance behavior even though the nondistracted walkers were able to dedicate their visual resources during walking. In other words, collisions can be avoided in advance only if anticipation is performed mutually among pedestrians. Therefore, mutual anticipation would contribute more to a smooth walk than one-way anticipation. Together, the results show that avoidance maneuvers are normally a cooperative process, and self-organization (in this case, lane formation) is improved if pedestrians mutually anticipate their motions.
Understanding anticipatory interaction between pedestrians would provide fundamental insights into crowd dynamics and a crucial step toward effective and safe management of mass events. Our results describe how people navigate in close surroundings and the degree of effect that distracted people have on a crowd. Potential real-life applications in daily crowd management include efficient signage, customized navigation, and wayfinding. Distracted pedestrians can reduce the efficiency of pedestrian flows and, hence, the capacity of pedestrian facilities, considerably. As more and more people tend to stare at their phones while walking, it may be necessary to adjust planning and capacity manuals and design guidelines for pedestrian facilities. Using an eye-tracking system would be an effective measure to fully explore the effects of various degrees of anticipation on interactions among pedestrians according to the difficulty of the distraction task and the number of distracted pedestrians (i.e., their proportion to the total). Because human cognitive resources are limited and competitive (21), nonvisual distractions (i.e., something other than “looking at a phone”) may also influence the anticipatory ability of pedestrians. It would therefore be worthwhile to consider conducting different kinds of distraction experiments, for example, having pedestrians do mental math problems while walking or perform some other mental task while walking.
Moreover, we experimentally investigated the functional anticipatory interactions in human crowds, but it is important to explore representations in theoretical modeling. We expect that our results will provide quantitative support to existing models, especially vision-based or velocity-based ones (1012, 14), some of which are inspired from a cognitive science approach rather than physics-inspired repulsive interactions. In these models, pedestrians actively select the path with the longest distance to the predicted collision with the neighbors. For example, it would be reasonable to consider a simulation that represents the distracted pedestrians with distance-dependent interactions and nondistracted pedestrians with velocity-dependent ones. Yet, at the same time, the present findings may additionally require that pedestrians alter their behavior depending on the uncertainty of oncoming neighbors’ motions. Human crowd models that better incorporate anticipatory behavior will provide a more reliable description and prediction of pedestrian flows and therefore open the way for the improvement of architecture and exit routes, as well as management strategies during mass events. Furthermore, future studies focusing on anticipation in human crowds might bring innovative perspectives in other research fields concerned with self-organizing systems in general. For example, anticipatory behavior contributes to cooperative human decision-making through synchronization between two persons (13, 15); animal groups are considered to achieve robust collective behavior through mutual anticipation (2530); and swarm robotics has problems in navigation and collision avoidance in the real world and needs a previously unknown approach in industrial applications (31). Anticipation likely plays a key role in a wide range of self-organizing systems. Thus, we expect that the present findings will become a foothold for building future models of collective human behavior, biological collectives, and swarm robotics.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Experimental design

The experimental setup was adapted from that described in our previous studies (6, 7). Fifty-four male participants were recruited for the study from university students (mean age = 21.0 ± 2.3 years). We used a straight corridor composed of three main parts: a measurement area (10 × 3 m) centered in a corridor and two waiting areas (12 × 3 m), one on each side of the measurement area (fig. S1). Buffer zones (2 × 3 m) between the measurement and waiting areas allowed participants to stabilize their walking speed when entering the measurement area. Each waiting area was divided into sections along its length with nine lines perpendicular to the long axis of the corridor. The lines were separated by an equal distance of 1.5 m and were used as starting lines in the experiments. Destination areas were set at the end of each waiting area.
The experimental procedure was similar to that in our previous experiments (6, 7); however, we revised some details to intervene in the anticipatory interactions among pedestrians in crowds by making some of the participants distracted by means of mobile phone use. We first divided participants into two groups of 27 to have balanced bidirectional flows. Then, participants in each group were instructed to take random positions on randomly selected starting lines in each waiting area, with three participants per line. At the start signal, they were asked to walk in their usual way toward the exit at the opposite end of the mock corridor and to continue walking even after exiting the measurement area until entering the destination area on the opposite side to avoid the measurement area becoming jammed at the end of the experiment. Before starting the experiments, we informed all participants that there could be three people who would walk while using a mobile phone. We did this to decrease the risk of direct collision involving physical contact that could be induced by the distraction and to ensure that the same number of participants knew of the existence of distracted pedestrians during the experiment (i.e., to avoid a sort of progressive learning process in which participants gradually begin to understand details of the experimental procedure).
Three participants were randomly selected from one of the groups to use a mobile phone during the experiments to distract pedestrians visually and intervene in the anticipatory interactions among them. The three phone users were placed in only one of the two groups during a trial to decrease the risk of collision among distracted pedestrians, and we did not observe any hard physical contact in any of our experiments. Each distracted participant performed the mobile phone tasks for as many as two replicates. The three distracted participants were instructed on how to perform the mobile phone tasks immediately before each trial as described below.
In addition to observing the distracted pedestrians’ behavior, we attempted to observe the behavior of pedestrians who walked toward the distracted pedestrians. We hypothesized that distracted pedestrians located in the front of the group, which could directly collide with the oncoming crowd, would have the most influence on overall crowd dynamics in our bidirectional flow experiment. We therefore set three experimental conditions in addition to a baseline condition in which nobody was distracted: front, middle (mid), and rear. Under the front condition, three participants in either group were randomly selected to perform the additional task while walking and assigned to the frontmost three positions (i.e., behind the first starting line) of their respective group. The mid- and rear conditions were the same as the front condition except that the distracted participants were assigned to the middle (the fifth starting line) or rear (the ninth line) positions, respectively.
We conducted two pretest trials without the mobile phone distractions to confirm participants had correctly understood instructions given to them before conducting the main experiments. The main experiments were divided into two sessions of six blocks each. As mentioned previously, we divided participants into two groups that were maintained during a single session, but the group members were randomly shuffled between the two sessions. In each block, four trials with four different conditions (i.e., front, middle, rear, and baseline) were conducted in a randomized order. Moreover, the arrangement of participants at trial onset took into account the statistical effects of the experiment environments associated with walking direction (see also section S2). In total, experiments were replicated 12 times under each condition.
To control for the possibility that a decrease of walking speed induced by the mobile phone distraction (18) might influence the lane formation process rather than anticipatory ability, we conducted additional experiments that were similar to the above ones, but the three selected pedestrians were only asked to walk slowly instead of performing a task on a mobile phone. As with the main experiment, we considered the same three conditions regarding where these three slow-walking pedestrians were located in the group: front, middle, and rear. The additional experiments were replicated three times under each condition.

Mobile phone task

Distracted walking has become such a major cause of pedestrian accidents that many researchers have begun to investigate the influence of mobile phone distraction, especially on the behavior of a single pedestrian at a crossing. To introduce a mobile phone distraction in a crowd experiment, we selected a typing task, which has been shown to clearly distract pedestrians’ visual attention in terms of scanning frequency, fixation points, and fixation times toward traffic environments (18, 19). Inspired by previous studies, we used a simple arithmetic task with a single-digit addition problem similar to Kraepelin’s arithmetic test. A simple interface was constructed with Android Studio version 3.5 (https://developer.android.com), in which a single-digit addition problem, numeric keyboard, and OK button were presented on the display (fig. S7, A to C). After the user types in an answer and pushes the OK button, the next problem is presented regardless of whether the answer was correct or incorrect. To remove the influence of different mobile phones, the same phone model was used by all participants in the distracted role (UMIDIGI A3, 5.7-inch display, aspect ratio 19:9, 186 g in weight).
We asked participants to start solving problems from the moment they heard the start signal until they entered the destination area and to endeavor to solve as many problems as possible. The number of answers and the proportion of correct answers were recorded for each participant. After one rear-condition trial, however, one mobile phone had a technical problem, so the number of answers and proportion of correct answers were lost for that one sample (i.e., we obtained 35 samples for the rear condition and 36 samples for the other conditions). We then checked whether there were any differences in performance of calculations among the conditions. To control for the effect of differences in walking distance and speed among conditions on the number of answers, we estimated the number of answers per second by using the number of answers, distance between the start lines and the start of the opposite destination area (i.e., the approximate distance that pedestrians walked during the calculation period), and the median value of the walking speed (traveled distance in the measurement area divided by the crossing time) of the distracted pedestrians under each condition. We confirmed that there were no significant differences among the conditions (i.e., front, middle, and rear; see fig. S7, D and E) in number of answers per second (H = 2.83, P = 0.24, N = 107) or the proportion of correct answers (H = 1.34, P = 0.51, N = 107) under each condition.

Video tracking

In all experiments, we recorded the measurement area from above using a Sony FDR-AX60 camcorder (3840 × 2160 pixels, 30 frames per second) fixed at a height of 21 m. The participants wore black T-shirts and different colored caps (yellow or red) to make it easier to distinguish the different flows from one another. From the video images, we tracked the time series of the individuals’ positions frame-by-frame, using the image-processing software PeTrack (32).

Ethical note

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Tokyo, and informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Definition of the order parameter and onset of lane formation

To investigate the self-organizing process of lane formation, we calculated the order parameter, which was originally used for colloidal fluids, but is now used for measuring the stratification degree of various bidirectional flows. The order parameter was measured by discretizing the measurement area into 0.2-m × 0.2-m cells (50 columns × 15 rows), as proposed and justified in previous studies (6, 33). We then focused on the rows of aligned cells along the corridor. The local order parameter φj in the jth row was calculated as
φ j = [ ( n j L n j R ) / ( n j L + n j R ) ] 2 if at least one pedestrian occupies the row , 0   otherwise ()
(1)
Here, njL and njR are the numbers of left and right walkers in the jth row, respectively. The global order parameter Φ was then calculated as
Φ = ( 1 / N )   Σ N φ j ()
(2)
where N is the number of rows. Φ was smoothed over a time window of three frames. Its value becomes 0 when the pedestrians in a row move in different directions or when no individuals occupy the row and approaches 1 if most of the pedestrians in the row move in the same direction, suggesting clear lane formation.
By using the order parameter, we considered the onset of unidirectional lanes as the time at which Φ first exceeded 0.8, as verified in a previous study (7). We considered this value reasonable in the previous study (7) because (i) the approximate time appropriately indicates when the lane formation is first achieved in a bidirectional flow; (ii) the parameter value does not significantly affect the onset of lane formation as long as it lies in a certain interval; and (iii) Φ reached this value in all trials (this was also true in the present experiment), so the onset of lane formation was determinable in all trials.

Statistical analysis

One-tailed Mann-Whitney–Wilcoxon tests were used to test differences of walking speed and the onset of lane formation between the baseline condition and each of the other conditions (front, middle, and rear) in the main experiments with distracted pedestrians and the additional experiments with slow-walking pedestrians. When making multiple comparisons, P values were adjusted by using false discovery rate control (34). For the mobile phone task, Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to test differences of number of answers per second and the proportion of correct answers among the experimental conditions. All statistical analyses were conducted using R version 3.6.0 (The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria).

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the editorial handling of A. Clauset and acknowledge constructive comments from two anonymous reviewers. We are also grateful to all of the participants and members of the Nishinari laboratory for help during the experiments and to D. Yanagisawa, Y. Oyama, M. Furukawa, and M. Abe for helpful discussions. Funding: This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI grant numbers JP20K20143, JP20K14992, and JP19K20384; JST-Mirai Program grant numbers JPMJMI17D4 and JPMJMI20D1; and MEXT Leading Initiative for Excellent Young Researchers grant number JPMXS0320200282. Author contributions: H.M., C.F., and Y.N. designed the study; H.M., C.F., and Y.N. performed the experiments; H.M. analyzed the data; K.N. provided funding acquisition, project administration, and resources; and H.M. and Y.N. wrote the paper. Competing interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests. Data and materials availability: All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper and/or the Supplementary Materials. Additional data related to this paper may be requested from the authors.

Supplementary Material

File (abe7758_data_file_s1.zip)
File (abe7758_data_file_s2.zip)
File (abe7758_data_file_s3.zip)
File (abe7758_movie_s1.mp4)
File (abe7758_movie_s2.mp4)
File (abe7758_movie_s3.mp4)
File (abe7758_movie_s4.mp4)
File (abe7758_sm.pdf)

REFERENCES AND NOTES

1
D. Helbing, A. Johansson, Pedestrian, crowd, and evacuation dynamics, in Encyclopedia of Complexity and Systems Science, R. A. Meyers, Ed. (Springer, 2009), pp. 6476–6495.
2
I. D. Couzin, Collective minds. Nature 445, 715 (2007).
3
H. Alnabulsi, J. Drury, Social identification moderates the effect of crowd density on safety at the Hajj. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 9091–9096 (2014).
4
D. Helbing, A. Johansson, H. Z. Al-Abideen, Dynamics of crowd disasters: An empirical study. Phys. Rev. E 75, 046109 (2007).
5
M. Moussaïd, E. G. Guillot, M. Moreau, J. Fehrenbach, O. Chabiron, S. Lemercier, J. Pettré, C. Appert-Rolland, P. Degond, G. Theraulaz, Traffic instabilities in self-organized pedestrian crowds. PLOS Comput. Biol. 8, e1002442 (2012).
6
C. Feliciani, K. Nishinari, Empirical analysis of the lane formation process in bidirectional pedestrian flow. Phys. Rev. E 94, 032304 (2016).
7
H. Murakami, C. Feliciani, K. Nishinari, Lévy walk process in self-organization of pedestrian crowds. J. R. Soc. Interface 16, 20180939 (2019).
8
D. Helbing, I. Farkas, T. Vicsek, Simulating dynamical features of escape panic. Nature 407, 487–490 (2000).
9
M. Moussaïd, D. Helbing, S. Garnier, A. Johansson, M. Combe, G. Theraulaz, Experimental study of the behavioural mechanisms underlying self-organization in human crowds. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 2755–2762 (2009).
10
M. Moussaïd, D. Helbing, G. Theraulaz, How simple rules determine pedestrian behavior and crowd disasters. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 6884–6888 (2011).
11
I. Karamouzas, B. Skinner, S. J. Guy, Universal power law governing pedestrian interactions. Phys. Rev. Lett. 113, 238701 (2014).
12
A. Johansson, Constant-net-time headway as a key mechanism behind pedestrian flow dynamics. Phys. Rev. E 80, 026120 (2009).
13
L. Noy, E. Dekel, U. Alon, The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 20947–20952 (2011).
14
A. Johansson, D. Helbing, P. K. Shukla, Specification of the social force pedestrian model by evolutionary adjustment to video tracking data. Adv. Complex Syst. 10, 271–288 (2007).
15
M. Gérin-Lajoie, C. L. Richards, B. J. McFadyen, The negotiation of stationary and moving obstructions during walking: Anticipatory locomotor adaptations and preservation of personal space. Motor Control 9, 242–269 (2005).
16
T. Ducourant, S. Vieilledent, Y. Kerlirzin, A. Berthoz, Timing and distance characteristics of interpersonal coordination during locomotion. Neurosci. Lett. 389, 6–11 (2005).
17
M. Haghani, Empirical methods in pedestrian, crowd and evacuation dynamics: Part I. Experimental methods and emerging topics. Saf. Sci. 129, 104743 (2020).
18
K. Jiang, F. Ling, Z. Feng, C. Ma, W. Kumfer, C. Shao, K. Wang, Effects of mobile phone distraction on pedestrians’ crossing behavior and visual attention allocation at a signalized intersection: An outdoor experimental study. Accid. Anal. Prev. 115, 170–177 (2018).
19
N. Alejalil, S. R. Davoodi, Mobile phone usage and its effects on pedestrians’ distraction. Int. J. High. Risk. Behav. Addict. 6, e35431 (2017).
20
S. Nowak, A. Schadschneider, Quantitative analysis of pedestrian counterflow in a cellular automaton model. Phys. Rev. E 85, 066128 (2012).
21
S. L. Franconeri, G. A. Alvarez, P. Cavanagh, Flexible cognitive resources: Competitive content maps for attention and memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 134–141 (2013).
22
J. L. Souman, I. Frissen, M. N. Sreenivasa, M. O. Ernst, Walking straight into circles. Curr. Biol. 19, 1538–1542 (2009).
23
L. Nummenmaa, J. Hyönä, J. K. Hietanen, I’ll walk this way: Eyes reveal the direction of locomotion and make passersby look and go the other way. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1454–1458 (2009).
24
N. F. Lepora, G. Pezzulo, Embodied choice: How action influences perceptual decision making. PLoS Comput. Biol. 11, e1004110 (2015).
25
H. Murakami, T. Niizato, T. Tomaru, Y. Nishiyama, Y. P. Gunji, Inherent noise appears as a Lévy walk in fish schools. Sci. Rep. 5, 10605 (2015).
26
H. Murakami, T. Tomaru, Y. Nishiyama, T. Moriyama, T. Niizato, Y. P. Gunji, Emergent runaway into an avoidance area in a swarm of soldier crabs. PLOS ONE 9, e97870 (2014).
27
A. Morin, J. B. Caussin, C. Eloy, D. Bartolo, Collective motion with anticipation: Flocking, spinning, and swarming. Phys. Rev. E 91, 012134 (2015).
28
P. Gerlee, K. Tunstrøm, T. Lundh, B. Wennberg, Impact of anticipation in dynamical systems. Phys. Rev. E 96, 062413 (2017).
29
H. Murakami, T. Niizato, Y. P. Gunji, Emergence of a coherent and cohesive swarm based on mutual anticipation. Sci. Rep. 7, 46447 (2017).
30
Y.-P. Gunji, H. Murakami, T. Tomaru, V. Basios, Inverse Bayesian inference in swarming behaviour of soldier crabs. Philos. Trans. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 376, 20170370 (2018).
31
M. Schranz, M. Umlauft, M. Sende, W. Elmenreich, Swarm robotic behaviors and current applications. Front. Robot. AI 7, 36 (2020).
32
M. Boltes, A. Seyfried, Collecting pedestrian trajectories. Neurocomputing 100, 127–133 (2013).
33
C. Feliciani, K. Nishinari, Measurement of congestion and intrinsic risk in pedestrian crowds. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 91, 124–155 (2018).
34
Y. Benjamini, Y. Hochberg, Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300 (1995).
35
D. A. Raichlen, B. M. Wood, A. D. Gordon, A. Z. Mabulla, F. W. Marlowe, H. Pontzer, Evidence of Lévy walk foraging patterns in human hunter–gatherers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 728–733 (2014).
36
R. Jurado-Piña, J. M. P. Mayora, Methodology to predict driver vision impairment situations caused by sun glare. Trans. Res. Rec. 2120, 12–17 (2009).

(0)eLetters

eLetters is a forum for ongoing peer review. eLetters are not edited, proofread, or indexed, but they are screened. eLetters should provide substantive and scholarly commentary on the article. Embedded figures cannot be submitted, and we discourage the use of figures within eLetters in general. If a figure is essential, please include a link to the figure within the text of the eLetter. Please read our Terms of Service before submitting an eLetter.

Log In to Submit a Response

No eLetters have been published for this article yet.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Science Advances
Volume 7 | Issue 12
March 2021

Submission history

Received: 14 September 2020
Accepted: 1 February 2021

Permissions

See the Reprints and Permissions page for information about permissions for this article.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the editorial handling of A. Clauset and acknowledge constructive comments from two anonymous reviewers. We are also grateful to all of the participants and members of the Nishinari laboratory for help during the experiments and to D. Yanagisawa, Y. Oyama, M. Furukawa, and M. Abe for helpful discussions. Funding: This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI grant numbers JP20K20143, JP20K14992, and JP19K20384; JST-Mirai Program grant numbers JPMJMI17D4 and JPMJMI20D1; and MEXT Leading Initiative for Excellent Young Researchers grant number JPMXS0320200282. Author contributions: H.M., C.F., and Y.N. designed the study; H.M., C.F., and Y.N. performed the experiments; H.M. analyzed the data; K.N. provided funding acquisition, project administration, and resources; and H.M. and Y.N. wrote the paper. Competing interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests. Data and materials availability: All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper and/or the Supplementary Materials. Additional data related to this paper may be requested from the authors.

Authors

Affiliations

Research Center for Advanced Science and Technology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 153-8904, Japan.
Present address: Faculty of Information and Human Science, Kyoto Institute of Technology, Kyoto 606-8585, Japan.
Claudio Feliciani
Research Center for Advanced Science and Technology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 153-8904, Japan.
Information and Management Systems Engineering, Nagaoka University of Technology, Niigata 940-2188, Japan.
Research Center for Advanced Science and Technology, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 153-8904, Japan.
Department of Aeronautics and Astronautics, Graduate School of Engineering, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 113-8656, Japan.

Funding Information

Notes

*
Corresponding author: Email: [email protected]

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Article Usage

Altmetrics

Citations

Cite as

Export citation

Select the format you want to export the citation of this publication.

Cited by

  1. Persistent and responsive collective motion with adaptive time delay, Science Advances, 10, 14, (2024)./doi/10.1126/sciadv.adk3914
    Abstract
  2. Descriptive Parameters and Its Hysteresis of the Group Separation and Recombination in Bicycle Points Races: Leader’s Velocity and Speed Difference between Leader and Main Group, Applied Sciences, 13, 3, (1315), (2023).https://doi.org/10.3390/app13031315
    Crossref
  3. Lapses in the Person Radar: ADHD Symptoms Predict Difficulty in Interpersonal Distancing, Journal of Attention Disorders, 27, 4, (368-380), (2023).https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547221149200
    Crossref
  4. Physics of Human Crowds, Annual Review of Condensed Matter Physics, 14, 1, (311-333), (2023).https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-conmatphys-031620-100450
    Crossref
  5. Lane nucleation in complex active flows, Science, 379, 6635, (923-928), (2023)./doi/10.1126/science.add8091
    Abstract
  6. Macroscopic and microscopic dynamics of a pedestrian cross-flow: Part II, modelling, Safety Science, 158, (105969), (2023).https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2022.105969
    Crossref
  7. Macroscopic and microscopic dynamics of a pedestrian cross-flow: Part I, experimental analysis, Safety Science, 158, (105953), (2023).https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssci.2022.105953
    Crossref
  8. Preventing congestion in crowd dynamics caused by reversing flow, Mathematical Modeling and Computing, 9, 3, (555-566), (2022).https://doi.org/10.23939/mmc2022.03.555
    Crossref
  9. undefined, 2022 International Conference on Robotics and Automation (ICRA), (8368-8374), (2022).https://doi.org/10.1109/ICRA46639.2022.9811772
    Crossref
  10. Pedestrian counterflow in a floor field cellular automaton model coupled with an evolutionary game, Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment, 2022, 8, (083402), (2022).https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-5468/ac8152
    Crossref
  11. See more
Loading...

View Options

View options

PDF format

Download this article as a PDF file

Download PDF

Check Access

Log in to view the full text

AAAS ID LOGIN

AAAS login provides access to Science for AAAS Members, and access to other journals in the Science family to users who have purchased individual subscriptions.

Log in via OpenAthens.
Log in via Shibboleth.

More options

Register for free to read this article

As a service to the community, this article is available for free. Login or register for free to read this article.

Media

Figures

Multimedia

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share on social media